Introduction

The hypothalamic peptide oxytocin (OXT) is a key modulator of social cognition and behaviors including fear learning [1, 2], stress responses [3,4,5], pair-bonding [6], and parent–infant attachment [7]. In both human and animal studies, OXT has been found to reduce threat-induced reactivity of the amygdala [1, 8,9,10,11,12,13]. Furthermore, current perspectives on the prosocial effects of OXT emphasize that the peptide enhances the salience of social signals by modulating the activation of the brain reward system [14,15,16,17]. This dual mechanism of action renders OXT a promising system for the treatment of various mental disorders associated with impaired social interaction abilities, such as autism spectrum or anxiety disorders [18, 19]. However, the rapid translation into clinical efficacy is currently hampered by limited insights into the dose-response relationships of OXT. We have recently shown that a dose of 24 international units (IU) appears optimal for inhibiting amygdala responses to social fear signals in healthy men [20]. However, there is accumulating evidence that the same dose causes the opposite effect in women [9, 21, 22]. Similarly, sexual-dimorphic anxiolytic effects have been observed in mice [23] and rats [24]. Possible sex-specific OXT effects on the brain reward system are less consistent, but have also been observed. While OXT enhances activation in reward-associated brain regions in response to the romantic partner in both sexes [14,15,16], it selectively increases striatal reactivity to reciprocated cooperation in men but not in women [25,26,27,28,29].

Further differential effects of OXT in women and men have been documented in various domains. For instance, in women, 24 IU of OXT improved the ability to recognize familial relationships [30], biased them to respond more altruistically [31], and increased the magnitude of perceived support during recall of negative autobiographical memories in a social context [32]. In men, 24 IU of OXT facilitated the accurate perception of competition, promoted self-interest moral judgements and had no significant effect on recalling emotional autobiographical memories. Given this empirical background, one might assume that OXT may have evolved to prompt affiliative efforts aimed at restoring positive social contacts in women (“tend-and-befriend” model) [33], but may primarily affect fight-or-flight responses via anxiolysis in men.

However, little is known about the basic mechanisms underlying these sexual-dimorphic effects. OXT might interact with gonadal hormones such as progesterone and estradiol, whose levels differ between women and men and which are implicated in the neural processing of emotional stimuli [34, 35]. Furthermore, OXT binding sites are widely distributed in the brain, and animal studies suggest that OXT receptor densities differ between sexes in various brain regions interconnected with the amygdala or striatal regions (e.g. the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis) [36, 37].

Importantly, however, higher doses of intranasal OXT (48 IU) tend to enhance amygdala reactivity to fearful stimuli in men [20], indicating an inverted-U relationship between the amount of administered OXT and anxiolytic effects on threat-induced amygdala activation. Assuming this inverted-U dose-response relationship is also true for women, sex-specific effects of OXT treatments might also be a byproduct of a shifted dose-dependent target engagement in women, initiated at lower doses compared to men [28, 38]. We thus hypothesized that lower doses might be needed in women to reveal a decrease in amygdala reactivity as observed in men after the administration of 24 IU of OXT.

Therefore, the present randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study aims to investigate potential dose-dependent effects of intranasal OXT on amygdala and striatal responses to emotional faces in healthy women and to validate the sex-specificity of OXT effects. A total of 92 healthy, right-handed women were randomly assigned to three doses of OXT (24 IU, 12 IU, and 6 IU). The rationale of focusing on lower rather than higher doses was based on evidence for higher baseline endogenous OXT levels in women compared to men [39], such that lower doses might be needed to produce anxiolytic effects in women. We thus hypothesized that 24 IU of intranasal OXT would increase amygdala responses to fearful stimuli in women, whereas lower doses (6 IU or 12 IU) would inhibit these responses. Since an inverted-U relationship has also been hypothesized between OXT dose and reward-associated brain activation [28], and given evidence for enhanced brain reward system activity after administration of 24 IU intranasal OXT in women [15, 16], we predicted heightened striatal responses to positive social stimuli after OXT administration and further hypothesized that this effect would be more pronounced after 24 IU compared to lower doses (6 IU or 12 IU). Finally, we hypothesized that OXT effects after administration of equivalent doses would significantly differ between sexes, whereas effects of lower doses of OXT in women (6, 12, and 24 IU) would mirror the effects of higher doses of OXT in men (12, 24, and 48 IU, respectively).

