Skip to main content
Log in

Chess Instruction Improves Cognitive Abilities and Academic Performance: Real Effects or Wishful Thinking?

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In accordance with the outcomes from a number of reports, there are cognitive and academic improvements derived from chess learning and chess playing. This evidence, however, endures three key limitations: (a) ignoring theoretical premises about the concept of transfer, (b) several shortcomings regarding ideal experiment guidelines, and (c) an uncritical faith in null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) statistical analyses. The present review scrutinized the NHST outcomes from 45 studies describing chess instruction interventions (n = 12,705) in nineteen countries that targeted cognitive ability (100 tests) and academic performance (108 tests), with a mean Hedge’s effect size g = 572 (95% CI = [0.127, 1.062]). There was a lower average statistical power, a higher proportion of false positive outcomes, larger publication biases, and lower replication rates for the studies in the academic performance domain than in the cognitive ability domain. These findings raised reasonable concerns over the evidence about the benefits of chess instruction, which was particularly problematic regarding academic achievement outcomes. Chess should perhaps be regularly taught, however, regardless of whether it has a direct impact or not in cognitive abilities and academic performance, because these are far transfer targets. The more likely impact of chess on near transfer outcomes from higher quality studies remains at present unexplored.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Data is available in the supplemental material.

Code Availability

Data and software code are available in Appendix 1 and 2.

References

  • Aciego, R., García, L., & Betancort, M. (2012). The benefits of chess for the intellectual and social-emotional enrichment in schoolchildren. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(2), 551–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atashafrouz, A. (2019). The effectiveness of chess on problem-solving, working memory, and concentration of male high school students. Iranian Evolutionary and Educational Psychology Journal, 249-258. https://doi.org/10.29252/ieepj.1.4.2

  • Aydin, M. (2015). Examining the impact of chess instruction for the visual impairment on mathematics. Educational Research and Reviews, 10(7), 907–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, D. C., & Fish, W. W. (2011). Our move: Using chess to improve math achievement for students who receive special education services. International Journal of Special Education, 26(3), 181–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bart, W. M. (2014). On the effect of chess training on scholastic achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartoš, F., & Schimmack, U. (2020a). Z-curve.2.0: Estimating Replication rates and discovery rates. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/urgtn.

  • Bartoš, F., & Schimmack, U. (2020b). zcurve: An R package for fitting z-curves. R package version 1.0.0, URL: https://github.com/FBartos/zcurve. Accessed 22 Feb 2020.

  • Basson, M. R. (2015). The effects of chess instruction on the intellectual development of grade R leaners. University of South Africa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beier, M. E., Campbell, M., & Crook, A. E. (2010). Developing and demonstrating knowledge: Ability and non-ability determinants of learning and performance. Intelligence, 38(1), 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, R. (2010). Cognitively based assessment of, for, and as learning: A preliminary theory of action for summative and formative assessment. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives, 8(2-3), 70–91.

  • Berkley, D. K. (2012). The impact of chess instruction on the critical thinking ability and mathematical achievement of developmental mathematics students. ProQuest LLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilalic, M., Smallbone, K., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2009). Why are (the best) women so good at chess? Participation rates and gender differences in intellectual domains. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 1161–1165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A. (2015). Evaluating fluid and crystallized abilities in the performance of an educational process. Instructional Science, 43, 427–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A. (2016). Expert performance of men and women: A cross-cultural study in the chess domain. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 90–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A. (2018). Top hundred chess experts: A cross-domain analysis of change over time. Intelligence, 71, 76–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A. (2021). Chess and individual differences. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A., & Aluja, A. (2013). A regression tree of the aptitudes, personality, and academic performance relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(6), 703–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A., Aluja, A., & Cornadó, M. P. (2015). Sex differences in chess performance: Analyzing participation rates, age, and practice in chess tournaments. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 117–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanch, A., & Llaveria, A. (2021). Ability and non-ability traits in chess skill. Personality and Individual Differences, 179, 110909.

  • Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. John Wiley & Sons.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brunner, J., & Schimmack, U. (2020). Estimating population mean power under conditions of heterogeneity and selection for significance. Meta-Psychology, 4, https://doi.org/10.15626/MP.1012018.1015874.

