Introduction

Perceptual training is essential for individuals who need specialized perceptual skills (Deveau et al., 2014; Frank et al., 2020) or suffer perceptual deficits (Huang et al., 2008; Levi & Li, 2009; Polat et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2014). Previous investigations have shown improved performance for perceptual tasks by training and attributed the training effects to modifications on sensory (Adab & Vogels, 2011; Chen et al., 2015; Furmanski et al., 2004; Jia et al., 2020; Schoups et al., 2001; Schwartz et al., 2002; Yan et al., 2014; Yang & Maunsell, 2004), decision making (Dosher et al., 2013; Jia et al., 2018; Kahnt et al., 2011; Kuai et al., 2013; Law & Gold, 2008), and short-term memory (Jia et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2016) processes. A classical finding in perceptual learning was that the training effects were largely specific to the trained tasks and stimuli (Ahissar & Hochstein, 1997; Fahle, 2005; Karni & Sagi, 1991), potentially limiting the applicability of perceptual training outside the laboratories. Transfers of training effects between tasks and stimuli were observed, but the generalization of perceptual learning was suggested to be influenced by various factors (Censor, 2013; Jeter et al., 2010; Larcombe et al., 2017; McGovern et al., 2012). Accordingly, previous literature has proposed different procedures to improve the transfer of training effects to untrained stimuli (Donovan et al., 2015; Donovan & Carrasco, 2018; Wang et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2010). However, given the potential complexity of the tasks and stimuli, the generalization issue remains a barrier for the real-world applications of perceptual training. Alternatively, if the required perceptual skills are within a specific collection of expertise (e.g., for an athlete or a radiologist; Deveau et al., 2014; Evans et al., 2013; Lago et al., 2021), training on a limited number of representative tasks or stimuli may facilitate the wider application of perceptual learning.

Training multiple tasks or stimuli has its own shortcoming as interference is a ubiquitous phenomenon of learning and memory (Anderson, 2003; Anderson et al., 1994; Herszage & Censor, 2018; Wimber et al., 2009). In perceptual learning literature, it has been shown that performance improvement of one task can be disrupted when the initial training is immediately followed by training on a second task (Been et al., 2011; Seitz et al., 2005; Shibata et al., 2017; Yotsumoto, Chang, et al., 2009a). This is a typical form of retroactive interference observed in memory studies (BrashersKrug et al., 1996; Osgood, 1948; Postman & Underwood, 1973). For example, Seitz and his colleagues have demonstrated that retroactive interference occurs in perceptual learning with a vernier acuity task (Seitz et al., 2005). Specifically, in their study, the second task interfered with the training effect of the first task only when the stimuli in the two tasks were in the same orientation and presented at the same retinal location. This finding suggested that the interference of perceptual memory in the vernier task was specific to location and orientation, and thus could be attributed to low-level visual processing (e.g., primary visual cortex). The shared neural populations for encoding and storage of the two trained tasks might underlie the observed interference effects.

However, multiple perceptual trainings do not always occur within a single day. In fact, the between-training intervals for expert skills may last from a few days to several months. It would be of interest to examine the interference effects of perceptual learning when different trainings are separated by at least 24 h because sleep is a critical factor for the long-lasting system consolidation and is believed to be an important modulator for perceptual learning (Tamaki et al., 2020; Yotsumoto, Sasaki, et al., 2009b). Taking this consideration into account, we aimed to evaluate the interference effects between two perceptual trainings that were conducted in two adjacent days.

The classical theory of memory interference highlights the resulting competition when a retrieval cue is associated with multiple items in the consolidated memory (Anderson et al., 1994; Anderson & Neely, 1996; Greeno, 1964; Postman & Underwood, 1973). That is, in memory retrieval, successful access to a target item from a cue depends not only on how strongly the cue is related to the target item, but also on the strength of the associations between the cue and other distracting items in the memory (Anderson et al., 1994; Anderson & Neely, 1996). When the cue-target association is not stronger than those between the cue and other competing items, memory interference occurs. The phenomenon of interference suggests that the already consolidated perceptual memory could also undergo a new learning as a result of competing cue-target associations.

In the present study, we first examined whether the consolidated perceptual memory from short-term training could be interfered with by a new perceptual training on the same task. We then investigated the nature of the interference and the potential approach to prevent such interference. We conducted five psychophysical experiments with separate groups of participants (30 participants in each group and 150 participants in total). Figure 1 summarizes the experimental procedures of the four experiments. Experiment 1 established a baseline training effect of an orientation detection task for the other three experiments. In Experiment 2, a retroactive interference effect was observed when two orientations were trained at the same retinal location on two adjacent days. Experiment 3 added a 6-h interval between the pre-test and training of the second orientation to examine the reactivation account of the interference effect. In Experiment 4, two contextual color cues were associated with the two orientations during the training and test sessions. Finally, Experiment 5 was conducted to examine the robustness of the color cue effect by associating the cues with the two orientations only during the training.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Overview of the procedures for the five experiments

Experiment 1: Control group

In Experiment 1, we established the baseline training effect on the orientation detection task when the Gabor stimuli were centrally presented. The participants were trained on one orientation (ori_1) and tested before and after the training. The post-test of ori_1 was conducted on the third day to match the other experiments.

Method

Participants

Thirty right-handed naïve participants (23 females, age range = 18–28 years, mean age = 21.47 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were recruited for the experiment. The local ethics committee approved the study.

Stimuli and apparatus

All stimuli were generated in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) using Psychtoolbox 3 package (Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) and presented on a gamma-corrected CRT display (1,024 × 768-pixel resolution, 85-Hz refresh rate). Subjects viewed the stimuli at a distance of 57 cm with their head stabilized using a chinrest. Gabor patches (spatial frequency = 1 cycle/degree, contrast = 100%, Gaussian filter sigma = 2.5, random spatial phase, radius = 5°) were presented at the center of the display surrounded by a gray background (mean luminance = 33 cd/m2) during all sessions. A noise pattern created from a sinusoidal luminance distribution was added to the Gabor patches at a given signal-to-noise (S/N) ratio. For example, a 10% S/N ratio represented the noise pattern that replaced 90% pixels of the Gabor patches.

Procedure

Orientation detection task

Participants completed a two-interval forced-choice orientation detection task used in previous studies of perceptual learning (Fig. 2a; Bang, Sasaki, et al., 2018a; Bang, Shibata, et al., 2018b; Shibata et al., 2017). Each trial began with a central fixation dot for 500 ms, followed by two 50-ms stimulus intervals. The two stimulus intervals were separated by a 300-ms blank interval with only the central fixation displayed. A Gabor patch with a certain S/N ratio and a patch of full noise (0% S/N ratio) were presented in the two stimulus intervals, with their order randomized. The participants were required to determine in which interval the Gabor appeared by pressing one of two buttons on the keyboard. There was no time limit for the response and no feedback was provided.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Method and results of Experiment 1. a Procedure for an example trial. b Behavioral results: thresholds on ori_1 in the pre-test and post-test sessions. *** p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors

Threshold measurement

In the orientation detection task, we used a three-down one-up staircase method to measure each participant’s threshold S/N ratio in each block. This method converged to 79.4% correct responses. In each block, the S/N ratio started with 25% and adaptively changed with a step size of 0.05 log units. Each staircase consisted of four practice and six experimental reversals. The participant’s threshold within a block was defined as the geometric mean of the experimental reversals. The first block in each test session (including pre-test and post-test) was discarded and we took the arithmetic average of the threshold S/N ratios across the remaining two blocks as the threshold S/N ratio for the tested orientation.

Training and test sessions

We adopted two orientations (10° and 70° from the horizontal axis) and they were randomly assigned as ori_1 and ori_2 across participants. The participants in Experiment 1 were regarded as the control group and were trained only on ori_1. On day 1, the participants completed a pre-test session of three blocks on ori_1 and then a training session of 16 blocks on ori_1. There was no task on day 2. On day 3, the participants completed post-test sessions on ori_1 and ori_2 in a fixed order, with each orientation being tested for three blocks.

Data analysis

To reduce the impact of initial threshold on the training effect, we considered mean percent improvement (MPI) as the dependent variable. MPI depicts the performance improvement and is defined by ((pre-test threshold on ori_1 – post-test threshold on ori_1)/pre-test threshold on ori_1) × 100 % for ori_1. The calculation of MPI for ori_2 is the same as that for ori_1.

Results and discussion

As shown in Fig. 2b, we observed a significant learning effect on the threshold of ori_1 (t(29) = 7.214, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.32). The MPI of ori_1 in Experiment 1 served as a baseline training effect for both ori_1 and ori_2 trainings in the following experiments.

Experiment 2: Interference group

In Experiment 2, after the training of the first orientation (ori_1) on day 1, we trained the participants on another orientation (ori_2) on day 2 and examined the training effects of both orientations on day 3. The interference effects between the two trainings were measured by comparing their MPIs between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2.

Method

Participants

Thirty right-handed naïve participants (24 females, age range = 18–26 years, mean age = 21.00 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were included in the experiment.

Stimuli and apparatus

The stimuli and apparatus in Experiment 2 were identical to Experiment 1.

Procedure

The procedure in Experiment 2 was identical to Experiment 1 with two exceptions. First, the participants completed a pre-test session of three blocks on ori_2 and then a training session of 16 blocks on ori_2 on day 2. Second, the testing order of the two orientations was counterbalanced across participants on day 3.

Results and discussion

As shown in Fig. 3a, we observed significant learning effects on both ori_1 (t(29) = 3.697, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.67) and ori_2 (t(29) = 4.449, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.81).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Results of Experiment 2. a Behavioral results: thresholds on ori_1 and ori_2 in the pre-test and post-test sessions. b Mean percent improvement (MPI) comparison for ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors

To examine whether training on ori_2 would introduce retroactive interference on the learning effect of ori_1, we compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 2. The result revealed that the MPI of Experiment 2 was significantly smaller than that of Experiment 1 (t(58) = 2.528, p = 0.014, Cohen’s d = 0.65, Fig. 3b). These results suggest that training on the detection of the second orientation induced retroactive interference on the previously trained detection of the first orientation. To test the possible proactive interference from ori_1 training to ori_2 training, we compared the MPI of ori_2 in Experiment 2 with the MPI of ori_1 in Experiment 1. The result revealed a trend of significant difference between the two MPIs (t(58) = 1.782, p = 0.08, Cohen’s d = 0.46).

We considered a few factors that may drive the observed retroactive interference effect. We first considered the reactivation account. The stimuli of ori_1 and ori_2 were similar in their shapes and other features (e.g., they were both Gabor patches). The training procedures were also identical for the two orientations. Therefore, the learning traces of the two trainings could be activated by each other during their retrievals. In this case, it was possible that the pre-test of ori_2 may have reactivated the consolidated learning trace of ori_1 and brought it to an unstable status that was susceptible to interruption (Bang, Shibata, et al., 2018b; Censor et al., 2010; Nader, 2015; Schiller et al., 2010). Once destabilized, the old learning trace of ori_1 could be disrupted by the following training on ori_2 and retroactive interference would be observed. To address this possible interpretation, we conducted Experiment 3 in which a 6-h interval was introduced between the pre-test and training of ori_2. The length of the interval was determined by previous literatures showing that the reactivated memory becomes reconsolidated 6 h after reactivation (Bang, Shibata, et al., 2018b; Censor et al., 2010; Huang & Li, 2022; Nader, 2015; Schiller et al., 2010; Shibata et al., 2017). If the reactivation account could explain the observed retroactive interference, we would expect to observe eliminated interference effect in Experiment 3.

Experiment 3: Interference group with a 6-h interval

Method

Participants

Thirty right-handed naïve participants (22 females, age range = 18–27 years, mean age = 22.23 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were included in the experiment.

Stimuli and apparatus

The stimuli and apparatus in Experiment 3 were identical to Experiment 1.

Procedure

The general procedure in Experiment 3 was identical to Experiment 2, except that on day 2, there was a 6-h interval between the pre-test and training sessions of ori_2.

