Testosterone replacement causes dose-dependent improvements in spatial memory among aged male rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104550Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Aged rats had impaired spatial working memory relative to young rats.

  • Aging and castration impaired performance on an object location memory task.

  • High and low, but not intermediate, doses of testosterone improved working memory.

  • An intermediate dose of testosterone improved long-term spatial memory.

  • Testosterone manipulations had no effect on BDNF levels in the brains of aged rats.

Abstract

Testosterone has been shown to have dose-dependent effects on spatial memory in males, but the effects of aging upon this relationship remain unclear. Additionally, the mechanism by which testosterone regulates memory is unknown, but may involve changes in brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) within specific brain regions. We tested the effects of age and testosterone on spatial memory among male rats using two spatial memory tasks: an object-location memory task (OLMT) and the radial-arm maze (RAM). Castration had minimal effect on performance on the RAM, but young rats (2 months) performed significantly fewer working memory errors than aged rats (20 months), and aged rats performed significantly fewer reference memory errors. Both age and castration impaired performance on the OLMT, with only the young rats with intact gonads successfully performing the task. Subsequent experiments involved daily injections of either drug vehicle or one of four doses of testosterone propionate (0.125, 0.250, 0.500, and 1.00 mg/rat) given to castrated aged males. On the RAM, a low physiological dose (0.125 mg) and high doses (0.500–1.000 mg) of testosterone improved working memory, while an intermediate dose (0.250 mg) did not. On the OLMT, only the 0.250 mg T group showed a significant increase in exploration ratios from the exposure trials to the testing trials, indicating that this group remembered the position of the objects. Brain tissue (prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and striatum) was collected from all subjects to assay BDNF. We found no evidence that testosterone influenced BDNF, indicating that it is unlikely that testosterone regulates spatial memory through changes in BDNF levels.

Introduction

Reduced endogenous testosterone may be an important contributor to age-related memory loss in men (Handelsman et al., 2015). Testosterone decline begins as early as age 30, and hypogonadism occurs in 20 % of men 60−69 year olds and in 50 % of men over 80 (Feldman et al., 2002). Numerous studies have shown a positive correlation between testosterone levels and cognitive ability in older men, including better spatial memory, suggesting that a reduction in testosterone levels may be partially responsible for age-related memory deficits in men (Barrett-Connor et al., 1999; Yaffe et al., 2002; Moffat et al., 2002). Additionally, testosterone reduction through age-related hypogonadism or artificial androgen deprivation has been identified as a risk factor for dementia (Lv et al., 2016; Nead et al., 2017). Testosterone supplementation improved spatial memory among patients with Alzheimer’s disease or mild cognitive impairment (Cherrier et al., 2005; Tan and Pu, 2003), but the benefits of androgen replacement therapies have been inconsistent (Kenny et al., 2004; Lu et al., 2006). Studies with healthy older men have also produced mixed results regarding the effects of androgen supplementation on cognitive ability (Cherrier et al., 2001; Vaughan et al., 2007; Wolf et al., 2000). These discrepancies may be due to differences among studies in the duration of hormone exposure, doses of testosterone used, and the specific aspects of spatial memory tested. Controlled experiments with animal models provide a useful way to disentangle these variables.

Spatial memory is one cognitive process that seems to be particularly influenced by both circulating testosterone and aging (Holland et al., 2011). Involving aspects of procedural, declarative, as well as both short-term and long-term memory, spatial memory is the process of recognizing, encoding, storing, and recalling spatial information about surroundings, positions of objects, or specific routes (Moscovitch et al., 2006). Testosterone seems to differentially influence the working and reference components of spatial memory. Working memory is a form of short-term memory that involves storage of relevant information while completing a specific task, whereas reference memory involves long-term storage of information from one task to be used for the next task (Cowan, 2008). Using a rat model, we recently demonstrated that testosterone enhances spatial working memory but has no effect on spatial reference memory (Wagner et al., 2018), which corroborates previous findings (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003; Gibbs and Johnson, 2008; Spritzer et al., 2011, 2008). However, experiments using the object location memory task (OLMT) suggest that testosterone influences some forms of long-term memory. Specifically, testosterone replacement was shown to improve spatial memory among castrated young adult male rats on the OLMT following both a 30 min retention period (McConnell et al., 2012) and a 2 h retention period (Jacome et al., 2016; Wagner et al., 2018). Thus, the length of the retention period may be a critical variable influencing the memory-enhancing effects of testosterone.