  • Burgoyne, A. P., Sala, G., Gobet, F., Macnamara, B. N., Campitelli, G., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2016). The relationship between cognitive ability and chess skill: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Intelligence, 59, 72–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buyk, J., & Ho, F. (2008). The effect of math and chess integrated instruction on math scores. The Chess Academy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campitelli, G., & Gobet, F. (2008). The role of practice in chess: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 18(4), 446–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christiaen, J., & Verhofstadt-Denève, L. (1981). Schaken en cognitieve ontwikkeling. Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en haar Grensgebieden, 36, 561–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleveland, A. (1907). The psychology of chess and of learning to play it. American Journal of Psychology, 18(3), 269–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. Lawerence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Steiner, P. M. (2010). Case matching and the reduction of selection bias in quasi-experiments: The relative importance of pretest measures of outcome, of unreliable measurement, and of mode of data analysis. Psychological Methods, 15(1), 56–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello, P. J. M. (2013). The gymnasium of the mind: Teaching chess in early childhood. Early Child Development and Care, 183(8), 1133–1146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Groot, A. D. (1965). Thought and choice in chess (2nd ed.). Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deary, I. J., Strand, S., Smith, P., & Fernandes, C. (2007). Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence, 35(1), 13–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DuCette, J. (2009). An evaluation of the Chess Challenge Program of ASAP/after school activities partnerships. Philadelphia, PA. https://www.saintlouischessclub.org/sites/default/files/DUCETTE_2009_EVALUATION.pdf. Accessed 28 Nov 2018.

  • Eberhard, J. W. (2003). The relationship between classroom chess instruction and verbal, quantitative, and nonverbal reasoning abilities of economically disadvantaged students. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Texas A&M University, College Station.

  • Ferguson, G. A. (1954). On learning and human ability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 95–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, G. A. (1956). On transfer and the abilities of man. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 121–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Amigo, J. (2008). Utilización de material didáctico con recursos de ajedrez para la enseñanza de las matemáticas. Estudio de sus efectos sobre una muestra de alumnos de segundo de primaria. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona.

  • Ferreira, D., & Palhares, P. (2008). Chess and problem solving involving patterns. The Mathematics Enthusiast, 5(2), 249–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrero, M., Vadillo, M. A., & León, S. P. (2021). A valid evaluation of the theory of multiple intelligences is not yet possible: Problems of methodological quality for intervention studies. Intelligence, 88, 101566.

  • Forrest, D., Davidson, D., Shucksmith, J., & Glendinning, T. (2005). Chess development in Aberdeen’s primary schools: A study of literacy and social capital. Aberdeen City Council. University of Aberdeen. Scottish Executive Education Department.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gao, Q., Feng, Y., Chen, W., & Ping, X. (2021). Does perceived chess skills mediate the relationship between fluid intelligence and academic performance? Journal of Psychology in Africa, 31(1), 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, N. V. (2008). Scholastic chess club participation and the academic achievement of hispanic fifth grade students in south Texas. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, University of Houston, Houston.

  • Gliga, F., & Flesner, P. I. (2014). Cognitive benefits of chess training in novice children. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 116, 962–967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gobet, F., & Campitelli, G. (2006). Educational benefits of chess instruction: A critical review. In T. Redman (Ed.), Chess and education: Selected essays from the Koltanowski conference. Chess Program at the University of Texas at Dallas.

  • Grau-Pérez, G., & Moreira, K. (2017). A study of the influence of chess on the executive functions in school-aged children. Estudios de Psicología, 38(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Häggström, O. (2017). The need for nuance in the null hypothesis significance testing debate. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 77(4), 616–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haier, R. J., Jung, R. E., Yeo, R. A., Head, K., & Alkire, M. T. (2005). The neuroanatomy of general intelligence: Sex matters. Neuroimage, 25, 320–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2010). Multivariate data analysis. Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F., & LaMay, M. L. (2000). The smarter sex: A critical review of sex differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, L. S., Stecher, B. M., & Klein, S. P. (2002). Making sense of test-based accountability in education. RAND.

  • Hong, S., & Bart, W. M. (2007). Cognitive effects of chess instruction on students at risk for academic failure. International Journal of Special Education, 22(3), 89–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horgan, D. D., & Morgan, D. (1990). Chess expertise in children. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 4(1), 109–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howard, R. W. (2014). Explaining male predominance at the apex of intellectual achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 68, 217–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isikgöz, E. (2016). Analysis of math success of secondary school students playing and not playing chess (Sakarya province sample). International Journal of Human Sciences, 13(1), 1689–1699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Islam, A., Lee, W. S., & Nicholas, A. (2021). The effects of chess instruction on academic and non-cognitive outcomes: Field experimental evidence from a developing country. Journal of Development Economics, 150, 102615.