Results and discussion

As shown in Fig. 4a, we observed significant learning effects on both ori_1 (t(29) = 2.141, p = 0.041, Cohen’s d = 0.39) and ori_2 (t(29) = 5.511, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.01).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Results of Experiment 3. a Behavioral results: thresholds on ori_1 and ori_2 in the pre-test and post-test sessions. b Mean percent improvement (MPI) comparison for ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 3. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors

We compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 3, and the result revealed that the MPI of Experiment 3 was significantly smaller than that of Experiment 1 (t(58) = 2.956, p = 0.005, Cohen’s d = 0.55, Fig. 4b). We also compared the MPI of ori_2 in Experiment 3 with the MPI of ori_1 in Experiment 1 and the result revealed no significant difference between the two MPIs (t(58) = 0.754, p = 0.454, Cohen’s d = 0.19). These results demonstrated a similar retroactive interference effect to Experiment 2 when a 6-h interval was introduced between the pre-test and training of ori_2. The results suggest that the observed retroactive interference effect could not be explained by the destabilized status of the learning trace of ori_1 due to the pre-test of ori_2. Therefore, the reactivation account was not a likely interpretation.

There were two other accounts that could serve as the potential interpretations for the observed interference effect. First, training to detect the Gabor stimuli recruited orientation selective neurons at the primary visual cortex. The neurons involved in the training may not be restricted to those selective to the trained orientation as only a fraction of neurons in visual cortex are dedicated to encode orientation information (Poort et al., 2015). However, the neurons recruited by both ori_1 and ori_2 training may come from an overlapping population of neurons that encoded their shared features and retinotopically responded to the same stimulus location. The interference could come from the new training to the old training as they would compete for the recruitment of the same population of neurons. We refer this possibility as the competing neuronal population account. Second, training may involve the establishment of association between stimuli orientations and training contexts that could benefit the retrieval of the learning traces during the test session. If, however, the two orientations were linked to the same contextual cue (e.g., training protocol or task set), retrieval errors would occur. The retrieval failure due to a shared contextual cue could also contribute to the observed interference effect. We refer this possibility as the shared context account.

We conducted Experiment 4 to dissociate these two possible interpretations. In Experiment 4, the two orientations were associated with different contextual color cues during training and tests. If the competing neuronal population account was true, we would again observe retroactive interference. If the shared context account was true, associating two orientations with the two contextual color cues would prevent the retroactive interference effect.

Experiment 4: Interference group with color cues – training and test

Method

Participants

Thirty right-handed naïve participants (16 females, age range = 18–29 years, mean age = 21.67 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were included in the experiment.

Stimuli and apparatus

The stimuli and apparatus in Experiment 4 were identical to those in Experiment 1.

Procedure

The general procedure in Experiment 4 was identical to Experiment 2, except that the Gabor stimuli of ori_1 and ori_2 were associated with fixation of different colors (red or green) in the training and test sessions (Fig. 5a). The color-orientation associations were counterbalanced across participants. The participants received no instruction about the fixation colors.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Method and results of Experiment 4. a Procedure for an example trial. The two orientations were associated with two colors (red or green) during the training and test sessions. b Behavioral results: thresholds on ori_1 and ori_2 in the pre-test and post-test sessions. c Mean percent improvement (MPI) comparison for ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 4. *** p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors

Results and discussion

As shown in Fig. 5b, we observed significant learning effects on both ori_1 (t(29) = 5.154, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.94) and ori_2 (t(29) = 6.941, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.27).

We compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 4 and the result revealed that the MPI of Experiment 4 was not significantly different from that of Experiment 1 (t(58) = 0.986, p = 0.328 , Cohen’s d = 0.25, Fig. 5c), suggesting that the first training would not be affected by the second training if the two orientations were associated with different color cues. We also compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 2 and Experiment 4 but found no significant difference between them (t(58) = 1.416, p = 0.162, Cohen’s d = 0.37). No significant difference between the MPI of ori_2 in Experiment 4 with the MPI of ori_1 in Experiment 1 was observed (t(58) = 0.284, p = 0.777, Cohen’s d = 0.07).