Materials and methods

Experimental design and participants

We used a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover design. Out of a total of 105 participants who were invited to a screening session prior to the testing sessions, 92 healthy, right-handed women were eligible for enrollment and randomly assigned to three groups differing in doses of administered treatment: 6 IU (n = 31), 12 IU (n = 31), and 24 IU (n = 30; for details, see Supplementary Materials and Methods). The study was approved by the local ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Bonn, Germany, and carried out in accordance with the latest revision of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent and were free of current physical or psychiatric illness as assessed by self-report and the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI [40]) in the screening session. None of the participants used hormonal contraceptives or was pregnant while taking part in the study. Following the screening session, participants underwent two identical testing sessions on separate days (one session after placebo [PLC] and one session after OXT administration), scheduled at least one month apart. The order of substance administration was randomized across participants. Two participants were excluded after the first testing session as they no longer fulfilled inclusion criteria (see Supplementary Materials and Methods), resulting in 90 participants (mean age ± SD = 23.9 ± 4.4 years) for the analyses (n = 30 in each dose group). Furthermore, 11 participants were excluded from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analyses due to technical malfunctions (n = 2) or excessive head motion ( >4 mm/°; n = 9). Thus, the final sample for fMRI analyses consisted of 79 participants (mean age ± SD = 24.0 ± 4.3 years; 6 IU: n = 28, 12 IU: n= 26, 24 IU: n = 25). There were no a priori differences between dose groups regarding demographic and psychological measurements (see Table S1).

Testing sessions

Participants completed two testing sessions, both including an fMRI scan and questionnaires measuring mood and state anxiety at the beginning and end of the testing session (the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, PANAS [41] and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI [42]; see Table S2). After completion of the questionnaires, participants self-administered 6, 12, or 24 IU of synthetic OXT (depending on dose group; Sigma-Tau Industrie Farmaceutiche Riunite S.p.A., Rome, Italy) or PLC via nasal spray under the supervision of a trained research assistant and in accordance with the latest standardization guidelines [43] (for details, see Supplementary Materials and Methods). The PLC solution contained identical ingredients except for the peptide itself. Imaging data were collected 45 min after substance administration while participants completed an emotional face recognition task [20]. Briefly, participants were instructed to decide via button press whether morphed face stimuli displayed a neutral, fearful, or happy expression of varying emotional intensities (neutral, low and high fearful, low and high happy) (for details, see [20]). Blood was sampled before treatment administration (baseline) and immediately after fMRI scanning. All participants were tested in the luteal phase of their menstrual cycle as assessed by self-report and validated by blood assays (estradiol and progesterone concentrations; see Supplementary Materials and Methods).

fMRI data acquisition and analysis

Imaging data were recorded on a 1.5 T Siemens Avanto MRI system (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) using an amygdala-sensitive sequence with an echo time (TE) decreased linearly by 10 ms between slice 9 and slice 14, resulting in a final TE of 40 ms [44]. Data were preprocessed (see Supplementary Materials and Methods) and analyzed using standard procedures in SPM8 software (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK) implemented in MATLAB 7.11 (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA) as in our previous study [20]. A two-stage approach based on the general linear model implemented in SPM8 was used for statistical analyses. For region of interest analyses, the amygdala and striatal regions (putamen, caudate nucleus, globus pallidus, and nucleus accumbens) were anatomically defined according to the Wake Forest University Pick Atlas [45, 46]. P values < 0.05 after correction for multiple comparisons (family-wise error, FWE) were considered significant. Finally, to examine possible dose-dependent effects, parameter estimates of significant OXT effects on amygdala and striatal responses to fearful or happy faces relative to neutral faces were averaged across all voxels of anatomically defined regions of interest, extracted, and analyzed in SPSS 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) using mixed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) and Bonferroni-corrected (pcorr) post hoc t-tests. We also explored whole brain OXT effects across all participants and dose groups (for details, see Supplementary Materials and Methods).

Behavioral, demographic, and neuroendocrine data were analyzed using standard procedures in SPSS 24 including mixed ANOVAs and t-tests. Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied if the assumption of sphericity was significantly violated as assessed with Mauchly’s test. Partial eta-squared and Cohen’s d were calculated as measures of effect size. To examine whether individual characteristics of the participants moderate the effectiveness of OXT, we conducted explorative Pearson’s product-moment correlations between OXT effects and behavioral, demographic, psychological, and neuroendocrine measurements. For results of behavioral data and correlation analyses, see Supplementary Results.

