figure b

Introduction

The states of impaired fasting glucose (IFG) and impaired glucose tolerance (IGT) are associated with a high, but heterogeneous, rate of progression to type 2 diabetes [1]. In Olmsted County, MN, USA, ‘only’ 40% of people with a fasting glucose ≥6.1 mmol/l progress to overt diabetes within 10 years [2]. This can be partially, but not completely, explained by lifestyle and heredity [3, 4]. In addition, a subset of people with isolated IFG with normal glucose tolerance (i.e. IFG/NGT) have normal beta cell function [5, 6], as quantified by the disposition index (DI) [7], in response to an oral challenge.

DI expresses insulin secretion as a function of insulin action [8] and is the standard measure of beta cell function. These data suggest that (at least in IFG/NGT) defects in the regulation of fasting glucose occur independently of defects in postprandial beta cell function [5]. This is in keeping with the results of genetic association studies that have identified loci that alter fasting glucose but not glucose tolerance and vice versa [9,10,11]. However, a recent longitudinal study in an Asian population [12] showed that the rate of conversion to type 2 diabetes is similar in IFG/NGT and in normal fasting glucose (NFG) with IGT (NFG/IGT), while it is highest in IFG with IGT (IFG/IGT). This suggests that progression to type 2 diabetes can occur in the absence of an abnormal beta cell response to an oral challenge.

The oral minimal model measures two components of beta cell responsivity (Φ) to an oral glucose challenge: dynamic (Φd), representing a response to rising glucose concentrations; and static (Φs), representing a response to constant glucose concentrations [6]. Neither component is abnormal in people with IFG/NGT. However, abnormalities of Φ are present in people with IGT [5]. This implies that changes in beta cell responses to glucose as quantified by Φ, and its components, cannot explain fasting hyperglycaemia.

If beta cell function is intact in people with isolated IFG, fasting insulin secretion would be expected to rise to restrain endogenous glucose production (EGP) and lower fasting glucose. Since this does not occur, one potential explanation is an increased threshold for glucose-stimulated insulin secretion as observed in people with mutations in glucokinase (GCK) [13]. Type 2 diabetes is characterised by an acquired defect in hepatic glucokinase activity [14, 15] but it is unknown if similar abnormalities occur in the beta cells of individuals with IFG/NGT. Our primary hypothesis is that IFG with or without abnormal glucose tolerance is associated with impaired beta cell sensing of glucose, i.e. an increased glucose threshold for insulin secretion.

Given that prediabetes predisposes to a disease (type 2 diabetes) characterised by defects in both beta cell and alpha cell function, there has been interest in the role of alpha cell dysfunction in the progression of prediabetes. This interest has been given added impetus by the observation that diabetes-associated genetic variation in TCF7L2 is associated with impaired postprandial glucagon suppression in non-diabetic individuals [16]. Elevated fasting glucagon has been associated with impaired insulin action [7, 17] and with progression of glucose intolerance [18]. Some [19, 20], but not all [6], prior experiments have failed to consistently document elevated fasting glucagon concentrations in people with IFG, compared with those with NFG. Nevertheless, in the presence of fasting hyperglycaemia, fasting glucagon concentrations indistinguishable from normal values imply a defect in the ability of glucose to suppress glucagon secretion by the alpha cell [21]. This formed the basis of our secondary hypothesis, namely that alpha cell dysfunction contributes to the pathogenesis of fasting hyperglycaemia.

To address these questions, we used a graded glucose infusion (GGI) [22] to measure insulin [23] and glucagon [24] secretion in response to rising glucose concentrations.

Methods

Screening

After approval from the Mayo Clinic Institutional Review Board, we recruited individuals through a combination of intramural and extramural advertising as well as from a database of prior participants whose fasting glucose and glucose tolerance status was known. To be eligible, individuals had no history of chronic illness or upper gastrointestinal surgery. Additionally, they were not taking medications that could affect glucose metabolism. Potentially eligible individuals who showed interest in participating were invited to the Clinical Research and Trials Unit (CRTU) for a screening visit. After written, informed consent was obtained, participants underwent a 2 h 75 g OGTT to characterise their glucose tolerance status, as previously described [5]. All participants were instructed to follow a weight-maintenance diet containing 55% carbohydrate, 30% fat and 15% protein for at least 3 days prior to the study. Body composition was measured at the time of screening using dual-energy x-ray absorptiometry (Lunar, Madison, WI, USA).

