Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Stress among university students: factorial structure and measurement invariance of the Italian version of the Effort-Reward Imbalance student questionnaire

Abstract

Background

In the last decade academic stress and its mental health implications amongst university students has become a global topic. The use of valid and theoretically-grounded measures of academic stress in university settings is crucial. The aim of this study was to examine the factorial structure, reliability and measurement invariance of the short student version of the effort-reward imbalance questionnaire (ERI-SQ).

Methods

A total of 6448 Italian university students participated in an online cross-sectional survey. The factorial structure was investigated using exploratory factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis. Finally, the measurement invariance of the ERI-SQ was investigated.

Results

Results from explorative and confirmatory factor analyses showed acceptable fits for the Italian version of the ERI-SQ. A modified version of 12 items showed the best fit to the data confirming the 3-factor model. Moreover, multigroup analyses showed metric invariance across gender and university course (health vs other courses).

Conclusions

In sum, our results suggest that the ERI-SQ is a valid, reliable and robust instrument for the measurement of stress among Italian university students.

Peer Review reports

Background

In the last decade, there has been a growing attention in investigating stress risk factors and well-being consequences among university studentā€™s population [1, 2]. Stress and mental health of university students is a crucial public health subject as healthy students will be the healthier workers of the future. Attending university has the potential to become a positive and satisfying experience for studentsā€™ life. However, there is empirical evidence that being a student may become a stressful experience [1, 3,4,5,6]. Stallman and Hurst [2] distinguished between eustress, important for student motivation and success at university, and distress, harmful for studentā€™s well-being, as it exposes to a higher risk of psychological (for example, anxiety and burnout), behavioral (for example eating disorders), physical health problems (for example, ulcers, high blood pressure, and headaches), and suicidal ideation [7,8,9,10]. Furthermore, many scholars found that high stress was linked to reduced academic performance, low grade averages, and low rates of graduation and higher dropout [11,12,13,14,15].

Academic stressors have been identified as including high workload, attending lessons, respecting deadlines, balancing university and private life, and economic issues. Those stressors are linked to a greater risk of distress and reduced academic achievement [1, 16,17,18,19].

Many authors adopted and extended original measures of stress, for example, by adapting work related stress measures to the university context [20, 21]. Most of these measures were designed for medical students [22] or employed measures of stress not specifically developed for the academic context [20,21,22].

According to Hilger-Kolb, Diehl, Herr, and Loerbroks [23], the vast majority of these measures lack a stress theoretical model. It may represent an important limitation as, meausers based on a common tested stress model may be better help researchers to capture the links between stress and health among university students and to develop theory-based interventions [21]. Effort-Reward Imbalance (ERI) [24] is among the most common tested and valid models of stress. According to this model, when high efforts are balanced by low rewards, the resulting imbalance may generate negative emotions and sustained stress experiences. Originally developed to investigate stress risks among workers, this model has been the theorethical root of many studies investigating stress in non-working contexts.

Recently, Wege, Muth, Angerer, and Siegrist [25] extended the original ERI model to the context of university and adapted the ERI short questionnaire to the university setting, showing good psychometric properties. Thus, according to this theoretical approach, studentsā€™ stress was defined as the result of an imbalance between effort, such as high study load, and reward, such as being respected from supervisors.

A vast number of empirical studies measuring effortā€“reward imbalance in workplace context confirmed good psychometric qualities of the ERI short questionnaire [26, 27]. Furthermore, psychometrically validated versions have been tested in 9 languages and in large European cohort studies, confirming the good psychometric qualities of the short ERI [28, 29].