To test the hypothesis of sexual-dimorphic OXT effects, parameter estimates of significant OXT effects in the female sample and the sample of our previous study with healthy men [20] were analyzed using mixed ANOVAs with the additional between-subject factor sex and two-sample t-tests. For details of comparative analyses, see Supplementary Materials and Methods.

Data analysis was preregistered prior to conducting any analyses (https://osf.io/yxrsf/).

Results

Neuroendocrine parameters

Estradiol, progesterone, progesterone/estradiol ratio, and OXT levels at baseline differed neither between dose groups nor between OXT and PLC testing sessions (see Table S3). No interaction between testing session and dose group was found (all Fs < 1.90, all ps > 0.15).

A mixed ANOVA with the within-subject factor treatment (OXT, PLC), the between-subject factor dose group (6 IU, 12 IU, 24 IU), and the pre-post difference in OXT plasma concentrations revealed main effects of treatment (F(1,85) = 338.00, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.80) and dose group (F(2,85) = 23.76, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.36). Furthermore, a significant interaction between treatment and dose group indicated a difference in the magnitude of OXT increase between dose groups (F(2,85) = 16.16, p< 0.001, ηp2 = 0.28). Post hoc tests revealed that the plasma OXT concentration was significantly increased after OXT administration in all dose groups (all ts > 11.51, all pscorr < 0.001, all ds > 3.25), but increases in OXT levels after 24 IU and 12 IU of OXT in contrast to PLC were significantly greater compared to 6 IU (24 IU vs. 6 IU: t(56) = 5.63, pcorr < 0.001, d = 1.48; 12 IU vs. 6 IU: t(43.27) = 5.16, pcorr < 0.001, d = 1.34), whereas there was no difference between the 12 and 24 IU groups (t(57) = 0.06, pcorr ≈ 1.00; see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1: Changes in plasma oxytocin (OXT) concentration after OXT and placebo (PLC) administration relative to baseline.
figure 1

Administration of 12 and 24 international units (IU) of OXT induced a significantly greater increase in OXT levels than 6 IU. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. Abbreviations: corr, Bonferroni-corrected; IU, international units; OXT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo. ***pcorr < 0.001.

fMRI results

OXT effects on amygdala responses

Across all participants and dose groups, OXT significantly enhanced right amygdala responses to low fearful (i.e. [OXT low fearful > neutral] > [PLC low fearful > neutral]; Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] peak coordinates x, y, z: 30, −8, −12, t(78) = 3.64, k = 135, pFWE = 0.02, see Fig. 2a) but not to high fearful faces (MNI peak coordinates x, y, z: 28, −2, −16, t(78) = 2.39, k = 25, pFWE = 0.25). No significant effects of OXT on left amygdala responses to low or high fearful faces or to fearful faces across emotional intensities were found (all ts < 2.40, all FWE-corrected ps > 0.22). Furthermore, we observed no dampening effects on amygdala activation (e.g. [OXT fearful > neutral] < [PLC fearful > neutral]) or significant OXT effects on amygdala responses to happy faces. Mixed ANOVAs with treatment (OXT, PLC) and emotion (e.g. neutral, fearful) as within-subject factors, dose group (6 IU, 12 IU, 24 IU) as between-subject factor, and extracted parameter estimates averaged across right amygdala voxels as dependent variables were calculated to further investigate OXT effects on amygdala activation. Importantly, significant interactions of treatment and emotion confirmed OXT effects on responses to fearful (F(1,76) = 7.03, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.09) and low fearful faces (F(1,76) = 6.70, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08; see Fig. 2a) in contrast to neutral faces, whereas no interaction of treatment and emotion was observed for amygdala responses to high fearful faces (F(1,76) = 1.86, p = 0.18). Post hoc tests revealed significantly greater amygdala responses to fearful (t(78) = 4.14, pcorr < 0.001, d= 0.15) and low fearful faces (t(78) = 3.75, pcorr < 0.001, d= 0.15; see Fig. 2a) compared to neutral faces after OXT administration while no other post-hoc comparison reached significance (all ts < 1.86, all pscorr > 0.27). For further analyses of OXT effects on amygdala responses, see Supplementary Results.

