Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published online by De Gruyter Mouton January 1, 2024

Differential indexing in Kamang: a viewpoint alternation

  • Katherine Walker ORCID logo EMAIL logo
From the journal Linguistics Vanguard

Abstract

In Kamang (Alor-Pantar, Indonesia), some verbs alternate between indexing the S or P argument with a prefix (from several different series) and occurring unprefixed; that is, Kamang has differential argument indexing. Through a qualitative study of a spoken-language corpus, this paper investigates the alternation between one of the prefix series and zero-marking. Previously described as indicating increased patientivity on intransitive motion and posture verbs, the alternation is here analysed in terms of a shift in event view: unprefixed verbs express events holistically, while prefixed verbs shift the viewpoint towards the “elaboration phase”, the temporal and causal middle and end of an event. Elaboration constructions are associated with resultative and completive semantics and, relatedly, greater affectedness of the undergoer argument. Cross-linguistically, such constructions (e.g., resultatives and middles) are frequently subject to lexical restrictions, and the same applies to the Kamang alternation.

1 Introduction

Differential argument indexing (DAI) is a type of alternation whereby arguments are expressed differently on different verbs or in different contexts.[1] For instance, in Kamang (Alor-Pantar, Indonesia), tak ‘run’ has no index for the S argument in (1a) and a third person index ge- in (1b).[2]

(1)
a.
kui tak
dog run
‘The dog runs.’
(Schapper 2014: 326)
b.
kui ge-tak
dog 3./e/-run
‘The dog ran off (was forced to run).’
(Schapper 2014: 326)

Previously, differential prefixes like those in (1b) have been described as indicating greater patientivity on intransitive motion and posture verbs (Schapper 2014: 325–326).[3] Based on the qualitative analysis of a small corpus of spoken Kamang, the present study expands this analysis. It shows that the alternation is less restricted than previously described: it applies to verbs from other semantic classes and to both intransitive and transitive verbs, though lexical restrictions persist. Further, while patientivity remains a factor, I present a new analysis in terms of a shift in event view, which is otherwise morphologically unmarked. That is, unprefixed verbs like in (1a) express events holistically, while prefixed verbs like in (1b) shift the viewpoint towards what I call the “elaboration phase”, the temporal and causal middle and end of an event. Elaboration constructions are associated with resultative and completive semantics and, relatedly, greater affectedness of the undergoer argument. Cross-linguistically, elaboration-phase constructions (e.g., resultatives and middles) are also frequently subject to lexical restrictions. The event view and features of the elaboration phase are described in Section 2. Section 3 presents the Kamang language and data, and Section 4 summarizes the findings.

2 The event view and viewpoint shift

In Croft’s (1994) event view, an event has three phases: causebecomestate. Both causal and temporal chains are evoked by the left-to-right arrows: from actor to undergoer, and from initiation to completion (DeLancey 1982; Fauconnier 2013). Each phase includes the phases to the right of it. Hence, a construction may encode all three phases (“causative”, e.g., the rock broke the window), the two phases become and state (“inchoative”; the window broke) or the state phase only (“stative”; the window is broken; Croft 1994: 93). Causatives are generally transitive and include both A and P arguments, while inchoatives and statives are generally intransitive; however, any phase can be realized as an intransitive with an S argument (Croft 1994: 109).

Languages provide various means for encoding different phases and for shifting or narrowing the viewpoint. Common grammatical systems for this purpose are voice and diathesis: the categories that align with each viewpoint are given in Table 1. For instance, voice alternation in English shifts from the standard “causative” viewpoint encompassing all three phases in an active clause – the rock broke the window – to focus on the middle and end phases in a passive construction: the window was broken (by the rock). In this alternation, the cause phase is semantically greyed out, which is realized syntactically by encoding the actor in a non-obligatory prepositional phrase. From a holistic view of the whole event, the passive shifts focus to the end of the causal chain, the undergoer, which becomes the subject.

Table 1:

Event phases and associated categories (after Croft 1994: 113).

Event Causative Inchoative Stative
Event phases causebecomestate becomestate state
Argument role A/S P/S P/S
Semantic role Actor Undergoer Undergoer
Voice Active Middle Passive
Diathesis Causative Inchoative Resultative

Though the event view is tripartite, binary alternations are frequent. DAI systems of S arguments (split-S, semantic alignment; see, e.g., Wichmann 2008) are one example, which Croft aligns with the event view as shown in (2). That is, “active” intransitive predicates align under cause (i.e., include all three event phases), while “stative” predicates align under become and state. However, many languages, such as Dobel (Austronesian, Indonesia), group become with cause rather than with state, called a dynamic-stative split (Klamer 2008: 248). In Dobel, patientive postclitics index S on stative predicates (e.g., ŋeŋan=ni [heavy=3sg.U.an] ‘he is heavy’; Hughes 2000: 148), while agentive proclitics index S on almost all other intransitive predicates. This includes those without initiators, such as inchoative ŋ=kwoy [2sg.A=die] ‘you die’ (Hughes 2000: 137). Given the potential confusion between “active” and “dynamic”, Croft’s split (cause vs. become-state) will be referred to here as an initiation-elaboration split.

