Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T15:36:16.024Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rethinking Multilingual Experience through a Systems Framework of Bilingualism: Response to Commentaries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2023

Debra A. Titone*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University; Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, Montreal, Canada
Mehrgol Tiv
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University; Centre for Research on Brain, Language, and Music, Montreal, Canada
*
Address for correspondence: Debra A. Titone (Email: debra.titone@mcgill.ca) or Mehrgol Tiv (Email: mehrgol.tiv@mail.mcgill.ca), Department of Psychology, McGill University, 2001 McGill College Ave., Montréal, QC H3A 1G1, CA

Abstract

In Rethinking Multilingual Experience through a Systems Framework of Bilingualism (Titone & Tiv, 2022), we encouraged psycholinguists and cognitive neuroscientists to consider integrating social and ecological aspects of multilingualism into a collective understanding of its cognitive and neurocognitive bases (i.e., to rethink experience). We then offered a framework – the Systems Framework of Bilingualism and described empirical challenges and potential solutions with applying this framework to new research. Since the paper's publication, several eminent colleagues read and commented on our Keynote, noting both its strengths and areas for improvement. We read each commentary with enthusiasm and gratitude. Here, we briefly respond to several salient points raised, which led us to clarify and improve our theoretical approach. We first address what the commentaries agreed were strengths of the framework. We follow this with a discussion of what the commentaries stated could be improved or extended. We conclude with ways that we modified our model to collectively address concerns raised in the commentaries.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahooja, A, Brouillard, M, Quirk, E, Ballinger, S, Polka, L, Byers-Heinlein, K and Kircher, R (2022) Family language policy among Québec-based parents raising multilingual infants and toddlers: A study of resources as a form of language management. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 120. doi:10.1080/01434632.2022.2050918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bak, TH (2016) Cooking pasta in La Paz: Bilingualism, bias and the replication crisis. In Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism (Vol. 6, Issue 5). John Benjamins, pp. 699717. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16002.bakGoogle Scholar
Ballinger, S, Brouillard, M, Ahooja, A, Kircher, R, Polka, L and Byers-Heinlein, K (2020) Intersections of official and family language policy in Quebec. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 115. doi:10.1080/01434632.2020.1752699Google Scholar
Calabria, M, Costa, A, Green, DW and Abutalebi, J (2018) Neural basis of bilingual language control. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1426, 221235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell-Harris, C (2019) Language learning following immigration: Modeling choices and challenges. In Diallo SY, Wildman W, LeRon Shults F and Tolk A (eds), Human Simulation: Perspectives, Insights, and Applications. New York/Cham: Springer, pp. 95123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell-Harris, C (2022) The systems framework dispels causal illusions and organizes multi-causal models. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000098Google Scholar
Danek, A, Rainer, T and Della Sala, S (2022) Ockham's razor, not a barber's weapon but a writer's tool. Brain : a Journal of Neurology 145, 18701873. doi:10.1093/brain/awac159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Bruin, A (2019) Not all bilinguals are the same: a call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences 9, 33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLuca, V (2022) Exploring nuance in both experience and adaptation: Commentary on Titone and Tiv (2022). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000360Google Scholar
Ellis, NC (2022) Bilingual language cognition as a complex adaptive system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000372Google Scholar
Genesee, F (2022) Sociocultural dimensons of early dual language learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000384Google Scholar
Green, DW (2011) Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in Psychology 2, 103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, DW (2022) On path diagrams and the neurophenomenal field in bilinguals. Languages 7, 260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, DW and Kroll, JF (2019) The neurolinguistics of bilingualism. The Oxford Handbook of Neurolinguistics, 261.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F (1998) Studying bilinguals: methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, 131149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F (2016) The Complementarity Principle and its impact on processing, acquisition, and dominance. In Silva-Corvalán C and Treffers-Daller J (eds), Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gullifer, J and Anderson, JA (2022) Challenges of complexity, and possible solutions: a commentary on rethinking multilingual experience through a Systems Framework of Bilingualism by Titone and Tiv. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000165Google Scholar
Hernández-Rivera, E, Gullifer, JW and Titone, D (2022) Socioecological and psycholinguistic approaches to multilingual health literacy. Translational Issues in Psychological Science 8, 197209. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itzhak, I, Vingron, N, Baum, SR and Titone, D (2017) Bilingualism in the real world: How proficiency, emotion, and personality in a second language impact communication in clinical and legal settings. Translational Issues in Psychological Science 3, 48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroll, JF, Chan, A, Cheng, A and Scontras, G (2022) How to frame bilingualism in context: putting people and places in mind. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000074Google Scholar
Luk, G (2022) Justice and equity for whom? Reframing research on the “bilingual (dis) advantage”. Applied Psycholinguistics, 115.Google Scholar
Luk, G and Grundy, J (2022) The importance of recognizing social contexts in research on bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13. doi:10.1017/S1366728922000177Google Scholar
Navarro-Torres, CA, Beatty-Martínez, AL, Kroll, JF and Green, DW (2021) Research on bilingualism as discovery science. Brain and language 222, 105014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliatsikas, C (2020) Understanding structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23, 459471. doi:10.1017/S1366728919000130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titone, D and Tiv, M (2022) Rethinking multilingual experience through a Systems Framework of Bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 116. doi:10.1017/S1366728921001127Google Scholar
Wu, YJ and Thierry, G (2010) Investigating bilingual processing: the neglected role of language processing contexts. Frontiers in Psychology 1, 178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, Z and Schwieter, JW (2018) Recognizing the effects of language mode on the cognitive advantages of bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 366. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed