Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T06:40:27.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Recent developments of the pragmatic markers kind of and sort of in spoken British English

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2021

SUSAN REICHELT*
Affiliation:
Advanced Data and Information Literacy Track, Fachgruppe Medienwissenschaft, University of Konstanz, Postfach 157, Universitätsstr. 10, 78457 Konstanz Germany susan.reichelt@uni-konstanz.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study reports on recent changes in the use of the hedges kind of and sort of in spoken British English over the past twenty years. A quantitative analysis of these features within subsets of the original BNC 1994 (BNC Consortium 2007) and BNC 2014 (Love et al. 2017) suggests a systematic encroaching of kind of into contexts that are traditionally occupied by sort of. This is highlighted in apparent-time patterns in which younger speakers are leading in use as well as real-time patterns that show a significant increase in use between 1994 and 2014.

The hedges sort of and kind of are often treated as semantically equivalent, yet show distributional differences across different varieties of English. This article reports on an ongoing shift in the use of kind of as well as a relatively stable use of sort of. Its main focus is a detailed sociolinguistic analysis of both variants, which, in addition to social factors involved, teases apart some of the linguistic aspects of this shift.

In line with the theme of this special issue, the article draws attention to the usefulness of comparable, or comparably made, corpora that allow for focused studies of linguistic change across speakers, generations, registers and communities.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

1 Introduction

This study investigates recent changes in the use of the pragmatic markers kind of and sort of across different age cohorts, genders and linguistic contexts in spoken British English. In previous linguistic research, pragmatic markers have been described as fuzzy items (Lakoff Reference Lakoff, Hockney, Harper and Freed1975: 234), as regards both meaning and function. The linguistic forms that are included in the broad group of pragmatic markers (e.g. like, well, you know, I mean, so) are generally defined as carrying ‘little obvious propositional meaning’ (Beeching Reference Beeching2016: 23) and operating predominantly on interactional levels of conversation. The items are syntactically flexibly employed and prone to fast-paced change, not just in terms of frequencies, but also in terms of the varying syntactic contexts in which they appear, their semantic uses and their social meanings.

The pragmatic markers kind of and sort of have been studied before, albeit not as extensively as other pragmatic markers, such as like (e.g. Buchstaller Reference Buchstaller2006; D'Arcy Reference D'Arcy2007, Reference D'Arcy2017; Buchstaller & D'Arcy Reference Buchstaller and D'Arcy2009) or you know (Erman Reference Erman2001; Macaulay Reference Macaulay2002; Fox Tree & Schrock Reference Tree, Jean and Schrock2002). Kind of and sort of as pragmatic markers are, for the purpose of this article, conceptualised as markers that add hedging (i.e. mitigation, downtoning, vagueness) to an utterance or specific parts of an utterance (see section 2 for a summary of functional definitions).

Tracing changes in use and function is challenging for a variety of reasons, from defining the envelope of variation (i.e. the variable context; see Wiltschko et al. Reference Wiltschko, Denis and D'Arcy2018), to disambiguating the functions, or finding enough markers in comparable contexts to make systematic judgments on their uses. The increasing availability of diachronic corpora of spoken language is a meaningful asset in collecting evidence of ongoing change. This article serves as a first step to looking at the most recent developments of the pragmatic markers kind of and sort of and their uses as shaped by social and linguistic factors. A second step will be to investigate syntactic factors in more detail, which is included here as a preliminary analysis (see section 4).

In order to describe its use and development, kind of is investigated alongside the ‘near synonymous’ sort of (Gries & David Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007: 1; see also Aijmer Reference Aijmer1984: 118; Mauranen Reference Mauranen, Aijmer and Stenström2004: 179), which is the preferred variant in British English varieties (Miskovic-Lukovic Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009: 619). Research has found that there might be syntactic preferences determining the variation between these two hedges (cf. Gries & David Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007), further detailed in section 4. Miskovic-Lukovic (Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009: 619) provides an in-depth analysis interpreting when the hedges kind of and/or sort of are used and, importantly for the current study, whether the inclusion of both kind of and sort of under one umbrella of variation is indeed warranted. While noting some possible semantic differences, she claims that, structurally and functionally, these items are used similarly, and they seem to have developed in comparable ways (Miskovic-Lukovic Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009: 607–8). The present study thus investigates the development of kind of in direct comparison to its nearest equivalent, also to see whether a change in one of them might affect the other. The study aims to address two research questions:

  1. 1. How has the use of the pragmatic markers kind of and sort of changed over the course of twenty years?

  2. 2. Which factors correlate with the observed changes?

A brief account of the two markers, their functions and uses is followed by an introduction of the data and methods used in the study. The analysis focuses first on frequency distribution across age and gender, both salient factors for linguistic change; this is followed by a brief syntactic analysis, and, finally, a multivariate analysis that statistically confirms the observations of change.

2 Pragmatic markers kind of and sort of

In some previous studies, kind of and sort of have been looked at as individual items (Aijmer Reference Aijmer2002; Fung & Carter Reference Fung and Carter2007; Beeching Reference Beeching2016), while other studies have covered both variants alongside each other (Gries & David Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007; Miskovic-Lukovic Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009). Yet other studies have focused on particular aspects of their uses, such as their role within the construction of general extenders, e.g. and that sort of thing (Aijmer Reference Aijmer2002; Cheshire Reference Cheshire2007). Similar to the contested definition of the whole group of pragmatic markers, the exact categorisation and functional definition of these two variants is still unclear (but see below for an attempt at a functional summary). For instance, it might be argued that one of the key defining features of pragmatic markers – the possibility of omissibility from discourse without changing the propositional content (see example (1)) – is not always fulfilled. As hedges, kind of and sort of might indeed affect the meaning of the modified item, as shown in example (2) below.

  1. (1) but then I just sort of think well there's four of us (BNC2014, female, age 19)

    *but then I just think well there's four of us

  2. (2) I'm kind of retired now and I'm j just like a show dog you know just like er b but we're kind of old now and just like settled down (BNC2014, female, age 29)

    *I'm retired now and I'm j just like a show dog you know just like er b but we're old now and just like settled down

In (2), kind of is used as a hedge to modify the two adjectives retired and old to diminish the impact these two words might have on the listener and the speaker's attitude towards their progressing age. By downtoning the following word, the meaning it carries in unmodified form is adapted and, as can be argued, modified in the process. Further functions are discussed in the remainder of this section.

Their apparent link to interpersonal meaning-making, politeness and face-saving strategies, mitigation and hedging has established kind of and sort of as typical pragmatic markers. Like many other markers, they carry very little propositional content but have retained semantic links to the lexical roots from which they have derived. Aijmer concludes that ‘the hedging meaning of “sort of” is therefore seen as an extension or change from the more literal meaning of “a sort of (a type of)”’ (Reference Aijmer2002: 180), a process also applicable to kind of. Both forms have developed from binominal constructions with subcategorisation meaning (Denison Reference Denison2002), in which the type noun (sort/kind) functions as the head of the phrase and is followed by of and a second noun. Over time, these constructions developed postdeterminer constructions, where ‘the string sort/kind of together with the primary determiner forms a complex determiner’ (Brems & Davidse Reference Brems and Davidse2010: 181). Here, the head of the binominal construction (kind/sort) is reanalysed and forms a determiner unit (primary determiner followed by kind of/sort of) that precedes the second noun, and as a result, the head-status of kind/sort is lost. Examples of these forms from this study's corpus (see section 3 for more information about the corpus) can be seen in (3) and (4).

  1. (3) people who like that kind of music (BNC 1994, male, age 41)

  2. (4) that's the sort of problem (BNC1994, male, age 51)

I call this the ‘type construction’ use of sort of/kind of, and consider its function as ‘propositional’. Brems & Davidse (Reference Brems and Davidse2010: 91) summarise different constructions with type nouns and point to the development of ‘pragmatic’ functions, such as hedging, since type nouns already indicate ‘that the description is only approximate’. Through the process of grammaticalisation (Hopper & Traugott Reference Hopper and Traugott1993), the propositional meaning of subcategorisation expands to include qualifying constructions, which carry pragmatic functions such as mitigation (Brems & Davidse Reference Brems and Davidse2010: 181).

Miskovic-Lukovic (Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009) summarises the development of kind of and sort of and notes that descriptions of the forms have covered shifts from noun constructions to adjectives (kind/sort of as adjectival modifiers of nouns), adverbs (kind/sort of as adverbial modifiers of verbs, adjectives, adverbs) and finally pragmatic particles (Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009: 609). This is indicated initially by the loss of the plural morpheme for sort/kind and, at a later point, the loss of the article preceding the construction. Following this development, sort/kind + of appeared as qualifying modifiers in contexts other than noun phrases (see example (6)). Further, both variants also include phonologically reduced items in spoken English: kinda and sorta (see Brinton Reference Brinton1996: 33–5).

In the following, using examples from the present study's corpus, I illustrate known pragmatic or discourse functions of kind of and sort of, first introduced by Beeching (Reference Beeching2016: 192–3). This categorisation (i) to (iv) is used as the basis for the present study. We see that the corpus results reflect the variants’ functional repertoire and are thus representative of the range of potential functions that they can serve within the context of spoken British English.

  1. i. Metacommenting, hedging and qualifying

Metacommenting, according to Aijmer (Reference Aijmer2002: 209), refers to linguistic distancing ‘from the responsibility for using words which are inappropriate because they are technical, trite, too informal, too formal, etc.’. The functions of hedging and qualifying relate to the previously discussed binominal uses where the modified construction is merely an approximation of the actual meaning of the modified item.

  1. (5) er you know you're mixing with you know highly sophisticated you know kind of cultured people in in in big project usually but you know step outside and meet more people from the street you know (BNC2014, female, age 38)

  2. (6) it's Friday today so everyone's sort of relaxed and good (BNC1994, male, age 14)

In example (5), the speaker seems aware that what she is saying might be taken the wrong way, so by adding kind of to the adjective ‘cultured’ she implies that the phrase is not to be taken too seriously. In (6), sort of approximates the meaning of ‘relaxed’ to be understood as ‘for a workday, this is not as stressful as a Monday’. The speaker is hedging ‘relaxed’ so as not to appear too cavalier at work.

  1. ii. Mitigating face threats

This function relates to processes of politeness and saving one's own or somebody else's face in discourse. In example (7) below, the speaker has inserted sort of in order to mitigate a possible insult, that is, the inference that the interlocutor, without the new and expensive glasses, looks unintelligent. He immediately follows up with another sort of, possibly to find a safer description for the other person.

  1. (7) they make you make you look sort of intelligent sort of (BNC1994, male, age 44)

  2. iii. Pause-filling

Pause-filling, in my data, includes examples of self-repair and general discourse particles that are not intended as fillers, but rather as communicative facilitators.

  1. (8) give us a cutch and stuff like that but then that was kind of er you know that was kind of it it wasn't really (BNC2014, female, age 27)

In (8), the speaker appears to be at a momentary loss for words, also noticeable through the hesitation marker er, repetition, self-repair and the pragmatic marker well.

  1. iv. General extender

The final function mentioned by Beeching (Reference Beeching2016: 193) is that of general extenders, which insert the variants sort of and kind of into a relatively stable construction: and/or + sort/kind of + thing/stuff. She points out that this function overlaps with face-threat mitigators; such overlaps highlight the difficulty in precisely defining functions of these variants.

  1. (9) A: and what did you like doing? (BNC2014, female, age 19)

    B: erm like badminton tennis (BNC2014, female, age 19)

    A: yeah (BNC2014, female, age 19)

    B: running just like long distance hiking kind of thing? (BNC2014, female, age 19)

In (9), the speaker seems to be saying that ‘long distance hiking’ might not be quite the right category for the type of sport she participates in.

Further to Beeching's (Reference Beeching2016) functions, I have also found the variants to be used in quotative function as in examples (10) and (11).

  1. (10) and he sort of oh what can I get you today mate? (BNC2014, male, age 66)

  2. (11) the next night her car was nicked from the carpark but she didn't seem all she she was sort of oh yeah someone nicked my car but she didn't seem indi[?] no I think she'd arranged to have that nicked (BNC2014, female, age 49)

The use of sort of and kind of in this function is relatively sparse, with only 11 tokens across over 8,500 relevant pragmatic marker variants. The approximation indexed by the marker signals less reliable reported speech and indicates to the listener that what is being retold as speech is only a near-accurate report.

Both kind of and sort of serve interpersonal and politeness functions in the sense that the propositional content is less strongly asserted and fuzzier compared to the same utterance without these forms. Beeching (Reference Beeching2016: 191–3) notes that distinguishing among these functions is nearly impossible without detailed manual coding and prosodic analysis. I would also question whether it is possible to attempt to neatly categorise individual tokens, as they might indeed serve more than one function in the same instance. For the present study, I focus on the general pragmatic function of kind of and sort of rather than a smaller-scale approach that would include a more detailed functional investigation. The overall goal is to obtain an initial analysis of ongoing change of these forms using a large corpus, before analysing the data on a functional level in future studies.