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Intranasal oxytocin (OXT) significantly enhanced right amygdala responses to low fearful faces compared to neutral faces across doses (interaction of treatment multilpy emotion: F(1,76) = 6.70, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08). Post-hoc t-tests revealed significantly greater amygdala reactivity to low fearful compared to neutral faces after OXT administration (t(78) = 3.75, p < 0.001, d= 0.15). b The OXT-induced increase in amygdala activation did not differ between dose groups. c Intranasal OXT significantly enhanced right putamen responses to low happy faces compared to neutral faces (interaction of treatment multilpy emotion: F(1,76) = 9.13, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.11). Post-hoc t-tests revealed significantly greater putamen reactivity to low happy compared to neutral faces after OXT administration (t(78) = 2.38, p = 0.02, d= 0.16). d The observed OXT effect on right putamen responses to social stimuli across emotional valence (low happy and neutral faces) was more pronounced in the dose groups of 24 and 12 international units (IU) compared to 6 IU (24 IU vs. 6 IU: t(34.87) = 2.19, p = 0.04, d= 0.60; 12 IU vs. 6 IU: t(52) = 2.32, p = 0.02, d= 0.63). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: IU, international unit; L, left; OXT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; R, right. *p< 0.05, **p< 0.01, ***p< 0.001.

OXT effects on striatal reactivity

OXT significantly enhanced activation in response to happy faces (i.e. [OXT happy > neutral] > [PLC happy > neutral]) across all dose groups in the right putamen (MNI peak coordinates x, y, z: 34, −18, −4, t(78) = 3.83, k = 45, pFWE = 0.03). Mixed ANOVAs with extracted parameter estimates averaged across all right putamen voxels as dependent variables confirmed a significant treatment multilpy emotion interaction for putamen responses across emotional intensities (F(1,76) = 6.42, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08) and for low happy faces (F(1,76) = 9.13, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.11; see Fig. 2c). Post-hoc tests indicated enhanced putamen reactivity to low happy compared to neutral faces after OXT administration (t(78) = 2.38, p = .02 [pcorr = 0.08]; see Fig. 2c; all other ts< 1.67, all pscorr > 0.40). In contrast, no significant interaction of treatment and emotion was found for putamen responses to high happy faces (F(1,76) = 2.43, p = 0.12). Therefore, we decided to focus on right putamen reactivity to happy faces across emotional intensities and to low happy faces. Additional analyses of putamen responses to happy faces and further results of OXT effects on striatal reactivity are reported in the Supplementary Results.

Dose-dependent effects

Importantly, no dose-dependent effects (main effect of dose or interactions with dose group) were found for right amygdala responses to fearful faces across emotional intensities (all Fs < 1.24, all ps > 0.29) or low fearful faces (all Fs < 0.72, all ps > 0.49; see Fig. 2b), indicating that OXT effects did not significantly differ between dose groups.

In contrast, trend-to-significant interactions of treatment and dose group were found for right putamen reactivity, implying dose-dependent OXT effects on responses to social stimuli across happy and neutral (F(2,76) = 3.08, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08) as well as low happy and neutral faces (F(2,76) = 3.12, p = .05, ηp2 = 0.08; see Fig. 2d). Post hoc tests disentangling the potential dose-dependent OXT effects on right putamen activation are reported in the Supplementary Results. No other dose-dependent effects (main effect of dose or interactions with dose group) were found for right putamen reactivity to happy or low happy faces (all Fs < 2.35, all ps > 0.10).

Whole brain

OXT significantly enhanced responses to happy stimuli ([OXT happy > neutral] > [PLC happy > neutral]) in the left precuneus (MNI peak coordinates x, y, z: −4, −68, 30, t(78) = 4.34, k = 144, pFWE = 0.02; see Fig. 3). We found no evidence for effects on fear-specific brain activation or for a dampening OXT effect (e.g. [OXT happy > neutral] < [PLC happy > neutral]).

Fig. 3
figure 3

a Across doses, intranasal oxytocin (OXT) significantly enhanced the responses to happy faces in the left precuneus (Montreal Neurological Institute peak coordinates x, y, z: −4, −68, 30, t(78) = 4.34, k = 144, pFWE = 0.02). b Post hoc tests revealed significantly enhanced resposes to happy compared to neutral faces after OXT administration (t(78) = 3.40, p = 0.001, d= 0.19), whereas precuneus response under placebo (PLC) was greater for neutral faces (t(78) = 2.12, p = 0.04, d= 0.12). Precuneus reactivity to neutral faces was significantly reduced after OXT administration compared to PLC (t(78) = −2.37, p = 0.02, d= −0.23). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: FWE, familywise error corrected; L, left; OXT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; R, right. *p< 0.05, **p< 0.01.