These results were not consistent with the competing neuronal population account and provided supporting evidence for the shared context account. That is, the shared contextual cue between the two orientations introduced the retroactive interference in Experiment 2. The results demonstrated that relating the two orientations to different contextual cues could prevent the retroactive interference effect.

To further examine the robustness of the contextual cue effect, we conducted Experiment 5, in which the contextual cues were presented during the training but not in the test.

Experiment 5: Interference group with color cues – training only

Method

Participants

Thirty right-handed naïve participants (20 females, age range = 18–27 years, mean age = 21.70 years) with normal or corrected-to-normal vision were included in the experiment.

Stimuli and apparatus

The stimuli and apparatus in Experiment 5 were identical to those in Experiment 1.

Procedure

The general procedure in Experiment 5 was identical to Experiment 4, except that the color-orientation associations existed only during the training sessions. The color of the fixation remained black for both orientations during the test sessions.

Results and discussion

As shown in Fig. 6a, we observed significant learning effects on both ori_1 (t(29) = 6.150, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.12) and ori_2 (t(29) = 6.798, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 1.24).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Results of Experiment 5. a Behavioral results: thresholds on ori_1 and ori_2 in the pre-test and post-test sessions. b Mean percent improvement (MPI) comparison for ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 5. *** p < 0.001. Error bars represent standard errors

We compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 1 and Experiment 5 and the result revealed that the MPI of Experiment 5 was not significantly different from that of Experiment 1 (t(58) = 0.421, p = 0.675 , Cohen’s d = 0.11, Fig. 6b), suggesting that the first training was not affected by the second training. When we compared the MPIs of ori_1 between Experiment 2 and Experiment 5, a marginally significant difference was observed (t(58) = 1.963, p = 0.054 , Cohen’s d = 0.51), further confirming the benefit of the contextual cues. There was also no significant difference in MPI of ori_1 between Experiment 4 and Experiment 5 (t(58)=0.531 p = 0.597 Cohen’s d = 0.14). No significant difference between the MPI of ori_2 in Experiment 5 with the MPI of ori_1 in Experiment 1 was observed (t(58) = 0.347, p = 0.730, Cohen’s d = 0.09). These results suggest that the effect of contextual cues established during the training could be observed in the test session in which the color cues were not presented.

General discussion

The present study investigated the interference effects between two orientations that were trained in the same detection task in two adjacent days. The results showed that training on the second orientation impaired the training effect of the first orientation, demonstrating a retroactive interference effect (Experiment 1 and Experiment 2). This effect could not be explained by the reactivation account and competing neuronal population account, as were evident in Experiment 3 and Experiment 4, respectively. Finally, associating the two orientations with two contextual color cues during the training prevented the emergence of the retroactive interference effect, providing the supporting evidence for the shared context account (Experiment 4 and Experiment 5). These findings suggest that multiple short-term perceptual trainings could suffer from retroactive interference, which is likely the result of higher-level factors such as shared contextual cues embedded in the tasks. To facilitate the efficiency of multiple trainings, associating the trained tasks with different contextual cues could be a solution in practice.

Perceptual learning has been argued to arise from plastic changes in primary sensory areas (Ahissar & Hochstein, 1997; Bao et al., 2010; Jehee et al., 2012; Schoups et al., 1995; Schoups et al., 2001; Schwartz et al., 2002; Yan et al., 2014). The training protocol adopted in the present study, despite being short term, nevertheless resulted in behavioral improvement on the trained task after one day’s consolidation. The retroactive interference effect observed in Experiment 2 where the two orientations were trained at the same retinal location could be attributed to a shared pool of neurons in primary visual cortex that responded to both orientations (Seitz et al., 2005). However, this interpretation was not supported by the absence of a retroactive interference effect in Experiment 4 in which the two orientations were in the same retinal location but associated with different colors during the training and test. These results supported another interpretation that the shared contextual cues between the two orientations might be a critical factor.