3 Methodology

3.1 Data

The data used in this study come from two subsets created from the spoken sections of (a) The British National Corpus (BNC) from 1994 and (b) the BNC 2014. The resulting corpus is nearly equally balanced across different categories (gender, region, socioeconomic background, word count contribution) and specifically designed to support research into linguistic change. Speakers who are included in the subset corpus range from age 5 to 95 with years of birth spanning a century (1899 (BNC1994) and 2009 (BNC2014)). This allows for a combination of apparent-time and real-time approaches, which disambiguates possible interpretations of generational change and age-grading, which is often difficult with studies following only one approach (see below).

The study aims at obtaining as full a picture as possible of the use of kind of and sort of across all age cohorts to allow for different approaches and comparability with future studies. Thus, in order to put the subset corpus together, some of the meta-information from the original corpora had to be re-coded; most importantly, age groups were aligned to create seven age cohorts (see table 1). This differs from the parent corpora, which do not use similar age delineations, and from the original BNC, which does not distinguish age differences beyond the age of 60.

Table 1. Subset corpus distribution across gender and age

Linguistic change is often found first with typical innovators: usually (female) adolescents around the age of 17 (Stenström Reference Stenström, Hasselgård and Oksefjell1999, Reference Stenström2002; Chambers Reference Chambers2003; Tagliamonte & D'Arcy Reference Tagliamonte and D'Arcy2009; Tagliamonte Reference Tagliamonte2016). Older speakers, in comparison, exhibit relative stability in their stylistic repertoire (Labov Reference Labov2001; Wagner Reference Wagner2012); this stability has led to a lack of linguistic interest and, as Coupland notes, is a reflection of ‘ageist stereotypes of older adults being “set in their ways”’ (Reference Coupland, Coupland, Sarangi and Candlin2001: 191–3). Elsewhere, he warns that a main focus on younger speakers associates older age cohorts with ‘an unmarked demographic condition’ (Reference Coupland, Nussbaum and Coupland2004: 69) and prevents diverse and balanced speaker representation in linguistic research (see also Barbieri Reference Barbieri2008: 59; Pichler et al. Reference Pichler, Wagner and Hesson2018: 2). Further, by extending the age ranges, apparent-time and real-time approaches can be combined to allow for a more thorough investigation. The apparent-time approach involves the study of linguistic change at a single point in time instead of comparing language use as time passes, i.e. the real-time approach. The assumption behind the apparent-time approach is that different age cohorts reflect the language of their respective generations and can be compared. Thus, higher frequencies in use of a particular feature by younger speakers might indicate a change in progress, i.e. a new feature coming into use. However, age-based variation is not a sure sign of ongoing change, as Wagner (Reference Wagner2012: 371) highlights; apparent-time studies might overestimate variation phenomena such as age-grading as generational change. Real-time studies look at language change across different points in time. If this approach includes only particular age cohorts, e.g. only adolescents, the results might lead to misinterpretation of the data. Thus, combining approaches and including a wide glance across the age scale allows for a better analysis, ‘with the relative strengths of one approach offsetting the weaknesses of the other’ (Bailey Reference Bailey, Chambers, Trudgill and Schilling2002: 330). With this in mind, the subset corpus aims at including age cohorts at representative and comparable levels, although it must be noted that balancing efforts relied heavily on the distribution already existing in the parent corpora.

The second main speaker variable that is included in this study is gender. Due to lack of further information about the original BNC, it must be assumed that speakers contributing data to the corpora all identified as either male or female. While in the case of the BNC 2014 speakers were asked to indicate their gender on a free-text form, allowing for non-binary, a-gender, or other gender identities to be named, all participants identified a binary identity as either male or female (Robbie Love, personal correspondence). Table 1 presents a summary of the numbers of speakers in the age cohorts and genders in the two subcorpora.

Both subcorpora consist of 250 speakers, with 2,714,337 words from the original BNC and 5,938,032 words from the BNC 2014. Only speakers with at least 1,000 contributed words and complete meta-data were added to the subsets. The differing word counts in the subcorpora were addressed in the analyses by providing relative frequencies (in percentages and occurrences per 1,000 words).

Other social information embedded in the subset corpus is regional background (nine regions across the British Isles), occupation and socioeconomic status. However, the present study focuses on age and gender only.

3.2 Procedure

All tokens for kind of, kinda, sort of and sorta were extracted using the POS-tagged version of the subset corpus in Sketch Engine (Kilgarriff et al. Reference Kilgarriff, Baisa, Bušta, Jakubíček, Kovář, Michelfeit, Rychlý and Suchomel2014), including the immediate context (100 characters) to the left and right to allow for initial disambiguation. For the analysis, the phonologically reduced forms were included with their respective base forms. As previously mentioned, the distinction between the propositional and pragmatic functions of kind of and sort of in noun phrases is marked syntactically by the position of the determiner. Because of possible expansion into new positions, however, all occurrences required semi-manual coding despite seemingly clear part-of-speech patterns. All tokens were coded for whether they were used as a pragmatic marker (including all functions as discussed in section 2) or as a propositional type noun construct. A second round of coding was then applied to all pragmatic markers, where it was determined what part of the sentence the marker modified.