  • Jerrim, J., Macmillan, L., Micklewright, J., Sawtell, M., & Wiggins, M. (2018). Does teaching children how to play cognitively demanding games improve their educational attainment? Evidence from a Randomised Controlled Trial of chess instruction in England. Journal of Human Resources, 53(4), 993–1021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, E., Easvaradoss, V., Abraham, S., & Chan, M. A. (2017). Mentoring children through chess training enhances cognitive functions. International Journal of Information and Education Technology, 7(9), 669–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, E., Easvaradoss, V., Abraham, S., & Jain, S. (2018). Enhancing verbal reasoning of school children through chess learning. International Journal of Scientific Engineering and Science, 2, 7–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, E., Easvaradoss, V., & Solomon, N. J. (2016). Impact of chess training on academic performance of rural Indian school children. Open Journal of Social Sciences, 4, 20–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kazemi, F., Yektayar, M., & Abad, A. M. B. (2012). Investigation the impact of chess play on developing meta-cognitive ability and math problem-solving power of students at different levels of education. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 32, 372–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Khosrorad, R., Kouhbanani, S. S., & Sani, A. R. (2014). Effectiveness of chess training for improving executive functions and mathematics performance of students with mathematics disorders. International Journal of Educational Investigations, 1(1), 283–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laidra, K., Pullmann, H., & Allik, J. (2007). Personality and intelligence as predictors of academic achievement: A cross-sectional study from elementary to secondary school. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 441–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liptrap, J. M. (1998). Chess and standard test scores. Chess Life, 3, 41–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margulies, S. (1992). The effect of chess on reading scores: District nine chess program second year report. The American Chess Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mel, B. N. (2021). Does playing chess improve mathematics scores? An experimental study among co-curricular chess students in Politeknic Kuching Sarawak. International Journal of Advanced Research in Education and Society, 13(2), 9–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meloni, C., & Fanari, R. (2019). Chess training effect on meta-cognitive processes and academic performance. Paper presented at the 16th international Conference on Cognition and Exploratory Learning in Digital Age. https://doi.org/10.33965/celda2019_201911L048.

  • Miller, J. (2017). Hypothesis testing in the real world. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 77(4), 663–672.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & The PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med, 6(7), e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097.

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, aac4716.

  • Petrill, S. A., & Wilkerson, B. (2000). Intelligence and achievement: A behavioral genetic perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietschnig, J., Voracek, M., & Forman, A. K. (2010). Mozart effect-Shmozart effect: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 38(3), 314–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plucker, J. A., & Makel, M. C. (2021). Replication is important for educational psychology: Recent developments and key issues. Educational Psychologist, 56(2), 90–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramos, L., Arán, V., & Krumm, G. (2018). Funciones ejecutivas y práctica de ajedrez: Un estudio en niños escolarizados. Psicogente, 21(39), 25–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science achievement: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 645–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rifner, P. J. (1992). Playing chess: A study of the transfer of problem-solving skills in students with average and above average intelligence. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Purdue University, West Lafayette.

  • Romano, B. (2011). Does playing chess improve math learning? Promising (and inexpensive) results from Italy. University of Pennsylvania.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosholm, M., Mikkelsen, M. B., & Gumede, K. (2017). Your move: The effect of chess on mathematic test scores. PLoS ONE, 12(5), e0177257.

  • Sala, G., Foley, J. P., & Gobet, F. (2017). The effects of chess instruction on pupils’ cognitive and academic skills: State of the art and theoretical challenges. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2016). Do the benefits of chess instruction transfer to academic and cognitive skills? A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 18, 16–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does chess instruction improve mathematical problem-solving ability? Two experimental studies with an active control group. Learning & Behavior, 45, 414–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sala, G., Gobet, F., Trinchero, R., & Ventura, S. (2016). Does chess instruction enhance mathematical ability in children? A three-group design to control for placebo effects. Paper presented at the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, PA.