Nevertheless, our conclusion does not imply that no plastic change occurred in the primary visual cortex. In fact, previous studies have demonstrated that, using a perceptual training paradigm with Gabor stimuli, the training effect did not transfer much to a new orientation if it was at the same retinal location as the trained orientation (Dosher et al., 2013). The orientation specificity of the training effect suggests that training of the two orientations may not recruit the overlapping population of neurons in the primary visual cortex, further excluding the competing neuronal population account for the interpretation of our results. It was more likely that training of the two orientations in Experiment 2 involved two populations of orientation-selective neurons at the primary visual cortex, but established shared associations between the orientations and contextual cues (e.g., training protocol or task set). If performing the trained task initiated a retrieval process that depended on the orientation-context association, the retrieval of the perceptual memory would have been disrupted by the shared context between the two orientations. By introducing orientation-color associations in Experiment 4 and Experiment 5, the two orientations were associated with different contextual cues and the reliable retrieval of their learning traces became possible. The recovery from retroactive interference by the orientation-color associations provided us with a new insight into the retrieval of learned perceptual skills. In these two experiments, the participants were not informed about the meaning of the color cues. They just passively perceived the cues when they performed the orientation detection tasks. Our results suggest that the contextual cue could serve as a critical factor for the successful retrieval of multiple perceptual learning traces. The literature in memory research suggested that the beneficial role of contextual cues could be achieved by facilitating a hippocampus-dependent pattern separation during the training phase as the hippocampus is known to play an important role in forming distinct memory traces for similar experiences (O'Reilly & Rudy, 2001). Particularly, the interaction between hippocampus and prefrontal cortex was suggested to support memory encoding and context-dependent memory retrieval (Preston & Eichenbaum, 2013). Under this framework, prefrontal cortex serves to develop different rules based on the contextual information from the hippocampus through learning and guides the retrieval of appropriate memory representations according to the rules. Indeed, prefrontal cortex has been suggested to mediate the decision process in perceptual learning (Kahnt et al., 2011). However, how the hippocampus-dependent mechanism is realized in perceptual learning remains an important issue to be further explored. Nevertheless, the effect of contextual cues during the training could be observed in the situation in which the color cues were not presented, demonstrating the robustness of the effect in the present study. We suggest that future investigations with neuroscientific approaches are required for revealing a full picture of the interaction between memory retrieval and sensory plasticity in perceptual learning.

There were a few studies in the literature that investigated the interferences between multiple perceptual trainings. In most of these studies, interferences were observed when the second training occurred immediately after or was separated by up to a few hours from the first training (Been et al., 2011; Seitz et al., 2005; Shibata et al., 2017; Yotsumoto, Chang, et al., 2009a). That is, the two trainings were conducted on the same day and the within-day switchover of the two trainings lasted for several days. Seitz et al. (2005) has demonstrated with a vernier task that multiple perceptual trainings could induce feature- and location-specific interference when the second training started immediately after the first training. However, the interference was not evident if the two trainings were separated by 1 h, suggesting that well-consolidated memory could prevent the interference from later training. In another study that adopted Gabor stimuli with an orientation discrimination task, Been et al. (2011) also revealed retroactive interferences in conditions in which the intervals between the two trainings varied from 0 to 24 h. Their results suggested that the overlap between neuronal populations of the two trained orientations was responsible for the interference. No evidence for the role of memory consolidation was found in this study.

In contrast to the findings in Seitz et al. (2005) and Been et al. (2011), we did not find evidence for the perceptual interpretation of the interference effects. There are three possible reasons for the different findings. First, as mentioned above, the first orientation was well consolidated with an overnight sleep before the second training in the present study, whereas the two trainings were separated up to a few hours in these two studies (except the 24-h condition in Been et al., 2011). Sleep-related consolidation is known to modulate perceptual learning (Tamaki et al., 2020; Yotsumoto, Sasaki, et al., 2009b) and may influence the interference mechanism of perceptual memory. One possibility is that, in the present study, sleep facilitated the process of system consolidation and transformed the perceptual learning traces into a more abstract representation that was beyond perceptual processing (Diekelmann & Born, 2010; Wang et al., 2016). Second, an outstanding difference between our study and these two studies was the task used for training. Previous literature has shown that learning to detect stimuli from a noisy background (e.g., orientation detection task) utilized a different mechanism from learning a fine discrimination task (e.g., vernier task and orientation discrimination task; Chang et al., 2014; Dosher & Lu, 2005; Yang et al., 2014). The different mechanisms of the two types of tasks could contribute to different levels of competition between neuronal populations recruited by two trainings. This possibility could be examined by future investigations with neuroscience approaches. Third, we adopted a short training approach in which each orientation was trained for 1 day, whereas each training was repeated for 5 (Seitz et al., 2005) and 15 (Been et al., 2011) days in these two studies. It is known that extensive training results in precise discrimination and involves lower-level processing (Ahissar & Hochstein, 1997; Hochstein & Ahissar, 2002). Thus, it is reasonable to suggest that the level at which the interference occurred was related to the level of training-related processing. As a result, we suggest that the interference induced by multiple perceptual trainings could be modulated by low-level perceptual processing or higher-level mnemonic mechanisms, given that different tasks and training protocols are adopted.