Experimental design

Participants were admitted to the CRTU at 17:00 hours on the day before the study. After consuming a standard 10 kcal/kg caffeine-free meal, they fasted overnight. The following morning at 06:00 hours a dorsal hand vein was cannulated and placed in a heated Plexiglas box maintained at 55°C to allow sampling of arterialised venous blood. The contralateral forearm vein was cannulated for glucose and hormone infusions. At 07:00 hours (−120 min) insulin (0.1 U/ml Humulin R; Eli Lilly, Indianapolis, IN, USA) was infused at a variable rate to lower and maintain glucose concentrations at 4.44 mmol/l (80 mg/dl) [25]. The insulin infusion was discontinued at 08:30 hours (−30 min). At 09:00 hours (0 min) a GGI using 50% dextrose commenced at 1 mg kg−1 min−1 and then increased to 2 (10:00 hours), 4 (11:00 hours) and 8 mg kg−1 min−1 (12:00 hours) every 60 min [22]. The experiment ended at 13:00 hours when all infusions were stopped. Participants consumed a meal and left the CRTU when it was safe to do so.

Analytic techniques

All blood was immediately placed on ice after collection, centrifuged at 4°C, separated and stored at −80°C until assay. Plasma glucose concentrations were measured using a Yellow Springs Glucose Analyzer (Yellow Springs Instruments, Yellow Springs, OH, USA). Plasma insulin concentrations were measured using a chemiluminescence assay (Access Assay; Beckman, Chaska, MN, USA). Plasma C-peptide was measured using a two-site immunoenzymatic sandwich assay (Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN, USA). Glucagon was measured using a two-site ELISA (Mercodia, Winston Salem, NC, USA) in accordance with the manufacturer’s instructions.

Calculations

The insulin secretion rate (ISR) was calculated from C-peptide concentrations using nonparametric deconvolution and population-based measures of C-peptide kinetics [26]. Linear regression was used to describe the increase of ISR in response to the observed glucose concentrations for each individual. We characterised individual responses using the ΔISRGlucose gradient and the intercept of this slope at a glucose of 4.44 mmol/l. We then utilised the individualised linear relationships, together with a non-negativity constraint, to predict the response of ISR over a wider range of 3.3–15 mmol/l. The lower limit was chosen to avoid the presumed glucose concentration at which insulin secretion is suppressed [27].

Likewise, glucagon secretion rate (GSR) was calculated from the measured glucagon concentrations observed during the experiments using nonparametric deconvolution and the population model of glucagon kinetics we described recently [24]. The exponential relationship of GSR to glucose concentrations in each individual was then used to derive the increment in glucose concentration necessary to suppress GSR by 50% (G50) in each participant. Please refer to the electronic supplementary material (ESM) Methods for more details.

The oral minimal model [8] was used to calculate insulin secretion and action at the time of screening as previously described [28, 29]

Statistical analysis

All continuous data are summarised as means ± SEM. AUC and area above basal (AAB) were calculated using the trapezoidal rule. One-way ANOVA and a Tukey’s post hoc test were used to determine between-group differences (parametric data). For nonparametric data, a Kruskal–Wallis test followed by a Dunn’s post hoc test was used. When necessary, multivariate analysis adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight was performed. BlueSky Statistics software v.7.10 (BlueSky Statistics, Chicago, IL, USA) and Prism 5 v.5.0f (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA) were utilised for the statistical analysis. Residuals for the conditional logistic regression of a particular variable with the covariates were used to confirm or refute the contribution of that variable to variation in the tested index. A p value <0.05 was considered statistically significant. Our power calculation for the glucose threshold of insulin secretion was based on observed (mean ± SD) glucose concentrations of 4.8 ± 0.5 mmol/l [16]. Assuming similar variability, 15 participants with IFG would give us the ability to detect a 0.36 mmol/l (7%) difference in glucose threshold compared with people with NFG.

