Deciding what to do: Observations from a psycho-motor laboratory, including the discovery of pre-crastination

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2022.104658Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Pre-crastination is the tendency to hasten tasks even at the expense of extra energy.

  • It is manifested in a vast range of behaviors, such as answering emails too soon, or submitting articles before they are ready.

  • More significant examples are convicting others quickly to get cases over and done with or going to war prematurely.

  • Pre-crastination was discovered in a study of psycho-motor performance. The focus of that study was biomechanical factors affecting task choices.

  • Psycho-motor research brings up principles of behavioral choice which help make sense of pre-crastination even though the discovery of pre-crastination was unexpected at first.

  • Pre-crastination appears to be due mainly to the desire to reduce the load on working memory, to keep one’s mental to-do-list as short as possible.

Abstract

A great deal of research has concerned choices of goods or services with different values receivable at various times. Temporal discounting – the magnification of values that can be obtained sooner rather than later – has proven to be immensely important in this regard. In the present article, we shift the focus from the receipt of goods or services to the performance of tasks. We show that temporal discounting also applies to task choices. Pre-crastination, the phenomenon we point to, was discovered by Rosenbaum, Gong, and Potts (2014) and is the tendency to hasten tasks even at the expense of extra energy. Pre-crastination was discovered in a study of psycho-motor performance, where the focus was on biomechanical factors affecting task choices. In the present article, we review that research, showing how the tendency found in the initial experiments are in fact illustrative of a more general motor-control tendency to inhibit easy forms of movement for the sake of later performance goals. Such inhibitory control may also be the basis for pre-crastination, provided one assumes that pre-crastination keeps working memory as clear as possible. A wide range of behavioral choices fit under the rubric of pre-crastination, such as answering emails too soon, submitting articles before they are ready, judging others before they should be judged, convicting others to get cases over with, and, in the worst case, going to war prematurely. Lack of temperance in these choices may seem to arise from impulsivity, but we argue against that view. The desire to “clear the decks” to be prepared for new challenges is, we suggest, a more apt account of pre-crastination.

Introduction

Charles Darwin showed that a simple mechanism can explain the proliferation of species: Species that pass on traits that happen to be adaptive are more likely to survive than species that pass on traits that happen to be less adaptive. B. F. Skinner extended this idea to shorter time scales. He showed that behaviors with adaptive features are more likely to recur than behaviors with less adaptive features.

Both in the Skinnerian short-term and in the Darwinian long-term, behaviors must be chosen. Consequences of those behaviors can come immediately or be delayed within and over lifetimes. Given the centrality of behavioral choice in both contexts, researchers who hope to understand the processes at play need to understand how behaviors are chosen. What, indeed, is the nature of those behavioral (or behavioural) processes?

It is common in studies of short-term behavioral choice to define distinct behavioral outcomes and then assess their probabilities given various reinforcement contingencies. The most familiar example is pressing one lever or another in a Skinner box. The actual implementation of behavior is seldom all-or-none, however. From the perspective of neuromuscular control, an immense number of variables must be specified by the neuro-motor system. A behavior may be defined externally with respect to some coding or categorization scheme such as “press lever A” or “press lever B,” but the act itself, carried out neuro-muscularly, has no a priori categorical identity. Giving a label to a behavior – calling the behavior A or B – is a descriptive or prescriptive overlay at best, a mental construct, as it were. Skinner knew this, of course, which is why he said that he studied instrumental or operant conditioning. Important to Skinner was the instrumental effect of the neuromuscular activity on the environment (how physical acts operated on the world), not the details of the movements themselves.

It does not follow from this perspective, however, that bodily movements should not been analyzed, and no one, Skinner least of all, ever claimed otherwise. The question, if one cares about bodily expression itself, is what to look for. What aspects of bodily behavior should be considered?