In a study that investigated the reconsolidation effect in perceptual learning, Bang et al. (2018b) adopted a Gabor orientation detection task as in the present study. Particularly, the long interval condition in their Study 1 was similar to our Experiment 2 except that there was a test for the first orientation 3.5 h before the pre-test of the second orientation in day 2. However, unlike our results, which showed a significant retroactive interference effect, their results revealed an intact training effect for the first orientation in day 3 (in their Fig. 1c). As suggested by their findings, the test of the first orientation in day 2 induced reactivation and reconsolidation processes. It has long been known that retrieval practice (i.e., testing) can facilitate the long-term retention of memory (Roediger & Karpicke, 2006). Therefore, the test of the first orientation on day 2 in their study would strengthen its original training effect. As suggested by another study of the same group (Shibata et al., 2017), the strengthened learning could stabilize the perceptual training effect and protect it from retroactive interference of a new learning (also see Potts & Shanks, 2012, for the case in associative learning). In our Experiment 2, there was no test for ori_1 in day 2 and this may explain the difference noted between the two studies. Future investigations are required to further elucidate the relationship between memory reconsolidation and perceptual training interference.

We also examined the possible proactive interference from the training of the first orientation to the training of the second orientation. We compared the MPIs of the second orientation in other experiments with the MPI in Experiment 1. Apart from a trend of significance in comparing Experiment 2 and Experiment 1, we did not find any other significant difference. We noted that the post-test in Experiment 1 was 2 days after the pre-test and training, and this gap in the other three experiments was 1 day. A previous study has suggested that proactive interference was more likely to happen if there was overlearning for the first trained task (Shibata et al., 2017). In the present study, two trainings were both short-term and equalized in terms of training length. Therefore, we suggest that there was only weak, if not no, proactive interference the present study.

Roving as a training method for perceptual learning has been extensively studied in the literature (Cong & Zhang, 2014; Dosher et al., 2020; Kuai et al., 2005; Parkosadze et al., 2008; Tartaglia et al., 2009; Yu et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2008). In contrast to the present study, in a roving experiment, trials of different conditions were randomly interleaved during the training, preventing subjects from learning the trained feature due to the interference between trials. Interestingly, Zhang et al. (2008) conducted a contrast discrimination experiment in which four roving contrasts (i.e., four stimulus conditions) were each assigned a tag of letter. They found that the tagging prevented learning from the disruption of roving and suggested that semantic tags could facilitate top-down attention during training. The contextual color cueing in our Experiment 4 shared a similar associative idea with the tagging manipulation in Zhang et al. (2008), but focused more on the retrieval process underlying the interference effects in perceptual training. The results of both studies suggested the importance of mnemonic mechanisms in understanding perceptual training.

In summary, we demonstrated that two short-term perceptual trainings suffered from retroactive interference due to a shared context related to training protocol or task set. Introducing color-based contextual cues could establish separate contexts for the two trainings and prevent the occurrence of retroactive interference. A hippocampus-dependent pattern of separation is likely to play a critical role in this process, but this idea needs future neuroscientific investigations to confirm it. Overall, our findings emphasize the roles of mnemonic mechanisms in fully understanding the phenomenon of perceptual learning.