  • Sala, G., Gorini, A., & Pravettoni, G. (2015). Mathematical problem-solving abilities and chess: An experimental study on young pupils. Sage Open, 5, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of gentoype®environment effects. Child Development, 54(2), 424–435.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. (2012). The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles. Psychological Methods, 17(4), 551–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlotter, M., Schwerdt, G., & Woessmann, L. (2011). Econometric methods for causal evaluation of education policies and practices: A non-technical guide. Education Economics, 19(2), 109–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholz, M., Niesch, H., Steffen, O., Ernst, B., Loeffler, M., Witruk, E., et al. (2008). Impact of chess training on mathematics performance and concentration ability of children with learning disabilities. International Journal of Special Education, 23(3), 138–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigirtmac, A. D. (2012). Does chess training affect conceptual development of six-year-old children in Turkey? Early Child Development and Care, 182, 797–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigirtmac, A. D. (2016). An investigation on the effectiveness of chess training on creativity and theory of mind development at early childhood. Educational Research and Reviews, 11(11), 1056–1063.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonsohn, U., Nelson, L. D., & Simmons, J. P. (2014). P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 534–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. P. (1998). A quantitative analysis of the effects of chess instruction on the mathematics achievement of southern, rural, black secondary students. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Louisiana Tech University.

  • Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. F. (1984). Toward a theory of cognitive aptitude for learning from instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(3), 347–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, R. E., & Swanson, J. (1992). Instructional psychology: aptitude, adaptation, and assessment. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 583–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stegariu, V., Popusoi, S. A., Abalasei, B., Voinea, N. L., Stelescu, I., Trofin, F. P., et al. (2019). The role of chess in the intellectual development of childrens from primary school. Sport and Society, 19(2), 58–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Raising the achievement of all students: Teaching for successful intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 14(4), 383–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Theme-park psychology: A case study regarding human intelligence and its implications for education. Educational Psychology Review, 12(2), 247–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storey, K. (2000). Teaching beginning chess skills to students with disabilities. Preventing School Failure, 44(2), 45–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, S., Pan, W., & Wang, L. L. (2010). A comprehensive review of effect size reporting and interpreting practices in academic journals in education and psychology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(4), 989–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomasina, C. P., & Adams, M. S. (2012). Chess from square a1: Incorporating chess into the gifted class. Gifted Child Today, 35(4), 243–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, M. (2003). Does the playing of chess lead to improved scholastic achievement. Issues in Educational Research, 13(2), 13–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions (I.). Psychological Review, 9, 247–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 265–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trinchero, R., & Sala, G. (2016). Chess training and mathematical problem solving: The role of teaching heuristics in transfer of learning. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 12(3), 655–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2011). Individual differences methods for randomized experiments. Psychological Methods, 16(3), 298–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vacha-Haase, T. (2001). Statistical significance should not be considered one of life’s guarantees: Effect sizes are needed. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61(2), 219–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velea, T., & Cojocaru, V. (2019). The effect of playing chess on focused attention. European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.12019.15402.15484

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Rousseau, M., Rajotte, T., Freiman, V., & Cabot-Thibault, J. (2018). Le jeu d’échecs dans les classes du primaire :un moyen ciblé pour développer les habiletés des garçons en résolution de problèmes. Formation et Profession, 26(3), 94–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., & Voyer, S. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 1174–1204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waterhouse, L. (2006). Inadequate evidence for multiple intelligences, mozart effect, and emotional intelligence theories. Educational Psychologist, 41(4), 247–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, T., Bailey, D. H., & Li, C. (2019). Aiming further: Addressing the need for high-quality longitudinal research in education. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 12(4), 648–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yap, K. O. (2006). Chess for success evaluation. Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory.

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was done with the support of the County Council of Lleida (Diputació de Lleida), Grant ID: 202100063). This research was performed within the Catalonian Consolidated Research Group SGR 00008. 2017–2019 period. The comments and suggestions from four anonymous reviewers were very much appreciated.

Funding

Diputació de Lleida (Grant ID: 202100063).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Not applicable

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Angel Blanch.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

The study did not require approval from ethics comittee.

Consent to Participate

Not applicable.

Consent for Publication

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

There are no conflicting/competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix A

Appendix A

Tables

Table 5 Input data corresponding to the p values of 100 tests of cognitive ability, and 108 tests of academic performance.

5,

Table 6 R code used to estimate the z-curves in cognitive ability and academic performance tests.

6

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Blanch, A. Chess Instruction Improves Cognitive Abilities and Academic Performance: Real Effects or Wishful Thinking?. Educ Psychol Rev 34, 1371–1398 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-022-09670-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-022-09670-9

Keywords

Navigation