Considerable evidence from studies with humans and rodents suggests that the relationship between circulating testosterone levels and spatial memory is non-linear (Muller et al., 2005; Nowak et al., 2014). Testing male rats on the Morris Water Maze (MWM),we previously demonstrated that certain doses of testosterone (0.250 and 1.00 mg/rat) impaired reversal learning (Spritzer et al., 2011). Using other memory tasks (RAM and OLMT), we demonstrated that high and low physiological doses of testosterone (0.125 and 0.500 mg/rat) improved spatial memory, whereas an intermediate dose (0.250 mg/rat) did not (Wagner et al., 2018). Another study with rats suggested a curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial reference memory, with a high physiological dose (0.750 mg/rat) of testosterone optimal for performance (Jia et al., 2013). Administering supra-physiological doses of testosterone has either no effect on memory (Gibbs, 2005) or impairs spatial memory (Emamian et al., 2010). Additionally, human studies suggest that the optimal level of testosterone for spatial memory varies with age (Holland et al., 2011), but this hypothesis has yet to be tested experimentally.

The physiological mechanism by which testosterone influences spatial memory remains unknown. The hippocampus, striatum, and prefrontal cortex have all been implicated in spatial memory (Compton et al., 1997; Liljeholm and O’Doherty, 2012), making them potential direct targets for testosterone. The hippocampus is essential for place learning (i.e., the processing of information about the positions of environmental cues relative to a goal and one’s own position), whereas the striatum is essential for response learning (i.e., the use of stimulus-response relationships to locate a goal). Initially, both the hippocampus and striatum are engaged in parallel to solve a spatial task, and then the striatum becomes more important as the task becomes habitual (Chang and Gold, 2003; Jacobson et al., 2012; Packard and McGaugh, 1996). The pre-frontal cortex seems to be more involved in response learning, and is specifically engaged during spatial working memory tasks to plan the next choice (de Bruin et al., 1997; Floresco et al., 1997). Evidence suggests that the pre-frontal cortex is not essential for performance on the OLMT (Chao et al., 2016) but is employed in parallel with other brain regions for solving RAM tasks (Floresco et al., 1997; Kolb et al., 1994). Due to its critical role in neural plasticity and spatial cognition, brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) could play a key role in the connection between testosterone and cognitive processing in select brain regions (Egan et al., 2003; Tyler et al., 2002). Some evidence indicates that male rats have higher hippocampal levels of BDNF than do females (Franklin and Perrot-Sinal, 2006). Although BDNF levels have been found to decrease with age in male rats, this decline does not seem to correlate with age-related changes in testosterone (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2008). Consequently, the relationship between testosterone and BDNF remains unclear and, therefore, merits further investigation.

Few past studies have tested the effects of testosterone on spatial memory in aged rodents (Bimonte-Nelson et al., 2003; Goudsmit et al., 1990), and none have used a broad dose range. Building upon our past research with young rats (Wagner et al., 2018), the present study tested the effects of both age and testosterone dose on spatial memory. Initial experiments tested the effects of castration and age by comparing the performance of 20-month-old and 2-month-old male rats on the RAM and OLMT. Subsequent experiments used the same tasks to test the cognitive effects of testosterone replacement on aged males. We also quantified BDNF levels in the hippocampus, striatum, and prefrontal cortex to assess a possible mechanistic link between testosterone and BDNF.