Results

Participant characteristics by fasting glucose and by glucose tolerance status

A total of 39 participants were studied. At the time of screening, 14 participants were classified as NFG/NGT. Participants with IFG and/or IGT were heavier. This trend was not explained by differences in lean body mass (Table 1). As previously observed, participants with NFG/IGT and those with IFG/IGT exhibited defects in insulin secretion and action as quantified by the oral minimal model [6]. See also ESM Fig. 1.

Table 1 Participant characteristics at the time of screening characterised by their fasting glucose and glucose tolerance status

Glucose, insulin, C-peptide and glucagon concentrations in groups classified by fasting glucose and by glucose tolerance status

By design, at the time when the insulin infusion was discontinued (−30 min), glucose concentrations did not differ between groups. However, in people with IFG/NGT, glucose concentrations over the subsequent 30 min (−30 to 0 min) were significantly increased compared with those with NFG/NGT. In response to GGI, glucose concentrations were higher in those with IFG/NGT and IFG/IGT (Fig. 1b and c, respectively), but by the end of the study did not differ between groups. See also ESM Table 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Glucose (ac), insulin (df), C-peptide (gi) and glucagon (jl) concentrations during the GGI, for participants with NFG/IGT (black circles), those with IFG/NGT (black squares) and those with IFG/IGT (black triangles), when compared with those with NFG/NGT (white circles)

Higher insulin concentrations at the time when insulin infusion was discontinued (−30 min) were observed in the IFG/NGT and the IFG/IGT groups. Insulin concentrations in response to GGI did not differ between these two groups (Fig. 1e and f, respectively). See also ESM Table 1.

Fasting C-peptide concentrations were significantly increased in people with NFG/IGT, compared with those with NFG/NGT. In response to GGI, C-peptide concentrations were increased in all three groups with prediabetes compared with NFG/NGT (Fig. 1g–l). See also ESM Table 1.

Fasting glucagon concentrations as well as the concentrations observed during the GGI did not differ between groups (Fig. 1j–l). See also ESM Table 1.

ISR over time and its relationship with glucose concentrations in groups classified by fasting glucose and by glucose tolerance status

The ISR over time did not differ between groups whether considered as absolute values (Fig. 2a–c) or as integrated AUC (data not shown). See also ESM Fig. 2 for data from representative individuals.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The mean (± SEM) ISR over time observed during the GGI for participants with NFG/IGT (a), those with IFG/NGT (b) and those with IFG/IGT (c), compared with those with NFG/NGT. The corresponding panels (df) show the relationship of ISR (mean ± SEM) and glucose concentrations. In addition, the symbols (black circle [NFG/IGT], black square [IFG/NGT], black triangle [IFG/IGT] and white circle [NFG/NGT]) in (df) show the relationship between mean fasting ISR and glucose on the day of the screening visit

The model for the relationship of ISR with glucose predicts that ISR is completely suppressed at 3.0 ± 0.3 vs 2.5 ± 0.4 vs 3.3 ± 0.3 vs 2.7 ± 0.2 mmol/l (NFG/NGT vs NFG/IGT vs IFG/NGT vs IFG/IGT, respectively, p=0.50; Fig. 2d–f). AUC was compared for glucose values ≤5.55 mmol/l, values >5.55 and ≤7.77 mmol/l, and values >7.77 mmol/l. These thresholds were chosen using the criteria for diagnosis of IFG and of IGT.

This analysis showed that for glucose concentrations >5.55 mmol/l, people with IFG and/or IGT exhibited a similar relationship to that observed in NFG/NGT. On the other hand, for glucose concentrations ≤5.55 mmol/l, people with NFG/IGT secreted more insulin (0.35 ± 0.03 vs 0.55 ± 0.06 vs 0.43 ± 0.12 vs 0.44 ± 0.05 nmol/min per mmol/l, NFG/NGT vs NFG/IGT vs IFG/NGT vs IFG/IGT, respectively, p=0.04; Fig. 2d–f).