(2)

An initiation-elaboration split is described for Pilagá (Guaicuruan,[4] South America), which indexes S/A arguments with either “set A” or “set B” affixes (Vidal 2008).[5] For some verbs, affix alternation is claimed to express a viewpoint split: events viewed from the start (causative) encode S/A with set A affixes, while events viewed at the moment of change (inchoative) or their endpoint (resultant state) encode S/A with set B affixes (Vidal 2008: 429). In (3), the set A prefix indexes the actor of an event viewed from the start, while a set B prefix indexes an undergoer S argument of an event with no cause phase. Set B affixes are also associated with greater affectedness (Vidal 2008: 430); this is a common property of an endpoint view, as detailed below.[6]

(3) Pilagá
a.
ya-čemat-iyi
setA.3-roast-dir
‘They roast (fish).’
(Vidal 2001: 174)
b.
na-čemat-aʕak
setB.3-roast-Val1
‘It is roasted.’
(Vidal 2001: 174)

Constructions that encode or shift focus to the elaboration phase are associated with a number of properties, of which three are relevant here. First, they may express telic or atelic aspect (“change of state” and/or “unbounded processes and activities”, respectively; Arkadiev 2008: 105–106), since the elaboration phase encompasses both inchoatives (change of state: telic) and statives (atelic). For instance, resultatives “express a state implying a previous event” (Nedjalkov and Jaxontov 1988: 6); that is, that a change of state has (just) taken place.

Another aspectual category associated with the elaboration phase is the completive: “to do something thoroughly and to completion” (Bybee et al. 1994: 18). Fauconnier (2013) links completive aspect to endpoint focus, which entails centring the undergoer and thus creating a stronger association with properties of a prototypical transitive P argument, such as greater affectedness or low volitionality (Fauconnier 2013; cf. Hopper and Thompson 1980). This is the second property: the association between the undergoer argument in an elaboration alternant and features of a prototypical P argument.

Third, initiation-elaboration alternations are often subject to lexical restrictions and lexicalization. That is, only some verbal bases alternate and alternants may have idiosyncratic meanings. This is attested, for instance, in cross-linguistic investigations of resultative and middle constructions (Beavers 2012; Kemmer 1994; Nedjalkov and Jaxontov 1988). For verbal bases that do alternate, the same semantic classes recur in the literature. These include translational motion verbs, illustrated in (4), body action or body posture verbs, in (5), and emotion/cognition predicates, in (6) (see Bybee et al. 1994; Kemmer 1993: 147; Nedjalkov and Jaxontov 1988; Sil’nickij 1988). Note the lexical restrictions illustrated in (5), where semantically similar verbs sich hinsetzen ‘sit down’ and aufstehen ‘stand up’ admit different construction types, and in (6), where these verbs do not alternate with an unaffixed variant.

(4)
a.
Spanish (Indo-European)
Middle ir-se ‘go away’, caer-se ‘fall’
Active ir ‘go’, caer ‘fall’
(Kemmer 1994: 186)
b.
English (Indo-European)
Resultative he is gone
Active he went
(cf. Bybee et al. 1994: 54)
(5)
German (Indo-European)
Middle sich hinsetzen ‘sit down’, sich hinlegen ‘lie down’
Active (*sich) aufstehen ‘stand up’
(Kemmer 1994: 185)
(6)
Fang (Bantu): middle constructions with -a (“associative”, see also Dom et al. 2016)
-yá-á ‘to get angry’ (*-yá)
-zob-a ‘to regret; to be embarrassed by unpleasant news, be sad’ (*-zob)
-sím-á ‘to remember, think’ (*-sím)
(Bostoen and Nzang-Bie 2010: 1279)

Emotion/cognition predicates illustrate that the argument has more features of a prototypical P: arguments of such predicates “naturally exhibit low control, will, or instigation” (Vidal 2008: 428). They are also a common locus for differential S/A marking cross-linguistically (Nichols 2008: 129). Also, in Pilagá, there is pervasive lexicalization in mental state verbs (Vidal 2008: 430). Regardless of semantic class, alternation frequently results in lexicalized – that is, idiosyncratic – meaning change. For example, in Russian the resultative form rassejan ‘scattered’ has an unpredictable lexicalized meaning ‘absent-minded’ (Nedjalkov and Jaxontov 1988: 61).