Concerning the student version of the ERI, there is limited psychometric information available. Given the importance of academic stress for understanding studentsā€™ mental health risk, the aim of this study was to investigate the psychometric properties of the Italian version of the ERI-student questionnaire [25]. To address this goal, we examined the factor structure of the Italian version of the ERI-SQ, assessed internal consistency for the dimensions of effort, reward, and over-commitment, and test the measurement invariance of the ERI-SQ.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The study population (convenience sample) was recruited through a public announcement at electronic learning platforms for students and university studentsā€™ associationsā€™ network that contained an invitation for participating in a ā€œHealth Promoting Universityā€ survey. The online survey was implemented with Limesurvey from October 16th, 2017 to November 27th, 2017 and was restricted to enrolled university students (bachelor level and master level). The surveyā€™s homepage reported the online informed consent form with specific information about study purpose, general description of the questionnaire, including information about risks and benefits of participation. Also, the time necessary to complete the survey (less than 10ā€‰min) and privacy policy information were reported. Specifically, to ensure anonimity, we did not register ip address neither requested any another sensitive data. The investigators and research team did not employ any active advertising to increase recruitment rates neither played any active role in selecting and/or targeting specific subpopulations of respondents. A total of 9883 students agreed to participate in the survey with 6448 (65.24%) completing the survey (target population: 1.654.680 Italian university students in 2017). The Italian version of the ERI-SQ (see Table 4 in Appendix) was translated following the back-translation procedure [30].

Demographics

The sample for this research consisted of 75.5% females (nā€‰=ā€‰4869). Participants in this study ranged from 19 to 56ā€‰years of age, Mā€‰=ā€‰22.97, SDā€‰=ā€‰3.01. 56.2% (3624) were enrolled in bachelor prrogrammes and 43.8% (2824) in master programmes. 39.6% (2551) were enrolled in health related courses (such as medicine, nursing, psychology, and biomedical science).

Measures

Stress was assessed with the ERI-SQ [25] that was developed for use in student samples. The version adopted in this study consists of 14 items that constitute three scales: Effort (EFF; 3 items; example: ā€œI have constant time pressure due to a heavy study loadā€), Rewards (REW; 6 items; example: ā€œI receive the respect I deserve from my supervisors/teachersā€), and over-commitment (OC; 6 items; example: ā€œAs soon as I get up in the morning I start thinking about study problemsā€). All items are scored on a 4-point rating scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Average scores of items ratings for each subscale were calculated following appropriate recoding.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed with R [31] and Rstudio [32]. The factorial structure was investigated using exploratory factor analysis (EFA; psych package) [33] and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA; lavaan package) [34]. The dataset was randomly split in half to allow for independent EFA (training set) and CFA (test set). A robust ML estimator was used for correcting violations of multivariate normality.

The analyses were conducted in two stages. Firstly, an EFA with principal axis factor (PAF) analysis was performed. Using Hornā€™s Parallel Analysis for factor retention. Internal consistency was assessed via Cronbachā€™s alpha coefficient.

The second stage of analysis involved investigating the factor structure of the Italian version of the ERI-SQ, a series of CFA were performed. As Mardiaā€™s test of multivariate kurtosis (28.78, pā€‰<ā€‰.0001) showed multivariate non-normality, we investigated model fit with robust maximum likelihood (MLM) [35]. We compared alternative models: a 1-factor model, in which all 14 items were assessed as one common factor, a 3-factor model where items reflected the three subscales of the ERI-SQ, and a three-factor model with adjustments made according to error theory. We considered several fit indices: Ļ‡2(S-B Ļ‡2) [36], the robust root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA); the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) and the robust comparative fit index (CFI). For CFI, scoreā€‰>ā€‰.90 indicated acceptable model fit. For both RMSEA and SRMR, scoreā€‰ā‰¤ā€‰.05 was considered a good fit, andā€‰ā‰„ā€‰.08 a fair fit [37, 38].

