Skip to content
Licensed Unlicensed Requires Authentication Published online by De Gruyter Mouton December 21, 2022

Stylistic alignment in natural conversation involving second language speakers

  • YeonJoo Jung

    YeonJoo Jung is an assistant professor in the Department of English Education at Pusan National University in South Korea. Her interests include the application of experimental techniques from psychology to second language (L2) processing and acquisition research as well as the use of natural language processing tools in assessing linguistic features of written and spoken data produced by L2 learners.

    EMAIL logo
    and Scott Crossley

    Scott Crossley is a Professor at Georgia State University. His interests include the application and development of natural language processing tools in educational technology. He has published articles on the use of natural language processing tool to examine lexical acquisition, writing proficiency, reading comprehension, discourse processing, language assessment, and automatic feedback in intelligent tutoring systems.

Abstract

To date, a growing body of second language (L2) research has investigated linguistic alignment as a pedagogical intervention, focusing on L2 learners’ alignment behaviors in task-based interactions (e.g., Jung, YeonJoo, YouJin Kim & John Murphy. 2017. The role of task repetition in learning word-stress patterns through auditory priming tasks. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 39(2). 319–346; Kim, YouJin, YeonJoo Jung & Stephen Skalicky. 2019. Linguistic alignment, learner characteristics, and the production of stranded prepositions in relative clauses: Comparing FTF and SCMC contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 41(5). 937–969). Linguistic alignment refers to a tendency where one speaker’s utterances align with particular language features of those of the other speaker in dialogue. The current study investigated how L2 speakers’ alignment behaviors differ in natural dialogues between L2-L1 and L2-L2 dyads in terms of language style (i.e., stylistic alignment) and the role of non-linguistic factors in the occurrence of stylistic alignment. The study analyzed a corpus of 360 texts using a computational tool. Results showed that stylistic alignment occurred to a greater extent in the L2-L2 dyad than in the L2-L1 dyad with respect to the word range, word frequency, word imageability, and proportion of bigrams proportion produced by the interlocutors. Furthermore, findings demonstrated the degree of stylistic alignment on each of the four selected lexical features was affected by numerous factors including age, group membership, nonnative speaker status, familiarity between interlocutors, and linguistic distance between L1 and L2. The effect of each factor on stylistic alignment in conversation is discussed in detail.


Corresponding author: YeonJoo Jung, Department of English Education, Pusan National University, Busandaehak-ro 63beon-gil 2, Geumjeong-gu, Busan 46241, Republic of Korea, E-mail:

About the authors

YeonJoo Jung

YeonJoo Jung is an assistant professor in the Department of English Education at Pusan National University in South Korea. Her interests include the application of experimental techniques from psychology to second language (L2) processing and acquisition research as well as the use of natural language processing tools in assessing linguistic features of written and spoken data produced by L2 learners.

Scott Crossley

Scott Crossley is a Professor at Georgia State University. His interests include the application and development of natural language processing tools in educational technology. He has published articles on the use of natural language processing tool to examine lexical acquisition, writing proficiency, reading comprehension, discourse processing, language assessment, and automatic feedback in intelligent tutoring systems.

References

Adamson, Hugh Douglas & Vera M. Regan. 1991. The acquisition of community speech norms by Asian immigrants learning English as a second language: A preliminary study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 13(1). 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100009694.Search in Google Scholar

Argamon, Shlomo, Whitelaw Casey, Paul Chase, Sobhan Raj Hota, Navendu Garg & Shlomo Levitan. 2007. Stylistic text classification using functional lexical features. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 58(6). 802–822. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.20553.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Rolf Harald, Doug J. Davidson & Douglas M. Bates. 2008. Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4). 390–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005.Search in Google Scholar

Balcetis, Emily E. & Rick Dale. 2005. An exploration of social modulation of syntactic priming. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society, vol. 27. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/0kn388rj (accessed 17 December 2019).Search in Google Scholar

Bates, Douglas M., Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker & Steven Walker. 2015. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4_. R package version 1.1-9. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4 (accessed 5 September 2019).Search in Google Scholar

Bayram, Burcu & Vivian P. Ta. 2018. Diplomatic chameleons: Language style matching and agreement in international diplomatic negotiations. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research 12(1). 23–40. https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12142.Search in Google Scholar

Beebe, Leslie. 1985. Input: Choosing the right stuff. In Susan M. Gass & Carolyn G. Madden (eds.), Input in second language acquisition, 104–144. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Search in Google Scholar