These three properties of elaboration-view constructions – mixed telicity, greater affectedness, and lexical restrictions or lexicalization – are shown below to characterize the Kamang /e/-series alternation. I therefore analyse it as an initiation-elaboration split, which nevertheless differs from the Pilagá split in important ways.

3 Case study: Kamang /e/-series

3.1 Language and data

Kamang is a severely endangered Alor-Pantar (Papuan/non-Austronesian) language spoken by around 6,000 people in Indonesia (Schapper 2014: 286–287). The language possesses up to seven prefix paradigms that index the S or P argument. Table 2 gives three of the most common prefix paradigms – the /a/-series, /o/-series, and /e/-series[7] – plus the dative and self-benefactive paradigms, which are phonologically similar to the /e/-series.

Table 2:

Selected Kamang prefix paradigms (Schapper 2014: 322, 331).

/a/-series /o/-series /e/-series Dative Self-benefactive
1sg na- no- ne- nee- ne′-
2sg a- o- e- ee- e′-
3 ga- wo- ge- gee- ge′-
cmn a ta- to- te- tee- te′-
1pl.excl ni- nio- ni- nii- ni′-
1pl.incl si- sio- si- sii- si′-
2pl i- io- i- ii- i′-
  1. aThe “common” prefixes are used for reciprocal and generic reference (Schapper 2014: 314).

Around a third of verbal bases obligatorily bear an index from a single prefix series; the rest are either never prefixed, or participate in DAI. Various alternation types are attested – between prefixation and zero-marking, or between prefixation with indexes from different series (see Fedden et al. 2013, 2014; Schapper 2014; Walker et al. 2023). This study focuses on the alternation presented in (1) above and in (7) below; namely, between /e/-series prefixation and zero-marking.

(7)
a.
dum=a nih
child=spec sit
‘The child sits.’
(Schapper 2014: 326)
b.
dum=a ge-nih
child=spec 3./e/-sit
‘The child sat/lived (there for a long time).’
(Schapper 2014: 326)

The /e/-series alternation has been described as indicating greater patientivity of S on motion and posture verbs (Schapper 2014). This has two typical interpretations: greater affectedness and/or greater duration. In (1a), zero-marking is used for a less-affected S argument whereas, in (1b), an /e/-series prefix “indicates the dog’s running away was caused by an external event, such as someone kicking at the dog” (more affected; Schapper 2014: 255). In terms of duration, the prefixed form indicates that a motion or posture was held for a long time. In (7b), nih ‘sit’ acquires the “somewhat lexicalized meaning … ‘live, reside’” (Schapper 2014: 326) with an /e/-series prefix.

The present study aims to provide a more detailed picture of the /e/-series alternation through a qualitative analysis of examples attested in a corpus of spoken Kamang. The corpus includes monologic narratives, conversations, and structured (i.e., using stimuli) and unstructured elicitation recorded by Antoinette Schapper and George Saad between 2010 and 2020.[8] Part of this corpus – annotated for grammatical relations and animacy with the GRAID schema (Haig and Schnell 2014, 2022) – is publicly available (Schapper et al. forthcoming). Target verbal bases were those recorded in the lexicon or the annotated sub-corpus as alternating between zero-marking and /e/-series prefixation. Utterances containing these verbs were then extracted for comparison.

There are two points of note. First, plural forms of the /e/-series are homophonous with /a/-series plurals, which may lead to ambiguity. To confirm alternation with the /e/-series, verbal bases needed to be attested at least once with an unambiguous singular prefix (all of them were). Second, the /e/-series is phonologically similar to the dative and self-benefactive series. The phonetic differences between the three series in natural speech can be minimal if not imperceptible (Al-Gariri 2022). Since the dative prefix occurs only very rarely (Fedden and Brown 2017: 424–426; Schapper 2014: 324), ambiguity with the /e/-series is unlikely. However, the self-benefactive is frequent in discourse. To minimize the chance of including self-benefactives, I selected examples that (i) previous researchers had glossed as /e/-series and (ii) had accessible recordings in which no glottal stop was audible. Some examples found only in elicitation data could be discarded when the speaker explicitly mentioned the glottal stop (by referring to the glottal stop grapheme <′>). Consequently, only the clearest instances of /e/-series prefixation were included in the study, totalling 123 tokens for 21 lexical verbs. Some verbs occur much more frequently with zero: here, 20 examples were selected for comparison, prioritizing good quality recordings and clear surrounding context. Given the modest sample size (a common challenge in research on endangered languages) and the exclusion of unclear cases, future research will likely find that additional verbal bases can alternate.