Finally, the measurement invariance of the ERI-SQ was investigated. We performed a series of multi-group CFAs. We tested 5 nested models with progressive constrained parameters: Model 0 tested for configural invariance; Model 1 tested for metric invariance (constrained factor loadings); Model 2 tested for scalar invariance (constrained factor loadings and item intercepts); Model 3 tested for uniqueness invariance (constrained factor loadings, item intercepts, and residual item variances/covariances); Model 4 tested for structural invariance (constrained factor loadings, item intercepts, and factor variances/covariances). Models were compared by using the chi-square (Ļ‡2) [39]. In comparing nested models, we considered changes in CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR indices as follows: Ī”CFI ā‰¤ āˆ’ā€‰0.02 [40, 41], Ī”RMSEA ā‰¤0.015, and Ī”SRMR ā‰¤0.03 for tests of factor loading invariance [40, 42] and Ī”CFI ā‰¤-0.01, RMSEA ā‰¤0.015, and SRMR ā‰¤0.01 for test of scalar invariance [42].

Results

Exploratory factor analysis

We split the dataset (nĀ =Ā 6448) into random training and test samples. EFA was performed on the training sample (nĀ =ā€‰3879). Results from parallel analysis with 5000 parallel data sets using 95th percentile random eigenvalue showed that the eigenvalues for the first three factors exceeded those generated by the random data sets. Subsequently, a three-factor solution was inspected in a principal axis factor analysis with varimax rotation on the 14 items of the ERI-SQ (Table 1).

Table 1 Factor patter matrix for the Italian version of the ERI-SQ

The EFA revealed that two items (EFF2 ā€œI have many interruptions and disturbances while preparing for my examsā€ and REW4r ā€œ I am not sure whether I can successfully accomplish my university trainingsā€) loaded on the same factor. An item analysis revealed that, probably, both items have a general and ambiguous formulation among student population. These items were therefore deleted from all analyses, as subsequent analyses were conducted with the remaining 12 items. We then re-conducted a principle axis factor analysis with varimax rotation. The three factors collectively explained 40.0% of the variance in the three facets. After rotation, the factors were interpreted as effort, reward and over-commitment.

Confirmatory factor analysis

Based on the results from the EFA, three models were tested on the test sample (nā€‰=ā€‰3879; TableĀ 2).

Table 2 Fit Indices of the MBI-GS Students from the CFA

Fit indices for the unidimensional model S-BĻ‡2(54)ā€‰=ā€‰1833.95, rCFIā€‰=ā€‰.78, rTLIā€‰=ā€‰.73, RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.109, SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.084 suggested that the model did not provide a good fit to the data. We next considered the three-factor model [21]. Fit indices suggested this model fits the data well, S-BĻ‡2(51)ā€‰=ā€‰384.17, rCFIā€‰=ā€‰.96, rTLIā€‰=ā€‰.95, rRMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.048, SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.033. The Ļ‡2 difference test was significant, Ī”S-BĻ‡2(3)ā€‰=ā€‰1449.79, pā€‰<ā€‰.001. All standardized factor loadings were significant.

Internal consistency was .66 for reward, and .78 for overcommitment. Correlations between the three latent factors were as follows: āˆ’.30 between effort and reward, .52 between effort and over-commitment, āˆ’.33 between reward and over-commitment. Mean scores were: effortā€‰=ā€‰3.04 (SDā€‰=ā€‰0.59), rewardā€‰=ā€‰2.67 (SDā€‰=ā€‰0.48) and over-commitmentā€‰=ā€‰2.65 (SDā€‰=ā€‰0.63). The mean value of the effort-reward ratio was 1.20 (SDā€‰=ā€‰0.41).

Measurement invariance

Next, for testing measurement invariance, we conducted a series of multi-group CFAs across different groups: health (medicine, nursing, etc.) vs other courses (engineering, economy, etc.) and gender (male vs female).