At the time of the screening OGTT, the relationship of fasting ISR with fasting glucose exhibited a similar relationship to that observed during the GGI study day (as shown in Fig. 2d–f).

Indices of insulin secretion derived from the GGI in groups classified by fasting glucose status and by glucose tolerance status and the relationship of these indices with fasting and post-challenge glucose concentrations

The ΔISRGlucose gradient observed during GGI did not differ across the groups defined by fasting glucose status and by glucose tolerance status (Fig. 3a).

Fig. 3
figure 3

(a, d) The mean (± SEM) gradient of the relationship between ISR and glucose (ΔISRGlucose) and its intercept (a and d, respectively) for participants with NFG/NGT (white circles), NFG/IGT (black circles), IFG/NGT (black squares) and IFG/IGT (black triangles) during the GGI. There was no correlation of either variable with fasting (b and e, respectively) and peak (c and f, respectively) glucose concentrations at the time of the screening OGTT. R2 and p values represent results from a multivariate analysis adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight. The inset panels represent residuals after adjusting for these covariates

To avoid categorical errors in the classification of participants with/without IFG and/or IGT, we examined the relationship of individual fasting glucose concentrations (and post-challenge values) observed at the time of screening with these indices. After adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight, the ΔISRGlucose gradient did not correlate with fasting glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening (Fig. 3b) or with peak post-OGTT glucose (Fig. 3c) or with 120 min glucose concentrations (data not shown).

Since the relationship of ISR to glucose for every individual is described both by its gradient and by its intercept, we also examined the latter variable across the groups defined by fasting glucose status (Fig. 3d) and by glucose tolerance status. In addition, we examined the relationship of individual fasting glucose concentrations (Fig. 3e) and post-challenge values (Fig. 3f) observed at the time of screening after adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight. A similar lack of correlation was observed.

The relationship of GSR and glucose concentrations in groups classified by fasting glucose status and by glucose tolerance status

Fasting GSR did not differ across the four groups. Similarly, rates of glucagon secretion did not differ over time in the NFG/IGT, IFG/NGT and IFG/IGT groups compared with the NFG/NGT group (Fig. 4a–c). In all groups, GSR exhibited an inverse exponential relationship with increasing glucose concentrations. Although nadir glucagon secretion did not differ among the groups (Fig. 4d, e), participants with IFG/NGT (Fig. 4e) and IFG/IGT (Fig. 4f) required higher glucose concentrations to suppress GSR to the values observed in NFG/NGT. See also ESM Fig. 2 for data from representative individuals.

Fig. 4
figure 4

The mean (± SEM) GSR over time observed during the GGI for participants with NFG/IGT (a), those with IFG/NGT (b) and those with IFG/IGT (c), respectively, compared with those with NFG/NGT. The corresponding panels (df) show the relationship of GSR (mean ± SEM) and glucose concentrations. In addition, the symbols (black circle [NFG/IGT], black square [IFG/NGT], black triangle [IFG/IGT] and white circle [NFG/NGT]) in panels (df) show the relationship between mean fasting GSR and glucose on the day of the screening visit

At the time of the screening OGTT, the relationship of GSR with fasting glucose exhibited a similar relationship to that observed during the GGI study day (as shown in Fig. 4d–f).

The G50 in groups classified by fasting glucose status and by glucose tolerance status and its relationship with fasting and post-challenge glucose concentrations

The inverse exponential relationship of glucagon secretion with glucose concentrations enables calculation of the increase in glucose concentration necessary to suppress GSR by 50%. This index (G50) allows comparison of alpha cell responsivity to glucose (Fig. 5a).

Fig. 5
figure 5

(a) The mean G50 (± SEM) for participants with NFG/NGT (white circles), NFG/IGT (black circles), IFG/NGT (black squares) and IFG/IGT (black triangles) during the GGI. *p<0.05 for a post hoc Tukey’s test vs the NFG/NGT group after a one-way ANOVA test. G50 correlated with fasting (b) and peak (c) glucose concentrations at the time of the screening OGTT. R2 and p values represent results from a multivariate analysis adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight. The inset panels represent residuals after adjusting for these covariates

In this cohort, G50 differed significantly (p<0.01) across the four groups characterised by fasting glucose, glucose tolerance or peak post-OGTT glucose concentrations. People with IFG/NGT (2.4 ± 0.2 mmol/l) and IFG/IGT (2.7 ± 0.6 mmol/l), but not those with NFG/IGT (1.6 ± 0.3 mmol/l), exhibited higher values than those observed in people with NFG/NGT (1.4 ± 0.2 mmol/l; Fig. 5a).