Several features of Kamang grammar are relevant to this study (for a full description, see Schapper 2014). There are several ways to mark aspect: non-obligatory perfective and imperfective suffixes, stem suppletion, and aspectual serialization constructions. Aspectual serialization involves a sequence of two verbs, the second of which expresses aspect. Completive aspect is marked by lai ‘finish’ and continuous aspect by dii ‘lie’. The latter is illustrated in (8).

(8)
an we dii me dii=a
thus go lie come lie=spec
‘(they) continued to come and go like this …’
(Schapper fieldnotes, spontaneous)

Also relevant here is the resultative construction: “one verb encodes the result of the event denoted by another verb. Result verbs are intransitive and occur as the final verb in the serialization” (Schapper 2014: 345). As shown in (9), the resultant state of the S argument is expressed when the previous verb is intransitive (‘fall down’).

(9)
mooi bong nok kok silang dii
banana tree one fall.down descend lie
‘A banana tree came falling down.’
(Schapper 2014: 345)

If the previous verb is transitive, the final verb expresses the resultant state or position of P (Schapper 2014: 346). In (10), the P argument (semantically a location) of tal ‘make fence’ is kadii ‘house’, which in turn is the S argument of the resultant state beela ‘surround(ed)’.

(10)
Lukas kadii go-tal beela
Lukas house 3./o/-make.fence surround
‘Lukas puts up a fence around the house.’
(Schapper 2014: 346)

In other verb sequences, the final verb expresses a property of the A argument of the preceding transitive verb. While the final verb in the resultative construction in (10) expresses a property of P, the final verb in an otherwise identical construction in (11) expresses a property of A. That is, ‘a man’ is standing, not the child he is holding.[9] There is currently no evidence that prefixation behaviour differs according to whether the final verb expresses the resultant state of the S, A, or P of the previous verb, but this remains a question for future research.

(11)
lami saak nok ge-dum ga-buh latsi
male old one 3./e/-child 3./a/-cradle stand
‘A man stands holding his child.’[10]
(Schapper fieldnotes, elicited)

Finally, Kamang does not require any arguments in the clause to be overt (e.g., in (8) the S argument ‘they’ is unexpressed). Independent arguments (pronouns or lexical noun phrases) are permitted to co-occur in a clause with the prefix that indexes them, although co-occurrence is generally dispreferred (see Walker et al. 2023).

3.2 Kamang /e/-series alternation

Verbal bases that participate in the /e/-series alternation are listed in Table 3 along with their frequencies in each construction. The translation is of the unprefixed lexical base; where prefixation leads to an idiosyncratic meaning change – a common feature of elaboration constructions – this is given in the rightmost column. Note that unprefixed posture verbs can have both telic ‘stand up, sit down’ and atelic readings ‘stand, sit’ (cf. Schapper 2014: 326).

Table 3:

Verbal bases exhibiting differential /e/-series indexing. Parentheses indicate alternations with scant data; “>20” indicates that 20 examples were selected from a larger total (see Section 3.1).

Unprefixed Prefixed with /e/-series
Verb base Meaning Count Idiosyncratic meaning Count
Posture verbs

nih sit (down) >20 live; reside 7
dii lie (down) >20 5
latsi stand (up) >20 4
(laka) hang (up) n/aa be naked n/a

Motion verbs

sila/silang descend >20 17
te go up straight 19 7
yaangme (yaangma) come down (pfv) 16 1
yaa go; reach >20 go home 22
tak run 20 run away 12

Other verbs

awila′ fill (intr.) 4 1
fanee work 15 strike 17
paisan/paisang be bright; shine 2 explanation/explain 2
(fun) touch 5 contribution/contribute 1
taa sleep >20 1
beta bump into; barge 2 3
baila′ buy 3 2
balkei sell 2 1
sika finish 3 4
mitan complete 7 understand 1

(Emotion/cognition verbs)

(maringki) startle(d) 1 3
(biee) afraid 1 9
(mai) hear 3 3

Total 223 123
  1. aNo clear example of alternation was available for the present study; alternation of laka is reported by Fedden and colleagues (2014: 63).

The items in parentheses in Table 3 were discounted due to insufficient data in the corpus to fully substantiate the alternation (e.g., due to difficulties in ruling out a transitivity difference). Three of these are emotion/cognition verbs (maringki ‘startle’, biee ‘afraid’, and mai ‘hear’): as mentioned, cross-linguistically such verbs frequently participate in DAI of S, and in elaboration constructions like resultatives and middles. Also relevant, then, is the idiosyncratic meaning shift from mitan ‘complete’ to the emotion/cognition verb ‘understand’ when prefixed.