First, a series of multi-group CFA (MGCFA) was conducted on the health and other university courses. TableĀ 3 shows that configural invariance was supported (Model 0) as fit the data well across health courses (nā€‰=ā€‰2551) and other courses (nā€‰=ā€‰3897): S-BĻ‡2(102)ā€‰=ā€‰398.06, CFIā€‰=ā€‰.962, RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.045, SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.032. All loadings were significant (pā€‰<ā€‰.01). We found support for metric invariance (Model 1): Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.001, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.001, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.002. Next, we did not find support for scalar invariance (Model 2; Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ .043; Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.019, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.017). As full scalar invariance was not supported, we tested for partial invariance. Inspecting modification indices, we found that three items from the reward subscale (REW2 ā€œI receive the respect I deserve from my fellow studentsā€; REW3 ā€œI am treated unfairly at universityā€; and REW6 ā€œConsidering all my efforts and achievements, my job promotion prospects are adequateā€) and all items from the over-commitment subscale lacked invariance. However, as showed on Table 3, partial scalar invariance (Model 2b) was not supported (Ī”CFā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.021, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.012, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.011).

Table 3 Test of invariance of the proposed three-factor structure of the ERI-SQ between health courses (nā€‰=ā€‰2551) and other courses (nā€‰=ā€‰3897) students, and female (nā€‰=ā€‰4869) vs male students (nā€‰=ā€‰1579): results of multigroup confirmatory factor analyses

Next, we performed a series of MGCFAs to test the invariance of the ERI-SQ between female and male students (Table 3). We found support for configural invariance (Model 0) across female (nā€‰=ā€‰4869) and male (nā€‰=ā€‰1579) groups: S-BĻ‡2(102)ā€‰=ā€‰445.20, CFIā€‰=ā€‰.956, RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.049, SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.033. All loadings were significant (pā€‰<ā€‰.01). Next, we found support for metric invariance (Model 1): Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’ .001, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.002, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.003. Next we found support for scalar invariance (Model 2): Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.009, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.003, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.002. Next uniqueness invariance (Model 3) was supported: Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.005, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.001, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.002. Finally, we found support for structural invariance (Model 4): Ī”CFIā€‰=ā€‰āˆ’.010, Ī”RMSEAā€‰=ā€‰.004, and Ī”SRMRā€‰=ā€‰.012.

Discussion

The main objective of this study was to examine the factorial validity and invariance of the Italian version of the ERI-SQ among Italian university students. Overall, our results confirmed the factorial structure underlying the ERI-SQ, as theorized by Siegrist [25] and reported by Wege and colleagues [25] in the student version of the ERI. However, in light of the conclusions drawn from the EFA, to enhance the fit of the model, we had to delete two items with high cross loadings. The deleted items were problematic in the Wege and colleagues [25] study too. Specifically, both items (EFF2 and REW4) showed a low factor loading in the CFA.

In the Italian sample, using a modified and shortened version (12 items) of the ERI-SQ, we confirmed the three factors structure components of the model, showing a satisfactory fit of the data structure with the theoretical concept. In sum, the current findings show that the ERI-SQ is as a reliable instrument for measuring academic stress among students.

Finally, as expected, we found support for metric invariance across gender and university course, health (medicine, nursing, etc.) vs other courses (engineering, economy, etc.). Mainly, MCFAs confirmed that the three-factor structure of the ERI-QS is (mostly) invariant across different groups. More specifically, we found support for parameter equivalence across gender (structural invariance), but the ERI-SQ was significantly different in health vs other courses. In fact, we were not able to find scalar invariance, suggesting that items REW2, REW3, REW6 and all the over-commitment items vary by academic courses. However, the lack of scalar invariance is a negligible issue for the Italian version of the ERI-SQ.

Implications and limitations

Results from our study showed that the Italian version of the ERI-SQ-10 provides a psychometrically sound measure of stress as defined in the ERI theoretical framework. The ERI-SQ is a brief and easy to administer university student stress measure. In this sense, using valid and reliable measures of stress is crucial for Italian university counselling services to advance in monitoring and understanding the levels of stress affecting students and how to support them. In this manner it would be possible to offer appropriate mental health support [43] when students are exposed to lack of reciprocity between spending high efforts and receiving low rewards during their student career.