Section snippets

Subjects

Young (2 months) and aged (20–22 months) Fischer 344 rats were obtained from the Aged Rodent Colony within the National Institute on Aging (Bethesda, MD, USA). Rats were either pair housed or triple housed in opaque polypropylene cages (21 × 42 × 21 cm) filled with Tek-Fresh bedding (Harlan Laboratories, Indianapolis, IN, USA). Animals had free access to tap water from glass bottles and were fed a soy-protein-free rodent diet (Harlan Teklad Diet 2020X). Housing rooms and testing rooms were

Experiment 1A: age and castration influenced RAM performance

Castration eliminated most of the circulating testosterone in rats used for Experiment 1A (Table 2). Specifically, seven rats from the Old/GDX group had serum testosterone concentrations below the detection limit of the assay (5 ng/ml), and the other five rats from the group had relatively low testosterone concentrations (0.06-0.16 ng/ml). Similarly, for the Young/GDX group, only three of the ten rats had serum testosterone concentrations above the detection limit (0.06-0.11 ng/ml). As

Discussion

Clinical studies have produced mixed results regarding the potential benefits of testosterone for the prevention and treatment of age-related memory loss and dementia (Fuller et al., 2007; Holland et al., 2011). Our results showed that testosterone replacement can improve spatial memory in an aged rat model, but the effects are dose-dependent and vary with the assessed form of memory (i.e., working memory or long-term memory). Old rats (20 months old) showed significant impairments in spatial

Contributors

E.C.B. Jaeger, L.E. Miller, E.C. Goins, C.E. Super, and M.D. Spritzer made significant contributions to the design of this study and to the writing of the manuscript. All co-authors contributed to data collection, data analysis and interpretation, provided input on revisions of this manuscript, and approved the final version of the submitted manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded by Middlebury College and the National Institute of Aging (NIH AREA grant number 1R15AG042155). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of funding agencies. The funding agencies had no role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the report for publication.

Declaration of Competing Interest

None.

Acknowledgements

We thank Lucy Botswick, Eliza Ferrari, Sedge Lucas, Laura Nelson, Leslie Panella, Maddie Pronovost, Ethan Roy, and Mark Sinks for their assistance with data collection. Melissa Glen and Clarissa Parker provided guidance on brain tissue processing. Mark Stefani assisted with maze construction and behavioral protocols. Jan Thornton provided guidance on the OLMT protocol. We also thank Vicki Major, Alexis Paquette, and the rest of the Middlebury College animal care staff.

References (77)

  • T.B. Franklin et al.

    Sex and ovarian steroids modulate brain-derived neruotrophic factor (BDNF) protein levels in rat hippocampus under stressful and non-stressful conditions

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2006)
  • K.M. Frick et al.

    Age-related spatial reference and working memory deficits assessed in the water maze

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1995)
  • R.B. Gibbs

    Testosterone and estradiol produce different effects on cognitive performance in male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2005)
  • E. Goudsmit et al.

    Testosterone fails to reverse spatial memory decline in aged rats and impairs retention in your and middle-aged animals

    Behav. Neural Biol.

    (1990)
  • M. Guidi et al.

    Contribution of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors to attention and episodic spatial memory during senescence

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (2015)
  • J. Holland et al.

    Testosterone levels and cognition in elderly men: a review

    Maturitas

    (2011)
  • S.A. Jablonski et al.

    Determinants of novel object and location recognition during development

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • T.K. Jacobson et al.

    Extensive training and hippocampus or striatum lesions: effect on place and response strategies

    Physiol. Behav.

    (2012)
  • J. Jia et al.

    Amelioratory effects of testosterone treatment on cognitive performance deficits induced by soluble Aβ1-42 oligomers injected into the hippocampus

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • R. Katoh-Semba et al.

    Age-related changes in levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in selected brain regions of rats, normal mice and senescence-accelerated mice: a comparison to those of nerve growth factor and neurotrophin-3

    Neurosci. Res.