Sexual-dimorphic OXT effects

Mixed ANOVAs with sex (female, male) as an additional between-subject factor were calculated for the observed OXT effects on right amygdala activation to low fearful faces, right putamen responses to happy faces across emotional intensities, and left amygdala reactivity to high fearful faces as evident in our previous study [20]. Parameter estimates of groups receiving the same dose of OXT in men and women were used as dependent variables (i.e. 12-IU- and 24-IU-dose groups with a latency of 45 min). Results revealed a significant interaction of treatment and sex on right amygdala reactivity (F(1,86) = 4.06, p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.05; see Fig. 4a) and interactions of treatment multilpy emotion multilpy sex on left amygdala response (F(1,86) = 4.74, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.05; see Fig. 4b) and right putamen reactivity (F(1,86) = 5.73, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.06; see Fig. 4c). Four post hoc two-sample t-tests were calculated for each significant interaction to compare parameter estimates of baseline contrasts between sexes. Tests showed significantly enhanced right amygdala reactivity to low fearful faces after OXT administration in women compared to men (t(68.11) = 2.60, pcorr = 0.046, d = 0.55), whereas a trend-to-significant smaller baseline left amygdala activation to high fearful faces (t(66.41) = −2.39, pcorr = 0.08) in women was no longer evident after OXT administration (t57.87) = 0.75, pcorr ≈ 1.00). In the right putamen, female participants showed attenuated baseline reactivity to neutral (t(88) = −3.07, pcorr = 0.01, d = −0.65) and happy faces (t(88) = −4.46, pcorr < 0.001, d = −0.95). Again, a-priori sex differences were reduced after OXT administration, with women still exhibiting smaller responses to neutral faces (t(76.66) = −2.97, pcorr = 0.02, d = −0.63) but not happy faces (t(79.84) = −2.03, pcorr = 0.18). No interactions of treatment and sex with dose were observed (all F < 1.06, all ps > 0.30), indicating that sex-specific OXT effects do not significantly differ between 12 and 24 IU of OXT. For further comparisons of sexual-dimorphic OXT effects between doses, see Supplementary Results.

Fig. 4: Effects of intranasal oxytocin (OXT) differed significantly between women and men ([20]; for dose groups of 12 and 24 IU with a latency of 45 min).
figure 4

a Across neutral and low fearful faces, right amygdala reactivity was enhanced in female participants but attenuated in male participants (interaction of treatment multilpy sex: F(1,86) = 4.06, p = 0.047, ηp2 = 0.05). b Likewise, OXT effects on emotion-specific left amygdala responses to high fearful faces differed between sexes (interaction of treatment multilpy emotion multilpy sex: F(1,86) = 4.74, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.05). c OXT effects on emotion-specific right putamen activation in response to happy faces were significantly more pronounced in women compared to men (interaction of treatment multilpy emotion multilpy sex: F(1,86) = 5.73, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.06). Error bars indicate standard error of the mean. Abbreviations: L, left; OXT, oxytocin; PLC, placebo; R, right. *p< 0.05.

Discussion

The present PLC-controlled, crossover design study sought to investigate dose-dependent OXT effects on reactivity of the amygdala and the brain reward system in healthy women by comparing the modulatory impact of three different doses (6 IU, 12 IU, and 24 IU) of intranasally administered OXT. Results revealed that OXT significantly enhanced amygdala responses to fearful and low fearful faces independent of dosing in the administered range. Furthermore, we found that OXT significantly increased striatal reactivity to happy and low happy faces and that these OXT effects might differ between dose groups, although post-hoc tests for this treatment multilpy dose interaction did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. Given significant differences between the sexes in the observed OXT effects, our data are consistent with the notion that OXT has opposing effects on amygdala reactivity in women and men and that these sex-specific effects are not only a byproduct of the hypothesized dose-response function following lower doses in women. OXT effects on striatal responses to happy faces, on the other hand, were more pronounced in women and striatal sexual-dimorphic baseline response patterns vanished after OXT administration.