The present study has several limitations. First, data were obtained from a convenience sample offering reduced generalizability of our results. However, for the purpose of the study this sample was deemed appropriate. Second, the Effort dimension was composed of only two items. A factor with only two items leads to a CFA that cannot be estimated unless constraining the model. Future research would overcome this limitation by reevaluating a wider version of the ERI and adapting other items from the Effort factor as defined in the ERI questionnaire [24]. Third, further research is also recommended concerning construct and criterion validity [44]. Specifically, we are not able to provide evidence of convergent validity (how closely the ERI-SQ is related to other variables and other measures of the same construct), and discriminant (ERI-SQ does not correlate with other variables that are theoretically not related). Future research would consider to analyse it by employing a multitrait-multimethod [45]. Finally, as one of the anonymous reviewers correctly pointed out, our study does not offer any evidence of criterion validity, mainly concurrent validity (the degree to which a measure correlates concurrently to an external criterion in the same domain [44]. However, according to Wege and colleagues [25], no studies have provided estimates of these validities for the ERI-SQ. Future research would provide evidence of it by analyzing the correlation between the ERI-SQ and a theoretically similar measure of student stress. In this sense, concurrent validity is an important area of future research. Fourth, we did not test for testā€“retest reliability. Future research should address these issues. Despite these important limitations, the Italian version of the ERI-SQ showed satisfactory psychometric properties.

Conclusions

In the present study, we found that the Italian version of the ERI-QS partially confirms the original version from Wege and colleagues [25]. We were able to show satisfactory psychometric properties of the ERI-SQ. Considering a high prevalence of academic distress among University students and the limited interventions aimed to reduce stress [46], universities should employ preventive interventions by measuring and controlling for potentially harmful psychosocial risk. In this sense, the Italian version of the ERI-QS presents a valid instrument for measuring academic stress on Italian-speaking university students.

Availability of data and materials

Raw data pertaining to analyses performed in this study are available available from the authors upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

CFA:

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

CFI:

Comparative Fit Index

EFA:

Exploratory Factor Analysis

EFF:

Effort

ERI:

Effort-Reward Imbalance

ERI-SQ:

Effort-Reward Imbalance Students Questionnaire

MGCFA:

Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis

ML:

Maximum Likelihood

MLM:

Robust Maximum Likelihood

OC:

Over-commitment

PAF:

Principal Axis Factor

REW:

Rewards

RMSEA:

Root Mean Square Error of Approximation

SD:

Standard Deviation

SRMR:

Standardized Root Mean Square Residual

References

  1. Stallman HM, Hurst CP. The university stress scale: measuring domains and extent of stress in university students. Aust Psychol. 2016;51:128ā€“34.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Stallman HM. Psychological distress in university students: a comparison with general population data. Aust Psychol. 2010;45(4):249ā€“57.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Chambel MJ, Curral L. Stress in academic life: work characteristics as predictors of student well-being and performance. Appl Psychol. 2005;54(1):135ā€“47.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Chiauzzi E, Brevard J, Thurn C, Decembrele S, Lord S. My student bodyā€“stress: an online stress management intervention for college students. J Health Commun. 2008;13(6):555ā€“72.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Salanova M, Schaufeli W, MartĆ­nez I, Breso E. How obstacles and facilitators predict academic performance: the mediating role of study burnout and engagement. Anxiety Stress Copin. 2010;23:53ā€“70.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Shin H, Puig A, Lee J, Lee JH, Lee SM. Cultural validation of the Maslach burnout inventory for Korean students. Asia Pac Educ Rev. 2011;12(4):633ā€“9.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Behere SP, Yadav R, Behere PB. A comparative study of stress among students of medicine, engineering, and nursing. Indian J Psychol Med. 2011;33(2):145ā€“8.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. Bergin A, Pakenham K. Law student stress: relationships between academic demands, social isolation, career pressure, study/life imbalance and adjustment outcomes in law students. Psychiat, Psych Law. 2015;22(3):388ā€“406.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  9. Rotenstein LS, Ramos MA, Torre M, Segal JB, Peluso MJ, Guille C, et al. Prevalence of depression, depressive symptoms, and suicidal ideation among medical students: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Jama. 2016;316(21):2214ā€“36.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. Portoghese I, Leiter MP, Maslach C, Galletta M, Porru F, Dā€™Aloja E, Finco G, Campagna M. Measuring Burnout Among University Students: Factorial Validity, Invariance, and Latent Profiles of the Italian Version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory Student Survey (MBI-SS). Front Psychol. 2018;9:2105.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  11. Dusselier L, Dunn B, Wang Y, Shelley IMC, Whalen DF. Personal, health, academic, and environmental predictors of stress for residence hall students. J Am Coll Heal. 2005;54(1):15ā€“24.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Storrie K, Ahern K, Tuckett A. A systematic review: students with mental health problemsā€”a growing problem. Int J Nurs Pract. 2010;16(1):1ā€“6.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Byrd DR, McKinney KJ. Individual, interpersonal, and institutional level factors associated with the mental health of college students. J Am Coll Heal. 2012;60(3):185ā€“93.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Keyes CL, Eisenberg D, Perry GS, Dube SR, Kroenke K, Dhingra SS. The relationship of level of positive mental health with current mental disorders in predicting suicidal behavior and academic impairment in college students. J Am Coll Heal. 2012;60(2):126ā€“33.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Salzer MS. A comparative study of campus experiences of college students with mental illnesses versus a general college sample. J Am Coll Heal. 2012;60(1):1ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Kerr S, Johnson VK, Gans SE, Krumrine J. Predicting adjustment during the transition to college: alexithymia, perceived stress, and psychological symptoms. J Coll Student Dev. 2004;45(6):593ā€“611.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Misra R, McKean M. College studentsā€™ academic stress and its relation to their anxiety, time management, and leisure satisfaction. Am J Health Stud. 2000;16:41ā€“51.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  18. Ryan ML, Shochet IM, Stallman HM. Universal online interventions might engage psychologically distressed university students who are unlikely to seek formal help. Adv Mental Health. 2010;9(1):73ā€“83.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  19. Shearer A, Hunt M, Chowdhury M, Nicol L. Effects of a brief mindfulness meditation intervention on student stress and heart rate variability. Int J Stress Manage. 2016;23(2):232ā€“54.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Dahlin M, Joneborg N, Runeson B. Stress and depression among medical students: a cross-sectional study. Med Educ. 2005;39(6):594ā€“604.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Dyrbye LN, Thomas MR, Shanafelt TD. Systematic review of depression, anxiety, and other indicators of psychological distress among US and Canadian medical students. Acad Med. 2006;81(4):354ā€“73.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Heinen I, Bullinger M, Kocalevent RD. Perceived stress in first year medical studentsā€”associations with personal resources and emotional distress. Bmc Med Educ. 2017;17:4.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  PubMed CentralĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. Hilger-Kolb J, Diehl K, Herr R, Loerbroks A. Effort-reward imbalance among students at German universities: associations with self-rated health and mental health. Int Arch Occ Env Hea. 2018;91(8):1011ā€“20.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  24. Siegrist J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. J Occup Health Psych. 1996;1(1):27ā€“41.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. Wege N, Li J, Muth T, Angerer P, Siegrist J. Student ERI: Psychometric properties of a new brief measure of effort-reward imbalance among university students. J Psychosom Res. 2017;94:64ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  26. Siegrist J, Starke D, Chandola T, Godin I, Marmot M, Niedhammer I, Peter R. The measurement of effortā€“reward imbalance at work: European comparisons. Soc Sci Med. 2004;58(8):1483ā€“99.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Leineweber C, Wege N, Westerlund H, Theorell T, Wahrendorf M, Siegrist J. How valid is a short measure of effortā€“reward imbalance at work? A replication study from Sweden. Occup Environ Med. 2010;67(8):526ā€“31.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Siegrist J, Dragano N, Nyberg ST, Lunau T, Alfredsson L, Erbel R, et al. Validating abbreviated measures of effort-reward imbalance at work in European cohort studies: the IPD-work consortium. Int Arch Occ Env Hea. 2014;87(3):249ā€“56.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Siegrist J, Wahrendorf M, Goldberg M, Zins M, Hoven H. Is effort-reward imbalance at work associated with different domains of health functioning? Baseline results from the French CONSTANCES study. Int Arch Occ Env Hea. 2019;92(4):467ā€“80.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  30. Brislin RW. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 1970;1(3):185ā€“216.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  31. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2017. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/