Consider the simple act of choosing one of two buckets to be carried to a location to get a reward. If different rewards are given for the two buckets, the bucket that will be chosen will probably be the one that brings the greater reward. If a huge financial reward is offered to carry a very heavy bucket over a long distance and a small financial reward is offered to carry a very light bucket over a short distance, there is a good chance the heavy-bucket-long-distance behavior will be favored. One could vary the distances, bucket weights, and rewards to arrive at a recipe for predicting which bucket will be chosen when. Still, one’s description of the choice would be incomplete, for a wildcard would beset the enterprise. Beneath the choice would be the actor’s sense of the physical effort to be expended. But this term effort … what is that exactly? Its nature, both physical and mental, would remain elusive. How could it be measured? How could the existence and identity of effort be nailed down, except circularly – that is, as whatever it is that somehow causes some behaviors to require much reward to appear, or heaps of punishment to remain on the sidelines?

Section snippets

Motor Control

The field where this topic often comes up is motor control. Motor control is the area of study concerned with the means by which the body moves and stays still, typically via neural and muscular activity.

The study of motor control is appealing both from an applied- and basic-science perspective. On the applied side, controlling body movement and stability is crucial in all we do – at home, in the workplace, in transportation, in educational and medical settings, in stores, in recreation, and so

Point-to-point movements

Informal observation of everyday physical performance suggests principles that may play a role in behavioral choice. When people make simple point-to-point movements – touching one target and then another – they rarely move as quickly as possible, nor do they always take the shortest path. Point-to-point movements are generally made at intermediate speeds with path lengths and curvatures that permit obstacle avoidance (Rosenbaum et al., 2001, Vaughan et al., 2001). More subtly, point-to-point

Principles of object manipulation: the end-state comfort effect

The point of the preceding remarks is that the neuromuscular implementation of behavior, even for simple point-to-point movements, is nontrivial. Thus behavioral choices, which surely take into account the ease or difficulty of needed behaviors, may be hard to predict in detail. More macroscopic features of motor control may lead to more encouraging outcomes. In that connection, and to suggest a more hopeful path, we next review some findings from our lab. These findings center on more

Principles of object manipulation: the grasp-height effect

Another phenomenon that reflects the same general principle as the end-state comfort effect relates to reaching high or low. As prelude to this study, consider that one day, the first author happened to reach for and grasp a bathroom plunger standing on his bathroom counter. His intent was to place the plunger on the floor. He noticed that he grasped the plunger high, which made sense because the plunger was destined for a low location. Had he grasped the plunger midway along its shaft or

Reaching and walking

The activities described above entailed reaching for and moving objects from one place to another while standing or sitting still; walking was not involved. But object manipulation does not only occur when one remains in place. Objects are often taken hold of and carried elsewhere, as in picking up an item from a grocery shelf and carrying it to the checkout counter. This observation set the stage for testing the generality of the hypothesis that avoidance of extreme final positions is an

Pre-crastination: the first study

In the studies reviewed in the last section, people chose which walking path to pursue to take hold of one object, a bucket. In the studies reviewed next, participants chose between two objects, taking into account the locations of the objects relative to where they needed to go. Another factor was the weight of the objects. The overarching question was which one of the two objects people who select. Although the methodological change was minor, the discovery that popped out from it was far

Pre-crastination named and followed up

When Rosenbaum, Gong, and Potts (before publication) prepared their paper for submission to Psychological Science, they realized that the behavioral tendency they had unearthed was, to a first approximation, the opposite of procrastination. Instead of putting off what had to be done, participants in their experiments had launched headlong into action as soon as possible, expending extra energy to do so.

Curious to know whether there was a word for the opposite of procrastination, Rosenbaum,

Pre-crastination in pigeons

Given the wide swath of activities where pre-crastination applies, it could be that doing things sooner rather than later is a fundamental bias of living creatures. The idea that it is was advocated by Edward Wasserman (2018), who called the tendency to pre-crastinate the “fierce urgency of now,” borrowing that phrase from Martin Luther King Jr., who used it in his famous speech at the 1963 March on Washington for Jobs and Freedom. Drawing on Dr. King’s phrase, Wasserman suggested that, all

Mere urgency

Pursuing the possibility that pre-crastination is widespread, one could imagine that it might even turn out that quick turn-around times are attractive in and of themselves. Acting quickly could bring its own reward, so to speak, apart from whatever goods are procured.