Bell, Allan. 1984. Language style as audience design. Language in Society 13(2). 145–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/s004740450001037x.Search in Google Scholar

Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Jamie Pearson & Janet F. McLean. 2010. Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics 42(9). 2355–2368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2009.12.012.Search in Google Scholar

Berger, Cynthia M., Scott A. Crossley & Kristopher Kyle. 2017. Using novel word context measures to predict human ratings of lexical proficiency. Journal of Educational Technology & Society 20(2). 201–212.Search in Google Scholar

Branigan, Holly P., Martin J. Pickering, Jamie Pearson & Ash Brown. 2011. The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers. Cognition 121(1). 41–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.011.Search in Google Scholar

Brennan, Susan E. & Herbert H. Clark. 1996. Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22(6). 1482–1493. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482.Search in Google Scholar

British National Corpus. 2007. version 3 (BNC XML ed.). Available at: http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Gordon D. A. 1984. A frequency count of 190, 000 words in the London-Lund Corpus of English Conversation. Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation and Computers 16(6). 502–532. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03200836.Search in Google Scholar

Brysbaert, Marc & Boris New. 2009. Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods 41(4). 977–990. https://doi.org/10.3758/brm.41.4.977.Search in Google Scholar

Brysbaert, Marc, Amy B. Warriner & Victor Kuperman. 2014. Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods 46(3). 904–911. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5.Search in Google Scholar

Cappella, Joseph N. 1996. Dynamic coordination of vocal and kinesic behavior in dyadic interaction: Methods, problems, and interpersonal outcomes. In James H. Watt & C. Arthur VanLear (eds.), Dynamic patterns in communication processes, 353–386. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Search in Google Scholar

Chartrand, Tanya L. & Amy N. Dalton. 2008. Mimicry: Its ubiquity, importance, and functionality. In Ezequiel Morsella, John A. Bargh & Peter M. Gollwitzer (eds.), Oxford handbook of human action, 458–483. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780195309980.003.0022Search in Google Scholar

Chiswick, Barry R. & Paul W. Miller. 2005. Linguistic distance: A quantitative measure of the distance between English and other languages. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 26(1). 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790710508668395.Search in Google Scholar

Clark, Herbert H. 1996. Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620539Search in Google Scholar

Costa, Albert, Martin J. Pickering & Antonella Sorace. 2008. Alignment in second language dialogue. Language & Cognitive Processes 23(4). 528–556. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960801920545.Search in Google Scholar

Crossely, Scott A., Tom Salsbury, Danielle S. McNamara & Scott Jarvis. 2011a. What is lexical proficiency? Some answers from computational models of speech data. TESOL Quarterly 45(1). 182–193. https://doi.org/10.5054/tq.2010.244019.Search in Google Scholar

Crossley, Amanda Clevinger & YouJin, Kim. 2014. The role of lexical properties and cohesive devices in text integration and their effect on human ratings of speaking proficiency. Language Assessment Quarterly 11(3). 250–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/15434303.2014.926905.Search in Google Scholar

Crossley, Kristopher Kyle & Thomas, Salsbury. 2016. A usage‐based investigation of L2 lexical acquisition: The role of input and output. The Modern Language Journal 100(3). 702–715. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12344Search in Google Scholar

Crossley, Scott A., Tom Salsbury, Danielle S. McNamara & Scott Jarvis. 2011b. Predicting lexical proficiency in language learner texts using computational indices. Language Testing 28(4). 561–580. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532210378031.Search in Google Scholar

Crossley, Scott, A. & Danielle, S. McNamara. 2012. Interlanguage talk: A computational analysis of non-native speakers’ lexical production and exposure. In Philip, M. & Chutima Boonthum-Denecke (eds.), Applied natural language processing: Identification, investigation and resolution, 425–437. Hershey, PA: IGI Global. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-60960-741-8.ch025.Search in Google Scholar

Crossley, Scott A., Stephen Skalicky, Mihai Dascalu, Danielle S. McNamara & Kristopher Kyle. 2017. Predicting text comprehension, processing, and familiarity in adult readers: New approaches to readability formulas. Discourse Processes 54(5–6). 340–359. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2017.1296264.Search in Google Scholar

