当前位置: X-MOL 学术Read. Res. Q. › 论文详情
Our official English website, www.x-mol.net, welcomes your feedback! (Note: you will need to create a separate account there.)
From the Editors
Reading Research Quarterly ( IF 3.9 ) Pub Date : 2021-01-19 , DOI: 10.1002/rrq.378
Amanda P. Goodwin , Robert T. Jiménez

image

image

Dear Colleagues,

We are pleased to present you with this issue of Reading Research Quarterly, which is full of new ways of thinking about literacy and assessments, with topics ranging from digital reading and multimodal composition to biliteracy assessments and effectiveness of reading interventions in international contexts. What sets this group of studies apart is how they build on the strong foundation of prior work to add key understandings. They also lean into the similarities and differences present across contexts, readers, texts, and tasks. Overall, this issue conveys advances in our understandings of literacy. We hope you find these pieces as exciting as we do.

We start with “Toward a Multifaceted Heuristic of Digital Reading to Inform Assessment, Research, Practice, and Policy.” In this invited commentary, Julie Coiro reviews the research to show what is known about digital literacy and to provide a heuristic “to systematically organize, label, and define a multifaceted set of increasingly complex terms, concepts, and practices that characterize the spectrum of digital reading experiences.” As she writes,

the multifaceted comprehension heuristic proposed in this commentary is designed to invite members of the literacy community to embrace and unpack the complexity of digital reading while also working to promote greater clarity around important dimensions of reading worthy of consideration in the future. Organizing dimensions of digital comprehension in [the RAND Reading Study Group’s] (2002) framework can help further expand our thinking by validating familiar understandings of text, activity, reader, and context as part of comprehension while also highlighting important differences within and across these elements as technology continues to transform our conceptions of reading in the years ahead.

We found this piece riveting and hope that it will continue to propel research forward in the area of digital reading.

Next, we present Blaine E. Smith, Mark B. Pacheco, and Mariia Khorosheva’s article entitled “Emergent Bilingual Students and Digital Multimodal Composition: A Systematic Review of Research in Secondary Classrooms.” Here, the authors analyze 70 studies that explored the different ways that technology and multiple modalities support emergent bilingual students’ academic and linguistic strengths. Results indicate five themes on how digital multimodal composing projects support identity expression, reshape language ideologies, leverage semiotic resources, expand linguistic repertoires, and support learning in general. As the Smith et al. write, research

has overwhelmingly presented the benefits for [emergent bilinguals], including impactful opportunities for agentively expressing identities, reshaping traditional classrooms, and expanding communicative repertoires. These findings underscore Kress’s (2003) assertion that multimodal literacies can offer “an inversion in semiotic power” (p. 9) that allows composers to express themselves in innovative and empowering ways not typically afforded with written texts.

The next study also considers new ways of working with emergent bilinguals, this time focusing on assessment. In “Beyond Monolingual Reading Assessments for Emerging Bilingual Learners: Expanding the Understanding of Biliteracy Assessment Through Writing,” Sandra A. Butvilofsky, Kathy Escamilla, Deena Gumina, and Elizabeth Silva Diaz explore ways to assess emergent bilinguals’ understandings in a more comprehensive and strengths‐based manner. As the authors write,

emerging bilingual learners’ biliteracy abilities are often underestimated when monolingual reading assessments, such as the DIBELS, are used to identify students as having difficulties in learning or to guide literacy instruction. [We] propose a holistic form of biliterate assessment that uses writing as a means to understand what emerging bilingual learners actually know about literacy.

Results suggested that analysis of student writing across Spanish and English provided deeper understandings of literacy understandings across languages. Butvilofsky et al. note,

Through our examination of students’ biliterate writing, we were able to uncover the vast amount of knowledge they had related to literacy, skills that were not apparent through either DIBELS or [its Spanish equivalent,] IDEL. In fact, some of the skills most often “remediated,” such as phonemic awareness, or ability to decode words and connect words and ideas in meaningful ways, were readily apparent in students’ Spanish and English writing samples. In fact, these students’ writing samples illustrate well the theory of holistic bilingualism in that what students knew in one language (e.g., the alphabetic principle, organization of text, the fact that punctuation marks are needed for meaning making) were frequently applied across languages in one direction or in bidirectional ways.