Aside from emotion/cognition verbs, Table 3 shows the groups of motion and posture verbs, which Schapper (2014) previously identified as participating in /e/-series alternation. These semantic groups are also common in elaboration constructions; nevertheless, there is evidence of lexical restrictions even within a semantic class. For motion verbs, tak ‘run’ alternates, but semantically similar verbal bases like maa ‘walk’, loo ‘walk’, and lila ‘fly’ are not (yet) attested with /e/-series prefixes. For posture verbs, lexicalization is manifested in the idiosyncratic meaning difference between unprefixed nih ‘sit’ and ‘live; reside’ when prefixed. The final group in Table 3 is labelled “other” due to its heterogeneity. It includes verbal bases that seem inherently intransitive and non-volitional (‘sleep’) and inherently transitive and volitional (‘buy’, ‘sell’), as well as bases that appear to be inherently durative (‘work’, ‘be bright; shine’) or inherently punctual (‘bump into’, ‘finish’).

In terms of systematic meaning differences between the alternants, increased duration and affectedness is only part of the story. In (12a), the S argument has been in position for a long time, yet latsi ‘stand’ is zero-marked. Conversely, in the prefixed ge-latsi in (12b), the S argument goes volitionally to stand ‘up on top’ and does not remain there long. It is difficult to impute a “greater affectedness” reading here.

(12)
a.
waima ang tanaa ge-kaang tung mane mi latsi-ma
drum dem still 3./e/-be.present up.there.straight village in stand-pfv
‘the drum is still present, stood up there in the village’
(Schapper fieldnotes, spontaneous)
b.
yaa=nte woi taa ge-latsi
go=but stone top 3./e/-stand
‘… and (he) went and stood up on top’
[kamang_frog]

While greater affectedness is often a property of arguments indexed with the /e/-series, it is one of several properties associated with elaboration constructions, including completion, inchoativity, resultativity, and features of middles. As the following examples illustrate, the /e/-series alternation is best analysed as a viewpoint split between zero-marked verbs that express events holistically – either with all three phases, or without internal phasal partitions – and /e/-series-marked verbs that shift focus to the elaboration phase.

Example (13) shows two almost identical resultative serializations, the final verb dii ‘lie’ expressing the position of the P argument of the preceding verb kuh ‘cover’: botil ‘bottle’ in (13a) and sukuu ‘hole’ in (13b).

(13)
a.
botil ge-tangpi me botil wo-kuh dii
bottle 3./e/-cap take bottle 3./o/-cover lie
‘The lid closes the bottle.’
(Schapper fieldnotes, elicited)
b.
woi baai nok me see sukuu ang wo-kuh ge-dii
stone great one take arrive.ipfv hole dem 3./o/-cover 3./e/-lie
‘(She) took the big stone and blocked the hole.’
[kamang_aribei]

Unprefixed dii in (13a) is best analysed as a state proper with no internal phases (the utterance was elicited to describe a static picture). In (13b), the utterance is part of a narrative expressing a series of dynamic events. Here, the prefixed verb is associated with a dynamic event with internal phases. This includes initiation (the initiator is unexpressed but clear from the context), but the focus is on the elaboration phase: ge-dii expresses a resultant state, which implies an inchoative phase.

Example (14) demonstrates another resultative serialization. Here the alternation applies not to a motion or posture verb, but to awila′ ‘fill’. In elicitation, the speaker described the meaning difference as a matter of degree; that is, the meaning is neutral ‘fill’ when zero-marked and completive – ‘completely full’ – with an /e/-series prefix. As described above, completive aspect is associated with the end phase of an event, which also implies a greater degree of affectedness of the indexed argument.

(14)
a.
ili me bak ite awila′
water take tub fill fill
‘(I) fill the tub with water.’
(Saad fieldnotes, elicited)
b.
na ili me bak ite ye-awila′
1sg.agt water take tub fill 3./e/-fill
‘I fill the tub with water (completely full).’
(Saad fieldnotes, elicited)

In (15), silang ‘descend’ alternates between a zero-marked variant with habitual reading in (15a), reflected in the translation ‘used to go down’, and a single punctual event denoted by the prefixed variant in (15b). In a habitual reading, the whole event from initiation to completion is relevant since it repeats in its entirety. This contrasts with the prefixed variant, which denotes a single punctual event with internal phases, of which the elaboration phase is in focus: what is important is that the woman has descended, resulting in her being in the hole. This pair of utterances – the only clear example involving habituality identified so far in the corpus – might seem contrary to the previous description of the /e/-series as indicating long duration; however, as mentioned, elaboration phases have telic and atelic characteristics due to including both inchoative and stative phases.

(15)
a.
anmante ingkoo sukuu=bo akmi mane mibo ga wo-oi silang=bee
but.then earlier hole=rel here village in=rel 3.agt 3./o/-towards descend=also
‘but then earlier there was a hole in the village here that she used to go down into …’
[kamang_aribei]
b.
male saak=a ilai-sa yaa me sukuu wo-oi ge-silang=a
female old=spec look-cmp go come hole 3./o/-towards 3./e/-descend=spec
‘[she lay there hiding and] saw the old women go and descend into a hole’
[kamang_aribei]

As well as intransitive verbs, several alternations involve transitive verbal bases, such as baila ‘buy’, balkei ‘sell’, and beta ‘bump into’. The last-mentioned is a naturally reciprocal event associated with middle semantics (Kemmer 1993: 102–108). The prefix on beta ‘bump into’ indicates greater affectedness of P: in elicitation, a speaker explained that unprefixed beta means ‘bump into’ or ‘barge’, while the prefixed version means ‘push over’. This follows from greater focus on the elaboration phase of the event, which features P. The corpus yields one alternation pair, shown in (16), which describe the same stimulus (Fedden et al. 2010b), in which man A bumps into man B, causing the latter to step backward. The two speakers frame the event differently: in (16a), man A merely bumps an arm, whereas in (16b), beta is prefixed when it indexes man B, who is more affected by the push than an arm.

(16)
a.
lami saak nok sue-h=bo ga-tang=a beta
male old one arrive.ipfv-purp=lnk 3./a/-hand.arm=spec bump.into
‘An old man comes and hits his arm.’
(Schapper fieldnotes, elicited)
b.
lami saak nok sue ge-nok ge-beta
male old one arrive.ipfv 3./e/-friend 3./e/-bump.into
‘An old man comes and pushes away his friend.’
(Schapper fieldnotes, elicited)

The difference in meaning between prefixed and zero-marked baila ‘buy’ and balkei ‘sell’ is unclear. There are two possible interpretations, both of which fit an elaboration-focus analysis. First, the prefixed variant in (17b) may indicate greater focus on P than A. Indeed, the A argument is not mentioned when the prefix is present, while in the zero-marked variant in (17a), the speaker emphasizes the agentivity of A by using the agentive pronoun.[11]

(17)
a.
ga nuaanana baila lai-si=bo
3.agt possessions buy finish-ipfv=lnk
‘they finished/were finishing buying the things …’
[kamang_aribei]
b.
nuaanana ge-baila lai ge-balkei lai-sa
possessions 3./e/-buy finish 3./e/-sell finish-cmp
‘when (they) had bought and sold their things …’
[kamang_aribei]

The second analysis of (17) is a more telic interpretation of the prefixed verb: a resultative sense in which (all) the goods have entered into the resultant state of ‘bought’ or a completive sense in which the purchase is complete (compared to ‘being in the process of buying’). The latter interpretation is supported by the co-occurrence of the imperfective suffix -si with unprefixed baila in (17a).

4 Discussion and conclusion

Previous description of DAI with /e/-series prefixes stated that the /e/-series indexes more-patientive S arguments of motion and posture verbs. This paper extends this description in three ways: (i) not all motion verbs alternate, and a heterogeneous group of other verbal bases does alternate, potentially including emotion/cognition verbs; (ii) both S and P arguments can be indexed; and (iii) the alternation is better characterized in terms of elaboration focus. The elaboration phases of an event are the become and state phases, and entail a cluster of properties including features of a prototypical P argument (greater affectedness, low volitionality), completive aspect, resultativity, and features of middles. These properties – evoked individually or in combination – were demonstrated by several alternations, alongside an additional meaning difference whereby the prefixed verb denoted a punctual event and the zero-marked verb had a habitual reading.

Elaboration constructions such as resultatives and middles have a cross-linguistic tendency towards lexical restriction. This was already found for DAI in Kamang in general (Fedden et al. 2013, 2014; Walker et al. 2023), and was demonstrated here for /e/-series DAI: the number of verbal bases that alternate is quite low, not all verbal bases in a particular semantic class can alternate, and alternation often leads to lexicalized meaning differences.

The type of DAI alternation in which elaboration phases receive special marking was termed an initiation-elaboration split, exemplified by Pilagá in Section 2 and shown in the first line of (18). Kamang’s /e/-series alternation differs from an initiation-elaboration split proper in that it is asymmetrical in two ways. First, it is asymmetrical in the formal sense of alternating between a zero and a non-zero marker, rather than between two different affixes (see Witzlack-Makarevich and Seržant 2018: 23–25). Since Kamang does not index A arguments, the formal asymmetry reflects the fact that indexing the initiator is not an option to signal initiation focus. Second, it is asymmetrical because a zero-marked verb expresses a holistic view of the event, either with all three phases, or with no internal phases. This is schematized in (18).