As counter-intuitive as this possibility may be, it turns out that people do in fact like short deadlines per se. (The hypothesis has not yet been tested with non-human animals, as far as we know.) Zhu et al. (2018) documented

Quelling anxiety, inhibiting actions

Could it be that pre-crastination is a way of relieving anxiety? We take up that question and another in this brief section. The other question is whether pre-crastination simply reflects a problem with inhibiting actions?

Some have suggested that anxiety is the cause of pre-crastination. The suggestion has been made in personal communications to us. Although we understand the sentiment, for we, like others, have gotten nervous on occasion when we have had too much to do in too little time, we

Reducing the load on working memory

The foregoing remarks about anxiety and inhibition – two hypotheses we disfavor – set the stage for presentation of the hypothesis we do. The primary basis for pre-crastination in our view is the desire to reduce the load on working memory. Our reasons are fivefold.

First, because doing things reduces the number of things to be done, getting things done sooner reduces the amount of time those duties occupy one’s mind.

Second, having a mental to-do list is taxing, as shown in research by McDaniel

Final remarks

In this article we have discussed a series of studies that began in one field and ended in another. The field in which we started was motor control, a relatively little-studied area of psychology whose neglect is surprising considering that psychology is the science of mental life and behavior. One would think that such a field would take as one of its main missions the analysis of the translation of mental life into behavior. A variety of historical factors account for this neglect (Heuer, 2003

Acknowledgments

The research described here was supported by grants from the National Science Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, the Dutch National Science Foundation, the German National Science Foundation, the John Simon Guggenheim Foundation, and the Academic Senate of the University of California, Riverside. Jarrod Blinch, Suzanne Mitchell, and an anonymous reviewer provided helpful comments on the original manuscript.

References (64)

  • F. Bartlett

    Remembering: An Experimental and Social Study

    (1932)
  • Belluck, P. (July 16, 2012). Footprints to cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s are seen in gait. New York...
  • E. Burdet et al.

    The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance

    Nature

    (2001)
  • Burkeman, O. (July 5, 2014). This column will change your life: Precrastination. The Guardian....
  • Cappelletto, J. A. (2019). Mediators of decision making in action planning: Assessing the functional costs of...
  • R.G. Cohen et al.

    Where objects are grasped reveals how grasps are planned: generation and recall of motor plans

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2004)
  • J. DeMelo

    Precrastination: when the early bird gets the shaft

    New York Times Smart Living Sec.

    (2019)
  • D.C. Dennett et al.

    Time and the observer: the where and when of consciousness in the brain

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1992)
  • R. Draper

    To start a war: How the Bush administration took America to Iraq

    (2020)
  • I. Feghhi et al.

    Towards a common code for difficulty: navigating a narrow gap is like memorizing an extra digit

    Atten. Percept. Psychophys.

    (2021)
  • L.R. Fournier et al.

    Which task will we choose first? precrastination and cognitive load in task ordering

    Attent. Percept. Psychophys.

    (2018)
  • L.R. Fournier et al.

    Starting or finishing sooner? sequencing preferences in object transfer tasks

    Psychol. Res.

    (2019)
  • P. Haggard et al.

    Spatial patterns in the control of human arm movements

    J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform.

    (1996)
  • O. Herbort et al.

    Habitual and goal-directed factors in (everyday) object handling

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2011)
  • Heuer, H. (2003). Motor control. In A. Healy & R. Proctor (Eds.), Handbook of Psychology: Volume 4 (Experimental...
  • N. Hogan

    An organizing principle for a class of voluntary movements

    J. Neurosci.

    (1984)
  • N.H. Hunt et al.

    Acrobatic squirrels learn to leap and land on tree branches without falling

    Science

    (2021)
  • E.F. Loftus

    The malleability of human memory

    Am. Sci.

    (1979)
  • D.M. McBride et al.

    Precrastination in cognitive tasks

    Curr. Psychol.

    (2022)
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2007). Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Thousand...
  • M. Meyer et al.

    Higher-order action planning for individual and joint object manipulations

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (2013)
  • S.H. Mitchell

    Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers

    Psychopharmacology

    (1999)
  • Cited by (0)

    Invited revision for Behavioural Processes

    View full text