Our findings suggest that OXT increases the processing of social stimuli in women by strengthening the sensitivity for negative signals in the amygdala and by potentiating reward-related responses in the striatum. Importantly, OXT effects were more pronounced for low intensity emotional faces and absent when the salience of social stimuli was high due to high emotion intensity. Our findings therefore support the social salience hypothesis of OXT [17]. From an evolutionary perspective, OXT-induced increases in social salience may contribute to maternal bonding as stronger intrinsic connectivity within an overlapping network comprising the amygdala and striatal areas has been linked to both mothers’ behavioral synchrony with their infants and plasma OXT levels [47]. Importantly, previous studies have reported increased perceived social support [32] and enhanced stress-buffering effects of social support in women after the administration of OXT [48], suggesting that increased OXT signaling may still result in anti-anxiety effects in women when the OXT administration is combined with a supporting intervention. Thus, prosocial effects of OXT in men may primarily operate via anxiolysis, [1, 3,4,5, 8, 9, 13] whereas OXT may promote affiliative efforts and social bonding in women [6, 33] in a safe context by enhancing the salience of positive social cues in striatal regions and in the precuneus. Furthermore, in ambiguous situations, enhanced amygdala activation after OXT administration may result in avoidance behavior, but because OXT has been found to signal relationship distress in women [33], enhanced amygdala activation may provide the impetus for social contact to ameliorate stress.

Sexual-dimorphic OXT effects could be driven by interactions between gonadal steroid hormones and OXT. On the one hand, higher baseline levels of progesterone in healthy women are associated with hypersensitivity to negative stimuli in emotion processing brain regions including the amygdala [34, 49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. On the other hand, reward sensitivity and reward-related striatal activation seem to be enhanced in the late follicular phase of the menstrual cycle when estradiol levels are high and progesterone levels are low [35, 57,58,59]. These findings of reduced striatal responses to positive stimuli when progesterone levels are raised might explain the observed attenuated baseline putamen reactivity to happy faces in our female sample compared to male participants, as female participants were tested in the luteal phase when progesterone levels are high [60]. Thus, because women have higher estrogen and progesterone levels than men, a small dose of OXT (i.e. 6 IU) may be sufficient to intensify the sensitivity for potential threats in women. Importantly, however, previous studies, which examined the effects of 24 IU on amygdala reactivity to fearful faces in healthy women in the luteal [21] or follicular phase [61], consistently found an increase in amygdala activation, indicating that the direction of the OXT effect does not change with hormonal levels fluctuating across the menstrual cycle.

Another possible explanation for sexual-dimorphic OXT effects might be a difference in the distribution of OXT receptors in women and men, as is evident in rodents [36, 37]. However, no sex differences have been found to date for OXT binding sites in the human brain [62]. Importantly, OXT also binds to receptors of the related nonapeptide arginine vasopressin (AVP), although with lower affinity [63]. Considering the evidence for AVP effects in men, which are missing or even reversed in women [25, 26, 64,65,66], sexually-specific OXT effects might result from an interaction with the AVP system. Therefore, reported dose-dependent effects on amygdala activation in men missing in women might be explained by OXT occupying AVP receptors and producing AVP-like effects at higher OXT doses [20, 67].

Furthermore, opposing OXT effects on amygdala reactivity in women and men may be related to different intra-amygdalar targets of OXT. The amygdala is not a single homogenous structure but rather comprises several functionally specialized subnuclei that respond differentially to emotional stimuli [68]. Given a higher sensitivity of the centromedial amygdala to negative emotional stimuli compared to the basal amygdala [68], it is conceivable that OXT might reduce sensitivity of the centromedial amygdala in men while improving the sensitivity of basal nuclei in women. However, future studies with ultra-field imaging are required to reliably differentiate amygdala subnuclei.

Our study has some limitations. First, as we expected a reduction of amygdala activation in women following lower doses, we compared the effects of 24 IU OXT with those of smaller doses. Higher doses than 24 IU might be needed to detect the hypothesized inverted-U relationship between OXT dose and reward-associated brain activation [28] or dose-dependent effects on amygdala activation. Second, we explored the kinetics of OXT in a sample of healthy women, and further patient studies are warranted to extrapolate our findings to psychopathology.

Conclusively, our results provide support for the sexual-dimorphic effects of OXT and suggest that OXT increases the salience of social information in women by potentiating limbic and striatal reactivity to low intensity social stimuli. As such, our findings may have important implications for the therapeutic applications of OXT. Strict control of the therapeutic context and the combining of OXT with other anti-anxiety interventions may be required to produce anxiolytic effects in women. Further insights into sex-specific OXT signaling might also broaden our understanding of etiological and pathogenetic mechanisms underlying mental disorders associated with social dysfunctions and substantial sex differences in prevalence rates such as autism spectrum disorder.

Funding and disclosure

DS and RH are supported by a German Research Foundation (DFG) grant (SCHE 1913/5-1 and HU 1302/11-1). We report no competing biomedical financial interests or personal affiliations in connection with the content of this paper.