To avoid categorical errors in the classification of participants with/without IFG and/or IGT, we examined the relationship of individual fasting glucose concentrations (and post-challenge values) observed at the time of screening with G50. After adjusting for the effects of age, sex and weight, G50 correlated (R2=0.15, p=0.02) with fasting glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening (Fig. 5b). G50 also correlated with peak post-OGTT glucose (Fig. 5c) and with 120 min glucose concentrations (data not shown) observed at the time of screening.

Discussion

Prediabetes is a heterogenous condition with variable progression to type 2 diabetes. Conventionally, a 2 h 75 g OGTT has been used to classify prediabetes based on fasting and 120 min glucose concentrations (ESM Fig. 1, Table 1) [30]. Of the resulting three subgroups of prediabetes, the group with isolated IFG (i.e. IFG/NGT) has courted the most controversy [31]. Prior work suggested that these individuals exhibit normal beta cell function in response to an oral challenge although they still are at increased risk of progression to type 2 diabetes [12]. In addition, we previously demonstrated that beta cell function quantified by DI did not correlate with fasting glucose concentrations in this population [5].

A possible explanation for these observations is that people with IFG/NGT have an altered set-point for insulin secretion but a normal response to a subsequent glucose challenge. Indeed, there is considerable genetic evidence to suggest that fasting glucose concentrations are regulated independently of postprandial glucose concentrations [9, 32, 33]. To test our primary hypothesis, we used an experimental design where glucose concentrations were lowered to similar values in all participants. We then compared the relationship of ISR with changing glucose concentrations, during a GGI (Fig. 2).

We did not observe a significant difference of the ΔISRGlucose gradient across the spectrum of prediabetes when compared with NFG/NGT (Fig. 3), at least in the range of glucose concentrations observed during the study. These observations are congruent with the data obtained in response to OGTT (Table 1), where, as before, beta cell responsivity to glucose (Φ) is unchanged across categories of prediabetes [6]. However, when expressed as a function of prevailing insulin sensitivity (Si ) as a DI, beta cell function decreases with worsening glucose tolerance (Table 1). Future development of a novel model enabling expression of insulin secretion as a function of insulin action during GGI may allow discrimination of differences in islet function across subtypes of prediabetes, as is the case for OGTT.

During the GGI, we did not observe glucose concentrations below values where insulin secretion would be completely suppressed. In addition, we and others [22] did not observe an inflection point at which the ΔISRGlucose relationship changed over the range of observed glucose concentrations. More germane to our hypothesis, ISRs at glucose concentrations within the normal fasting range did not differ from those observed in NFG/NGT in people with IFG/NGT and IFG/IGT. This leads us to reject our primary hypothesis. Indeed beta cell responsivity to (fasting) glucose at the time of screening (Φbasal; Table 1) also did not differ between these two groups. It is also notable that the relationship of fasting ISR to fasting glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening was congruent with that observed during the GGI (Fig. 2).

Intriguingly, both Φbasal from the OGTT and the ISR observed during the GGI in this range of glucose concentrations were increased in people with NFG/IGT. Impaired insulin action is the primary defect observed in such individuals [34] and several authors have proposed that the consequent chronic beta cell stimulation may impair insulin secretion [35, 36]. NEFA are elevated after an overnight fast in individuals with impaired insulin action [37] and the rise in NEFA stimulates beta cell function [38, 39]. However, differences in NEFA concentrations are unlikely to explain our observation in NFG/IGT given the exposure to insulin (which would suppress NEFA) prior to the GGI. Both the gradient and intercept (Fig. 3) for the relationship of ISR with glucose concentrations did not correlate with fasting and post-challenge glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening.

Since defects in beta cell glucose sensing alone do not explain IFG, we examined whether alpha cell dysfunction contributes to the dysregulation of fasting glucose. Prior work using an OGTT or a mixed meal challenge has failed to show significant differences in absolute fasting glucagon concentrations across the subgroups of prediabetes [6]. This also appeared to be the case in our experiments (ESM Fig. 1, Table 1). However, there is genetic and observational evidence to suggest that alpha cell dysfunction contributes to progression to type 2 diabetes [16, 18].

When GSRs were deconvolved from glucagon concentrations observed during the GGI, we demonstrated a single-exponential relationship of glucagon secretion with glucose concentrations (Fig. 4). This relationship can be quantified by the change in glucose necessary to suppress the GSR by 50% (G50). In fact, people with IFG with or without IGT exhibited higher values of G50 compared with people with NFG, implying that alpha cell insensitivity to glucose contributes to IFG (Fig. 5). Indeed, when fasting GSRs are plotted against fasting glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening, these points lie on the regression line for glucagon secretion and glucose concentrations observed during the GGI (Fig. 4). This observation demonstrates that alpha cell dysfunction, as characterised during the GGI, can explain fasting hyperglycaemia in people with IFG. Impaired suppression of glucagon secretion by glucose, as quantified by increased G50, also correlated with increased post-challenge glucose concentrations at the time of screening (Fig. 5). This is consistent with the previously observed effects of impaired glucagon suppression on postprandial glucose concentrations even when insulin secretion is relatively preserved [40]. There was no correlation of G50 with surrogates of beta cell function (data not shown).

This experiment has some limitations that need to be addressed. The first is the relatively limited numbers of individuals in the subcategories of prediabetes. Similarly, given the variability in fasting and post-OGTT glucose concentrations, there is the possibility of categorical classification errors [41, 42]. To overcome these limitations, we have analysed the relationship of the study variables with fasting and post-challenge glucose concentrations observed at the time of screening, as continuous variables. To ensure comparable glucose concentrations across all participants on the day of the GGI, we infused insulin (−120 to −30 min). Although exogenous insulin can alter beta cell function, which might affect insulin secretion [43], the short t1/2 of insulin would ensure the absence of exogenous insulin by the start of glucose infusion (0 min). C-peptide-based measures of insulin secretion ensure that our results are not influenced by the presence of exogenous insulin. It is notable that the relationships of ISR and of GSR with glucose concentrations in the fasting state were congruent between the screening and study days (Figs 2, 4). This also suggests that exogenous insulin infusion did not alter islet function during the GGI.

Strengths of the experiment include the ability to compare beta cell and alpha cell function in response to an oral and intravenous challenge, and the relatively large overall sample size. The ability to deconvolve glucagon secretion from glucagon concentrations allows description of the exponential relationship between glucagon secretion and glucose concentrations. Furthermore, we propose a novel index that can quantify the suppression of the alpha cell by glucose in an individual. This index (G50) is increased in individuals with IFG and indeed appears to be the only identifiable abnormality of islet function in people with isolated IFG (IFG/NGT). An important corollary of this work is to emphasise the heterogeneity of prediabetes and the need to adapt therapy to the underlying pathogenic mechanisms. In a setting where alpha cell function is a significant contributor to hyperglycaemia, it may be reasonable to avoid pharmacotherapy that significantly increases fasting glucagon secretion [44].

In summary, we conclude that contrary to our hypothesis, defects in insulin secretion at low to normal glucose concentrations do not play a role in the pathogenesis of isolated IFG. Somewhat paradoxically, insulin secretion at lower glucose concentrations was enhanced in people with NFG/IGT. Whether this is a marker of early beta cell stress or of progression to type 2 diabetes remains to be ascertained. More importantly, we provide additional evidence for alpha cell dysfunction contributing to the pathogenesis of prediabetes, particularly in isolated IFG. In addition, we describe a novel index, G50, which is a marker of alpha cell responsiveness to glucose and may be useful in better characterising the heterogenous pathophysiology of prediabetes.