(18)

The ambiguity of zero-marking is unsurprising, given that zero in Kamang alternates not only with the /e/-series, but also with other prefix paradigms (as well as being the only option available for some verbal bases). That is, this is not a binary alternation, but a multidimensional one in which the default meanings of unprefixed verbs can be manipulated. Where other languages use voice or highly developed aspect systems, Kamang speakers have differential /e/-series indexing alongside predominantly non-obligatory aspect marking and resultative serialization to manipulate the semantics of a predicate. Though highly dependent on individual verbal bases, in general an /e/-series index will shift focus to the elaboration phase of an event. Further research – utilizing additional methodologies such as controlled experiments – is needed to confirm the description advanced here, which is based on the qualitative analysis of a small number of tokens. This should also test the acceptability of /e/-series prefixes on additional verbal bases and the degree of optionality on the verbal bases already identified, investigate the compatibility of DAI with aspectual marking, and compare the /e/-series alternation to the /o/-series alternation, which has also been described as indicating greater affectedness (Schapper 2014: 325).


Corresponding author: Katherine Walker, 168063 Amsterdam Center for Language and Communication, University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam, The Netherlands, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: VI.Vidi.195.008

Acknowledgements

The research and part of the data collection for this paper were funded by the NWO (Dutch Research Council) within the project VI.Vidi.195.008 at the University of Amsterdam. I am grateful to Antoinette Schapper for sharing her Kamang corpus, and to George Saad for the Kamang data he collected. My thanks also go to those who provided helpful feedback and suggestions on earlier versions of this paper: Kees Hengeveld, Eva van Lier, Sonja Riesberg, and two anonymous reviewers.

References

Al-Gariri, Husam. 2022. Vowel length contrast in prefixes of Kamang (dialectal variation). Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam.Search in Google Scholar

Arkadiev, Peter. 2008. Thematic roles, event structure, and argument encoding in semantically aligned languages. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 101–117. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199238385.003.0004Search in Google Scholar

Beavers, John. 2012. Resultative constructions. In Robert I. Binnick (ed.), The Oxford handbook of tense and aspect, 908–935. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195381979.013.0032Search in Google Scholar

Bostoen, Koen & Yolande Nzang-Bie. 2010. On how “middle” plus “associative/reciprocal” became “passive” in the Bantu A70 languages. Linguistics 48(6). 1255–1307. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2010.041.Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan, Revere Perkins & William Pagliuca. 1994. The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Corbett, Greville. 2006. Agreement. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Croft, William A. 1994. Voice: Beyond control and affectedness. In Barbara A. Fox & Paul J. Hopper (eds.), Voice: Form and function (Typological Studies in Language 27), 89–117. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.27.06croSearch in Google Scholar

DeLancey, Scott. 1982. Aspect, transitivity, and viewpoint. In Paul J. Hopper (ed.), Tense-aspect: Between semantics and pragmatics (Typological Studies in Language 1), 167–183. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.1.12delSearch in Google Scholar

Dom, Sebastian, Leonid Kulikov & Koen Bostoen. 2016. The middle as a voice category in Bantu: Setting the stage for further research. Lingua Posnaniensis 58(2). 129–149. https://doi.org/10.1515/linpo-2016-0012.Search in Google Scholar

Fauconnier, Stefanie. 2013. Completives as markers of non-volitionality. Folia Linguistica 47(1). 35–53. https://doi.org/10.1515/flin.2013.003.Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian. 2019. To agree or not to agree? A typology of sporadic agreement. In Matthew Baerman, Oliver Bond & Andrew Hippisley (eds.), Morphological perspectives: Papers in honour of Greville G. Corbett, 303–326. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.10.1515/9781474446020-015Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian & Dunstan Brown. 2017. Participant marking: Corpus study and video elicitation. In Marian Klamer (ed.), Alor-Pantar languages: History and typology, 2nd edn. (Studies in Diversity Linguistics), 403–446. Berlin: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian, Dunstan Brown & Greville G. Corbett. 2010a. Conditions on pronominal marking: Video P15 – hold person. Guildford: University of Surrey.Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian, Dunstan Brown & Greville G. Corbett. 2010b. Conditions on pronominal marking: Video C13 – bump into person. Guildford: University of Surrey.Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian, Dunstan Brown, Greville Corbett, Gary Holton, Marian Klamer, Laura C. Robinson & Antoinette Schapper. 2013. Conditions on pronominal marking in the Alor-Pantar languages. Linguistics 51(1). 33–74. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2013-0002.Search in Google Scholar