    Google ScholarĀ 

  32. RStudio Team RStudio. Integrated Development. Boston: R. R Studio, Inc; 2015. http://www.rstudio.com/

    Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Revelle W. Psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Evanston: Northwestern University; 2017.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  34. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling and more. Version 0.5ā€“12 (BETA). J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1ā€“36.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  35. Brown TA. Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford Publications; 2014.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  36. Satorra A, Bentler PM. Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis. In: von Eye A, Clogg CC, editors. Latent variable analysis: applications for developmental research. Thousand Oaks: Sage; 1994. p. 399ā€“419.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  37. Marsh HW, Hau KT, Wen Z. In search of golden rules: comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) findings. Struct Equ Model. 2004a;11(3):320ā€“41.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  38. Marsh HW, Wen Z, Hau KT. Structural equation models of latent interactions: evaluation of alternative estimation strategies and indicator construction. Psychol Methods. 2004b;9(3):275ā€“300.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  39. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238ā€“46.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  40. Meade AW, Johnson EC, Braddy PW. Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. J Appl Psychol. 2008;93(3):568ā€“92.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  41. Rutkowski L, Svetina D. Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international surveys. Educ Psychol Meas. 2014;74(1):31ā€“57.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  42. Chen FF. Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct Equ Modeling. 2007;14(3):464ā€“504.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  43. Roberti JW, Harrington LN, Storch E. Further psychometric support for the 10-item version of the perceived stress scale. J Coll Couns. 2006;9(2):135ā€“47.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  44. Cohen RJ, Swerdlik ME. Psychological testing and assessment. 6th ed. New York: McGraw Hill; 2005.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  45. Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psych Bull. 1959;56(2):81ā€“105.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  46. Regehr C, Glancy D, Pitts A. Interventions to reduce stress in university students: a review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disorders. 2013;148(1):1ā€“11.

    ArticleĀ  PubMedĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors gratefully acknowledge Prof. Johannes Siegrist and Prof. Nico Dragano for their careful reading and constructive feedbacks on the final draft of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was not funded.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IP, MG, FB and MC contributed to the conception and design of the study. IP, FB and AB contributed to the development procedure of the Italian version of ERI-SQ, including forward translation and back translation review. IP and FP contributed to the acquisition of data. IP analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MG, and AB supervised the analysis. SS, ED, GF and MC helped to draft and revise the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maura Galletta.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We conducted this study in accordance with (a) ethic committee of the University of Cagliari, (b) the Declaration of Helsinki in 1995 (as revised in Edinburgh 2000), and (c) with Italian privacy law (Decree No. 196/2003). Participation to the study was totally voluntary and written online informed consent was obtained by clicking on ā€œI acceptā€.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

IP is Associate Editor for BMC Psychology. However, this role was not in competing interest with the review of this manuscript. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisherā€™s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 4 Italian version of the ERI-SQ

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Portoghese, I., Galletta, M., Porru, F. et al. Stress among university students: factorial structure and measurement invariance of the Italian version of the Effort-Reward Imbalance student questionnaire. BMC Psychol 7, 68 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0343-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-0343-7

Keywords