Studies like these highlight the possibilities of using strengths‐based assessments to better guide instruction.

The next study, “Young Minority Home‐Language Students’ Biased Reading Self‐Concept and Its Consequences for Reading Development” by Robin Segerer, Frank Niklas, Sebastian Suggate, and Wolfgang Schneider, also considers multilingual students’ learning, in this case focusing on the relation between reading and self‐concept in Germany. Results from students with German, Turkish, or another home language suggest different relations of these constructs. Specifically,

students speaking a minority home language exhibited a higher reading self‐concept. Cross‐lagged paths revealed reciprocal effects between reading achievement and reading self‐concept from grade 1 to grade 2, particularly for students with German as a home language. Minority home‐language students showed significantly lower effects of reading achievement on their subsequent reading self‐concept from grade 1 to grade 2. From grade 2 onward, reading achievement predicted reading self‐concept, but not vice versa.

In explaining the implications of the findings, the authors argue that “teachers might capitalize on minority home‐language students’ favorable initial [reading self‐concept] and thereby support these students’ subsequent reading development.”

Next, in “Toward a Professional Development Model for Writing as a Digital, Participatory Process,” Emily Howell, Sara Perez, and W. Todd Abraham take a new look at how to support teachers in “mak[ing] writing a more digital, participatory process.” This formative experiment involved developing professional development for upper elementary teachers to help them

engage students in writing as both a digital process and a social process. The essential elements studied in this model include the following: (a) sustained professional development, including the gradual release of teacher inquiry; (b) writing as a social practice; and (c) the use of digital tools to engage students in writing.

Results suggested successes such as more integration of digital tools and confidence in teaching writing, but challenges persisted in that tools were applied with conventional writing forms rather than “explor[ing] how the tools could allow students to go beyond those forms to participate in the shared practice and culture.” This work shows the complexities of supports needed by teachers to engage students in new forms of writing.

The next article advances understandings of spelling. In “The Effect of Orthographic Neighbors on Second‐Grade Students’ Spelling Acquisition,” Yi‐Jui Iva Chen, Anne E. Cunningham, Sophia Rabe‐Hesketh, Stephen P. Hinshaw, and Robin C. Irey show potential mechanisms of support for second graders’ spelling. Specifically, findings suggest that the similar spellings of words relate to students’ spelling acquisition with supports for analogizing rime neighbors (e.g., rain/vain) but neither substitution neighbors (e.g., rain/ruin) nor transposition neighbors (e.g., clam/calm). This also combined with students’ orthographic processing ability. Overall, the authors note the likely importance of

supporting students’ orthographic processing skills at early stages of literacy education....A statistically significant interaction between orthographic processing and rime neighbors suggested that the effect of rime neighbors on spelling acquisition was moderated by learners’ orthographic processing ability. Thus, spelling instruction should perhaps be tailored to students’ levels of orthographic processing ability. For students with less developed orthographic processing skills, grouping words based on rime neighbors might help facilitate their spelling acquisition. Nevertheless, the benefit of rime neighbors is less evident for students with well‐developed orthographic processing skills.

Again, this highlights a new way of viewing spelling that is relevant to theory, research, and practice.