Doyle, Gabriel, Yurovsky Dan & Michael C. Frank. 2016. A robust framework for estimating linguistic alignment in social media conversations. Proceedings of the International Conference on World Wide Web 25. 637–648. https://doi.org/10.1145/2872427.2883091.Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Rod. 1997. Second language acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Fusaroli, Riccardo, Bahador Bahrami, Karsten Olsen, Andreas Roepstorff, Geraint Rees, Chris Frith & Kristian Tylén. 2012. Coming to terms quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science 23(8). 931–939. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612436816.Search in Google Scholar

Fusaroli, Riccardo & Kristian Tylén. 2016. Investigating conversational dynamics: Interactive alignment, Interpersonal synergy, and collective task performance. Cognitive Science 40(1). 145–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12251.Search in Google Scholar

Fussell, Susan R. & Robert M. Krauss. 1992. Coordination of knowledge in communication: Effects of speakers’ assumptions about others’ knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62(3). 378–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.3.378.Search in Google Scholar

Garrod, Simon & Martin J. Pickering. 2007. Automaticity of language production in monologue and dialogue. In Antje S. Meyer, Linda R. Wheeldon & Andrea Krott (eds.), Automaticity and control in language processing, advances in behavioral brain science, 1–21. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Search in Google Scholar

Garrod, Simon & Martin J. Pickering. 2009. Joint action, interactive alignment, and dialog. Topics in Cognitive Science 1(2). 292–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01020.x.Search in Google Scholar

Gass, Susan. 2003. Input and interaction. In Catherine Doughty & Michael Long (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition, 224–255. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470756492.ch9Search in Google Scholar

Geeslin, Kimberly L. 2020. Variationist perspective(s) on interlocutor individual differences. In Laura Gurzynski-Weiss (ed.), Cross-theoretical explorations of interlocutors and their individual differences, 127–157. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/lllt.53.06geeSearch in Google Scholar

Giles, Howard, Justin Coupland & Nikolas Coupland. 1991. Accommodation theory: Communication, context, and consequence. In Howard Giles, Justin Coupland & Nikolas Coupland (eds.), Contexts of accommodation: Developments in applied sociolinguistics, 1–68. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511663673.001Search in Google Scholar

Gonzales, Amy L., Jeffrey T. Hancock & James W. Pennebaker. 2010. Language style matching as a predictor of social dynamics in small groups. Communication Research 37(1). 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650209351468.Search in Google Scholar

Guo, Liang, Scott, A. & Danielle, S. McNamara. 2013. Predicting human judgments of essay quality in both integrated and independent second language writing samples: A comparison study. Assessing Writing, 18(3). 218–238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.asw.2013.05.002.Search in Google Scholar

Hoffman, Paul, Matthew A. Lambon Ralph & Timothy T. Rogers. 2013. Semantic diversity: A measure of semantic ambiguity based on variability in the contextual usage of words. Behavior Research Methods 45(3). 718–730. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0278-x.Search in Google Scholar

Hyland, Ken. 2008. As can be seen: Lexical bundles and disciplinary variation. English for Specific Purposes 27(1). 4–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.esp.2007.06.001.Search in Google Scholar

Ireland, Molly E. 2011. Three explanations for the link between language style matching and liking. Austin, TX: The University of Texas at Austin dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Ireland, Molly E. & James W. Pennebaker. 2010. Language style matching in writing: Synchrony in essays, correspondence, and poetry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99(3). 549–571. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020386.Search in Google Scholar

Ireland, Molly E., Richard B. Slatcher, Paul W. Eastwick, Lauren E. Scissors, Eli J. Finkel & James W. Pennebaker. 2011. Language style matching predicts relationship initiation and stability. Psychological Science 22(1). 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610392928.Search in Google Scholar

Jung, YeonJoo, Kim YouJin & Murphy John. 2017. The role of task repetition in learning word-stress patterns through auditory priming tasks. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 39(2). 319–346. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263117000031.Search in Google Scholar

Kim, Midam, William S. Horton & Ann R. Bradlow. 2011. Phonetic convergence in spontaneous conversations as a function of interlocutor language distance. Laboratory Phonology 2(1). 125–156. https://doi.org/10.1515/labphon.2011.004.Search in Google Scholar

Kim, YouJin, Jung YeonJoo & Skalicky. Stephen. 2019. Linguistic alignment, learner characteristics, and the production of stranded prepositions in relative clauses: Comparing FTF and SCMC contexts. tudies in Second Language Acquisition 41(5). 937–969. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000093.Search in Google Scholar