    (1998)
  • M. Li et al.

    Testosterone has sublayer-specific effects on dendritic spine maturation mediated by BDNF and PSD-95 in pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus CA1 area

    Brain Res.

    (2012)
  • M. Liljeholm et al.

    Contributions of the striatum to learning, motivation, and performance: an associative account

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2012)
  • M.N. Locklear et al.

    Assessment of the effects of sex and sex hormones on spatial cognition in adult rats using the Barnes maze

    Horm. Behav.

    (2014)
  • S.E.A. McConnell et al.

    The role of testicular hormones and luteinizing hormone in spatial memory in adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2012)
  • M. Moscovitch et al.

    The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2006)
  • T.V. Nguyen et al.

    Dihydrotestosterone activates CREB signaling in cultured hippocampal neurons

    Brain Res.

    (2009)
  • N.T. Nowak et al.

    Contributions of sex, testosterone, and androgen receptor CAG repeat number to virtual Morris water maze performance

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • M.G. Packard et al.

    Inactivation of the hippocampus or caudate nucleus with lidocaine differentially affects expression of place and response learning

    Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.

    (1996)
  • M. Ramsden et al.

    Androgens modulate neuronal vulnerability to kainate lesion

    Neuroscience

    (2003)
  • N.J. Sandstrom et al.

    Testosterone modulates performance on a spatial working memory task in male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2006)
  • N.W. Simon et al.

    Good things come to those who wait: attenuated discounting of delayed rewards in aged Fischer 344 rats

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2010)
  • M.D. Spritzer et al.

    Effects of testosterone on spatial learning and memory in adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2011)
  • M.D. Spritzer et al.

    Testosterone influences spatial strategy preferences among adult male rats

    Horm. Behav.

    (2013)
  • J. Stewart et al.

    The effects of life-long food restriction on spatial memory in young and aged Fischer 344 rats measured in the eight-arm radial and the Morris water mazes

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (1989)
  • B.A. Wagner et al.

    Effects of testosterone dose on spatial memory among castrated adult male rats

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2018)
  • O.T. Wolf et al.

    Testosterone and cognition in elderly men: a single testosterone injection blocks the practice effect in verbal fluency, but has no effect on spatial or verbal memory

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • L. Zurkovsky et al.

    Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems

    Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • C.A. Barnes

    Spatial memory deficit in senescent rats

    Can. J. Psychol.

    (1980)
  • Cited by (9)

    • Evaluation of the effect of nicotine and O-acetyl-L-carnitine on testosterone-induced spatial learning impairment in Morris water maze and assessment of protein markers

      2022, Learning and Motivation
      Citation Excerpt :

      Evidence from experiments of androgen effects on spatial memory in both animals and humans is complex and inconsistent. Some studies reported a positive link between testosterone and spatial ability (Alexander, Packard, & Hines, 1994; Vázquez-Pereyra, Rivas-Arancibia, Loaeza-Del Castillo, & Schneider-Rivas, 1995) and testosterone treatment caused dose-dependent improvements in spatial memory in rats (Jaeger et al., 2020). In addition, testosterone appears to increase adult neurogenesis within the hippocampus (Spritzer & Roy, 2020).

    • Dose-dependent effects of testosterone on spatial learning strategies and brain-derived neurotrophic factor in male rats

      2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, the effects of testosterone are not unique to appetitively motivated tasks, as a similar dose-dependent effect of testosterone was observed when an object-location memory task was used (Wagner et al., 2018). Furthermore, these effects seem to persist into old age, as we have recently obtained comparable results on the RAM using aged (20 months) male rats (Jaeger et al., 2020). It should be acknowledged that the serum testosterone concentrations were found to be significantly lower for rats in the 0.250 mg T group that was used for the response task compared to the rats used for the place task.

    • Impact of Testosterone on Alzheimer’s Disease

      2022, World Journal of Men's Health
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Authors contributed equally.

    View full text