The next study adds to understandings about how “deficits in phonological information processing in upper elementary students with specific learning disabilities in reading or spelling may increase, decrease, or remain stable over time.” Authors Claudia Schmidt, Janin Brandenburg, Jenny Busch, Gerhard Büttner, Dietmar Grube, Claudia Mähler, and Marcus Hasselhorn report in their study, entitled “Developmental Trajectories of Phonological Information Processing in Upper Elementary Students With Reading or Spelling Disabilities,” that deficits in phonological awareness remained across grades 3–5, whereas “rapid automatized naming was best described as a decreasing deficit.” These findings were consistent whether the students experienced reading or spelling challenges. The authors conclude,

there is no support for the overall validity of a single developmental pattern in German students with [learning disabilities] regarding the development of phonological processing. Depending on the aspect of phonological information processing that is considered, we found evidence in favor of persistent deficits (with respect to phonological awareness), increasing differences (the phonological loop), and decreasing deficits (alphanumeric [rapid automatized naming]).

The final study adds new thinking about advancing literacy in more fragile international contexts. In “Improving Reading Instruction and Students’ Reading Skills in the Early Grades: Evidence From a Randomized Evaluation in Haiti,” Juan Carlos Guzmán, Kate Schuenke‐Lucien, Anthony J. D’Agostino, Mark Berends, and Andrew J. Elliot explore the effect of an early‐grade literacy intervention entitled Read to Learn. This intervention included professional development and coaching, provided supports for implementation, and incorporated materials in students’ first language, resulting in positive effects. As the authors note,

the results of this study are an important contribution to knowledge about what works to improve literacy outcomes for students in Haiti and other fragile contexts. Despite many difficulties, the results demonstrate considerable gains in most literacy skill areas.…A program such as Read to Learn might be a solution to the learning crisis in additional poor and fragile contexts.

We would like to thank our authors, reviewers, and staff for their support. Each of these pieces make a strong contribution to the field, and we look forward to watching how they impact the field. Please remember to follow us on Facebook (@ILARRQ) and on Twitter (@DrGoodWin4Lit). Also remember to check out our interviews with authors online as YouTube videos (https://www.youtube.com/playlist?list=PLPEkvzXdK-8qnih4Nx5OJ5XTexZsldc5T) or as videos or podcasts (https://ila.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/page/journal/19362722/homepage/rrqauthorinterviews). Finally, don’t forget to check out our first special issue on the science of reading (https://ila.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/19362722/2020/55/S1), and stay tuned for the release of our second special issue on the science of reading as well.



中文翻译:

来自编辑

图片

图片

亲爱的同事们,

我们很高兴向您介绍本期《阅读研究季刊》,该充满了关于素养和评估的新思维方式,主题涉及从数字阅读和多模式写作到双语能力评估以及国际背景下阅读干预的有效性。这组研究与众不同的是,它们是如何在先前工作的坚实基础上建立基础以增加关键理解的。他们还倾向于在上下文,读者,文本和任务之间存在相似性和差异性。总体而言,该问题传达了我们对读写能力的理解的进步。我们希望您能像我们一样感到激动。

我们从“迈向数字阅读的多层面启发法,以告知评估,研究,实践和政策”开始。在这份受邀的评论中,朱莉·科伊罗(Julie Coiro)回顾了这项研究,以表明人们对数字素养的了解,并提供启发式的“系统地组织,标记和定义多方面的,越来越复杂的术语,概念和实践,这些特征,概念和做法可表征数字频谱阅读经验。” 如她所写,

本评论中提出的多角度理解启发式方法旨在邀请识字社区成员拥抱和解开数字阅读的复杂性,同时还致力于在将来值得考虑的重要阅读方面提高清晰度。在[RAND阅读研究小组(2002)]框架中组织数字理解的维度,可以通过验证对文本,活动,读者和上下文的熟悉理解作为理解的一部分,从而帮助进一步扩展我们的思维,同时强调这些元素内部和之间的重要差异。随着技术的发展,未来几年我们的阅读观念将不断发生变化。