Fedden, Sebastian, Dunstan Brown, František Kratochvíl, Laura C. Robinson & Antoinette Schapper. 2014. Variation in pronominal indexing: Lexical stipulation vs. referential properties in Alor-Pantar languages. Studies in Language 38(1). 44–79. https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.38.1.02fed.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Haig, Geoffrey & Stefan Schnell. 2014. Annotations using GRAID (Grammatical Relations and Animacy in Discourse), version 7.0. https://multicast.aspra.uni-bamberg.de/ (accessed 22 December 2020).Search in Google Scholar

Haig, Geoffrey & Stefan Schnell (eds.). 2022. Multi-CAST: Multilingual corpus of annotated spoken texts, version 2211 [website]. Bamberg: University of Bamberg. https://multicast.aspra.uni-bamberg.de (accessed 15 January 2021).Search in Google Scholar

Hammarström, Harald, Robert Forkel, Martin Haspelmath & Sebastian Bank (eds.). 2022. Glottolog 4.5 [website]. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Search in Google Scholar

Hopper, Paul J. & Sandra A. Thompson. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language 56(2). 251–299. https://doi.org/10.2307/413757.Search in Google Scholar

Hughes, Jock. 2000. The morphology of Dobel, Aru, with special reference to reduplication. In Charles E. Grimes (ed.), Spices from the East: Papers in languages of eastern Indonesia, 131–180. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2011. Towards a typological study of differential object marking and differential object indexation. Pavia: University of Pavia PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Kemmer, Suzanne. 1993. The middle voice. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.23Search in Google Scholar

Kemmer, Suzanne. 1994. Middle voice, transitivity, and the elaboration of events. In Barbara A. Fox & Paul J. Hopper (eds.), Voice: Form and function (Typological Studies in Language 27), 179–230. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.27.09kemSearch in Google Scholar

Klamer, Marian. 2008. The semantics of semantic alignment in eastern Indonesia. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 221–251. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199238385.003.0009Search in Google Scholar

Kratochvíl, František. 2014. Differential argument realization in Abui. Linguistics 52(2). 543–602. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2013-0072.Search in Google Scholar

Lazard, Gilbert. 2005. What are we typologists doing? In David S. Rood, Adam Hodges & Zygmunt Frajzyngier (eds.), Linguistic diversity and language theories (Studies in Language Companion Series), 1–23. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.72.02lazSearch in Google Scholar

Nedjalkov, Vladimir P. & Sergej Je. Jaxontov. 1988. The typology of resultative constructions. In Vladimir P. Nedjalkov (ed.), Typology of resultative constructions (Typological Studies in Language 12; translated by Bernard Comrie), 3–62. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.12.06nedSearch in Google Scholar

Nichols, Johanna. 2008. Why are stative-active languages rare in Eurasia? A typological perspective on split-subject marking. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 121–139. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199238385.003.0005Search in Google Scholar

Schapper, Antoinette. 2014. Kamang. In Antoinette Schapper (ed.), The Papuan languages of Timor, Alor and Pantar, vol. 1, Sketch grammars (Pacific Linguistics 644), 285–350. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9781614515241.285Search in Google Scholar

Schapper, Antoinette, Saad George, Katherine Walker & Clemens Mayer. Forthcoming. Multi-CAST Kamang. In Geoffrey Haig & Stefan Schnell (eds.), Multi-CAST: Multilingual corpus of annotated spoken texts, version 2211 [website]. Bamberg: University of Bamberg.Search in Google Scholar

Sil’nickij, George G. 1988. The structure of verbal meaning and the resultative. In Vladimir P. Nedjalkov & Bernard Comrie (eds.), Typology of resultative constructions (Typological Studies in Language 12), 87–100. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.12.08silSearch in Google Scholar

Vidal, Alejandra. 2001. Pilagá grammar (Guaykuruan family, Argentina). Eugene: University of Oregon PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Vidal, Alejandra. 2008. Affectedness and viewpoint in Pilagá (Guaykuruan): A semantically aligned case-marking system. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 412–430. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199238385.003.0017Search in Google Scholar

Walker, Katherine, Pegah Faghiri & Eva van Lier. 2023. Differential argument indexing in Kamang. Studies in Language. https://doi.org/10.1075/sl.21077.wal.Search in Google Scholar

Wichmann, Søren. 2008. The study of semantic alignment: Retrospect and state of the art. In Mark Donohue & Søren Wichmann (eds.), The typology of semantic alignment, 1–23. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199238385.003.0001Search in Google Scholar

Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena & Ilja A. Seržant. 2018. Differential argument marking: Patterns of variation. In Ilja A Seržant & Alena Witzlack-Makarevich (eds.), Diachrony of differential argument marking, 1–40. Berlin: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2023-03-25
Accepted: 2023-04-03
Published Online: 2024-01-01

© 2023 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 3.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingvan-2023-0052/html
Scroll to top button