Krauss, Robert M. & Jennifer S. Pardo. 2004. Is alignment always the result of automatic priming? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2(2). 203–204.10.1017/S0140525X0436005XSearch in Google Scholar

Kuperman, Victor, Hans Stadthagen-Gonzales & Marc Brysbaert. 2012. Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30 thousand English words. Behavioral Research Methods 44(4). 978–990.10.3758/s13428-012-0210-4Search in Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per Bruun Brockhoff & Rune Haubo Bojesen Christensen. 2015. lmerTest: Tests in linear mixed effects models. R package version 2.0-29. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest (accessed 17 December 2019).10.18637/jss.v082.i13Search in Google Scholar

Kyle, Kristopher & A. Crossley Scott. 2015. Automatically assessing lexical sophistication: Indices, tools, findings, and application. TESOL Quarterly 49(4). 757–786. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.194.Search in Google Scholar

Kyle, Kristopher, A. Crossley Scott & M. Berger Cynthia. 2018. The tool for the automatic analysis of lexical sophistication (TAALES): version 2.0. Behavior Research Methods 50(13). 1030–1046.10.3758/s13428-017-0924-4Search in Google Scholar

Landauer, Thomas, Danielle S. McNamara, Simon Dennis & Walter Kintsch. 2007. LSA: A road to meaning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar

Levelt, Willem J. M. 1989. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lev-Ari, Shiri. 2015. Comprehending non-native speakers: Theory and evidence for adjustment in manner of processing. Frontiers in Psychology 5. 1546. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01546.Search in Google Scholar

McDonald, Scott A. & Richard C. Shillcock. 2001. Rethinking the word frequency effect: The neglected role of distributional information in lexical processing. Language and Speech 44(3). 295–322. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309010440030101.Search in Google Scholar

McDonough, Kim, Heike Neumann & Pavel Trofimovich. 2015. Eliciting production of L2 target structures through collaborative priming activities. Canadian Modern Language Review 71(1). 75–95. https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.2023.Search in Google Scholar

McDonough, Kim & Pavel Trofimovich. 2009. Using priming methods in second language research. New York, NY: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Meltzoff, Andrew N. 2005. Imitation and other minds: The “like me” hypothesis. Perspectives on Imitation: From Neuroscience to Social Science 2. 55–77.Search in Google Scholar

Nakagawa, Shinichi & Holger Schielzeth. 2013. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed‐effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 4(2). 133–142. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x.Search in Google Scholar

Niederhoffer, Kate G. & James W. Pennebaker. 2002. Linguistic style matching in social interaction. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 21(4). 337–360. https://doi.org/10.1177/026192702237953.Search in Google Scholar

Pickering, Martin J. & Simon Garrod. 2004. The interactive-alignment model: Developments and refinements. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(2). 212–225. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x04450055.Search in Google Scholar

Purmohammad, Mehdi. 2015. Linguistic alignment in L1–L2 dialogue. Languages and Dialogues 5(2). 312–333. https://doi.org/10.1075/ld.5.2.07pur.Search in Google Scholar

R Development Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (version 3.1.3) [software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Search in Google Scholar

Tarone, Elaine. 1983. On the variability of interlanguage systems. Applied Linguistics 4(2). 142–164. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/4.2.142.Search in Google Scholar

Tarone, Elaine. 1988. Variation in interlanguage. London, UK: Edward Arnold.Search in Google Scholar

Tarone, Elaine. 2007. Sociolinguistic approaches to second language acquisition research-1997–2007. The Modern Language Journal 91(1). 837–848. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00672.x.Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael & Malinda Carpenter. 2007. Shared intentionality. Developmental Science 10(1). 121–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x.Search in Google Scholar

Vinson, David W., Maryam Tabatabaeian Rick Dale & Nicholas D. Duran. 2016. Seeing and believing: Social influences on language processing. In Ramesh Kumar Mishra, Narayanan Srinivasan & Huettig Falk (eds.), Attention and vision in language processing, 197–213. Berlin, Germany: Springer.10.1007/978-81-322-2443-3_12Search in Google Scholar

Weatherholtz, Kodi, Kathryn Campbell-Kibler & T. Florian Jaeger. 2014. Socially-mediated syntactic alignment. Language Variation and Change 26(3). 387–420. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954394514000155.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2020-06-01
Accepted: 2022-09-14
Published Online: 2022-12-21

© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 1.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/applirev-2020-0048/html
Scroll to top button