我们发现这篇文章很吸引人,希望它能继续推动数字阅读领域的研究。

接下来,我们介绍布莱恩·史密斯(Blaine E. Smith),马克·帕切科(Mark B. Pacheco)和玛丽亚·霍罗谢娃(Maria Khorosheva)的文章,标题为“新兴的双语学生和数字多模式合成:中学课堂研究的系统综述”。在这里,作者分析了70项研究,这些研究探讨了技术和多种方式支持新兴双语学生的学术和语言能力的不同方式。结果表明了五个主题,涉及数字多模式组合项目如何支持身份表达,重塑语言意识形态,利用符号资源,扩展语言功能以及总体上支持学习。如史密斯等。写,研究

绝大多数地为[新兴双语者]带来了好处,包括有意义地表达身份,重塑传统教室和扩大交流方式的有效机会。这些发现强调了克雷斯(2003)的断言,即多峰文学可以提供“符号学力量的颠倒”(第9页),使作曲家能够以创新和授权的方式表达自己,而这通常是书面文本所无法提供的。

下一项研究还考虑了处理新兴双语者的新方法,这次重点是评估。Sandra A. Butvilofsky,Kathy Escamilla,Deena Gumina和Elizabeth Silva Diaz在“新兴语言学习者的单语阅读评估之外:通过写作扩展对双语评估的理解”中,探索了一种以更全面,更全面的方式评估新兴双语者理解的方法基于方式。正如作者所写,

当使用单语阅读评估(例如DIBELS)来识别学生学习困难或指导识字教学时,新兴的双语学习者的双语能力通常会被低估。[我们]提出了一种整体形式的双语评估,该评估以写作为手段来理解新兴的双语学习者对识字的实际了解。

结果表明,对跨西班牙语和英语的学生写作的分析提供了对跨语言识字理解的更深刻理解。Butvilofsky等。注意,

通过检查学生的两文写作,我们发现了与识字有关的大量知识,这些知识是通过DIBELS或[相当于西班牙文的语言] IDEL所不具备的。实际上,在学生的西班牙语和英语写作样本中,很容易理解一些最常“补救”的技能,例如音素意识,或以有意义的方式对单词进行解码以及将单词和思想联系起来的能力。实际上,这些学生的写作样本很好地说明了整体双语理论,因为学生在一种语言中所知道的内容(例如,字母原理,文本组织,需要标点符号的事实这一事实)经常被跨语言应用。以一个方向或双向方式。

此类研究突显了使用基于优势的评估来更好地指导教学的可能性。

Robin Segerer,Frank Niklas,Sebastian Suggate和Wolfgang Schneider所做的下一个研究“年轻的少数民族家庭学生的偏向阅读自我概念及其对阅读发展的后果”也考虑了多语言学生的学习,在这种情况下,重点是阅读与自我概念之间的关系。使用德语,土耳其语或其他母语的学生得出的结果表明,这些结构的关系不同。特别,

母语为母语的学生表现出较高的阅读自我概念。交叉滞后的路径揭示了从1年级到2年级的阅读成绩和阅读自我概念之间的相互影响,特别是对于德语为母语的学生。少数族裔学生从1年级到2年级,其阅读成就对其后续阅读自我概念的影响要低得多。从2年级开始,阅读成就可以预测阅读自我概念,反之则不然。

在解释这些发现的含义时,作者认为“教师可以利用少数族裔家庭语言学生的良好初始[阅读自我概念],从而支持这些学生随后的阅读发展。”

接下来,在“迈向以数字形式,参与性过程进行写作的职业发展模式”中,艾米莉·豪威尔,萨拉·佩雷斯和W.托德·亚伯拉罕重新审视了如何支持教师“以书写方式,使数字化,参与过程。” 此形成性实验涉及为高级基础教师发展专业发展以帮助他们

让学生写作既是数字过程又是社会过程。在该模型中研究的基本要素包括:(a)持续的专业发展,包括逐步释放教师询问;(b)写作是一种社会实践;(c)使用数字工具吸引学生写作。

结果显示出成功的成果,例如更多地整合了数字工具和对写作教学的信心,但是挑战仍然存在,因为这些工具以传统的写作形式应用,而不是“探讨工具如何使学生超越那些形式来参与写作。共同的实践和文化。” 这项工作表明,教师要使学生参与新的写作形式所需要的支持的复杂性。

下一篇文章将提高对拼写的理解。在“正交邻域对二年级学生拼写学习的影响”中,Yi-Jui Iva Chen,Anne E. Cunningham,Sophia Rabe-Hesketh,Stephen P. Hinshaw和Robin C. Irey展示了支持二年级学生的潜在机制年级学生的拼写。具体而言,研究结果表明,相似的单词拼写与学生的拼写习得有关,并具有支持雾邻居(例如,/徒劳)的支持,但不包括替代邻居(例如,/废墟)或换位邻居(例如,/镇定))。这还结合了学生的正字处理能力。总体而言,作者指出了

在识字教育的早期阶段支持学生的拼字处理技能....拼字处理与子邻居之间的统计学显着相互作用表明,学习者的拼字处理能力可以缓解子邻居对拼写习得的影响。因此,也许应该根据学生的拼字处理能力来定制拼写指导。对于拼写处理能力较差的学生,基于雾rim邻居对单词进行分组可能有助于他们的拼写学习。但是,对于拥有良好拼字处理技能的学生来说,雾rim邻居的好处并不明显。

同样,这突出显示了一种与理论,研究和实践相关的查看拼写的新方法。

接下来的研究增加了对“随着时间的推移,阅读或拼写有特殊学习障碍的高等基础学生的语音信息处理缺陷可能会增加,减少或保持稳定”的理解。作者Claudia Schmidt,Janin Brandenburg,Jen​​ny Busch,GerhardBüttner,Dietmar Grube,ClaudiaMähler和Marcus Hasselhorn在他们的研究中作了报告,题为“阅读或拼写障碍的高等基础学生的语音信息处理发展轨迹”,这在语音学上存在缺陷3-5年级之间的意识仍然保持不变,而“快速自动命名最能形容为赤字减少。” 无论学生遇到阅读或拼写挑战,这些发现都是一致的。作者得出结论,

对于有学习障碍的德国留学生,在语音处理发展方面,没有单一发展模式的整体有效性的支持。根据所考虑的语音信息处理方面,我们发现了证据,这些证据支持持续的缺陷(关于语音意识),差异增加(语音回路)和缺陷减少(字母数字[快速自动命名])。

最终研究为在更加脆弱的国际环境中提高识字率增加了新思路。在“改善早期的阅读教学和学生的阅读技巧:来自海地的随机评估的证据”中,Juan CarlosGuzmán,Kate Schuenke-Lucien,Anthony J. D'Agostino,Mark Berends和Andrew J. Elliot探索了名为“阅读学习”的早期扫盲干预措施的效果。这项干预措施包括专业发展和指导,为实施提供支持,并以学生的母语整合了教材,从而产生了积极的影响。如作者所述,

这项研究的结果对了解如何提高海地和其他脆弱环境的学生的识字效果的知识做出了重要贡献。尽管有许多困难,但结果表明在大多数识字技能领域都取得了相当大的进步。……诸如“阅读学习”之类的程序可能是在其他贫困和脆弱环境下解决学习危机的解决方案。

我们要感谢我们的作者,审稿人和工作人员的支持。所有这些作品都为该领域做出了巨大贡献,我们期待观察它们如何影响该领域。请记住在Facebook(@ILARRQ)和Twitter(@ DrGoodWin4Lit)上关注我们。另外请记住,以YouTube视频(https://www.youtube.com/playlist?list=PLPEkvzXdK-8qnih4Nx5OJ5XTexZsldc5T)或视频或播客(https://ila.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/)在线查看我们对作者的采访page / journal / 19362722 / homepage / rrqauthorinterviews)。最后,不要忘了阅读我们关于阅读科学的第一期特刊(https://ila.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/toc/19362722/2020/55/S1),敬请期待我们的发布第二本也是关于阅读科学的特刊。

更新日期:2021-01-20
down
wechat
bug