Overall, 13,706 tokens were extracted, of which 5,164 were propositional and 8,541 were pragmatic uses. The following analysis provides a general overview of the distribution and overall development of the pragmatic markers, followed by a more detailed look into age-based variation indicating a possible linguistic change.

4 Results and discussion

4.1 General distribution

A total of 8,541 pragmatic marker tokens were included in the analysis, of which 3,079 are kind of and 5,462 are sort of. This reflects previous findings that suggested that sort of is the preferred variant in British English varieties (cf. Miskovic-Lukovic Reference Miskovic-Lukovic2009; Beeching Reference Beeching2016). Both variants increased in relative use between 1994 and 2014, although the level of increase shows a stark contrast and points towards a noticeable uptake of kind of. Some more details on this development can be gained from the discourse values of both forms, which reflect the markers’ pragmatic function potential. Discourse values (d-values) are relative representations of a marker's ‘discourse function in relation to grammatical function expressed (in percent)’ (Stenström Reference Stenström and Svartvik1990: 161; Aijmer Reference Aijmer2002: 27). Based on the claim that the propositional use is a rather stable value, a shift in discourse values thus indicates a shift in the pragmatic use of a form (Beeching Reference Beeching2016: 77), pointing towards a feature undergoing change. The discourse value, represented in percentages, serves mainly as a normalisation method that enables comparison across different studies and different markers at various points in their development. For the present study, the above-mentioned uptake of kind of as a pragmatic marker is clearly visible (see table 2).

Table 2. Distribution of kind of and sort of in the subset corpus

In 1994, the discourse value for kind of is still relatively low (24 per cent), with 76 per cent of occurrences of all kind of tokens of the type noun constructions, but the discourse uses rise to 63 per cent in 2014. The discourse value for sort of is relatively stable in comparison (60 per cent in 1994 and 56 per cent in 2014); whether the slight decrease in pragmatic functions correlates to the increase of kind of will be shown in the more detailed age brackets in section 4.2.

4.2 Social factors

In the following, the apparent increase of kind of in relation to sort of will be observed across different age groups and at two points in time. Data here are represented as relative per 1,000 words.

Figure 1 shows the mean distribution of kind of across the seven age cohorts (upper panel). Data from 1994 reveal very low use with a slight increase in the age cohort 15 to 24, which fits neatly into the linguistic innovator bracket. Twenty years later, the lighter grey bars reveal that the use of kind of by this group (now aged 35 to 44) has increased even more (from 0.11 to 0.53). This observation holds true for all other generations as well, clearly indicating a change in progress on the level of generational increase as well as lifespan change (Sankoff Reference Sankoff and Fought2004). This means that the change is not just happening generation by generation, but that the use of kind of is increasing within each generation as well.

Figure 1. Kind of and sort of across age, per 1,000 words

In 2014, the most frequent use of pragmatic marker kind of appears in the age group 25 to 34, which is slightly beyond the adolescent innovation peak, but still within the range of young speakers. The slight dip in the youngest speaker groups (0–14 and 15–24) might give an indication of added social meaning that the considerable increase has caused. If that is the case, it means that the variant has developed into what Labov has defined as a marker, i.e. forms ‘that have attracted sufficient attention to emerge … in stylistic variation’ (Eckert Reference Eckert2008: 463). Kind of might thus be indexically tied to the generation preceding the younger speakers, motivating the latter to avoid using it as a form of stylistic distinction. However, to speak with any kind of certainty of such an interpretation, attitudinal data would be needed.

Looking at the age cohorts for sort of (lower panel), we can detect a relatively stable use across the generations. The highest use in 1994 appears, again, with speakers aged 15 to 24. By 2014, however, this has shifted to the age group 60 to 74, which is well beyond expected innovative ages. Further, for all younger age cohorts (0 up to 34), the use of sort of actually decreased between 1994 and 2014. Still, despite the noticeable increase of kind of, sort of remains the overall preferred variant and is relatively stable and unaffected. This is at odds with previous studies claiming that both variants are serving similar functions, as such a drastic increase in one variant would have replaced the other (the division of labour principle). This might then point to a structural expansion, i.e. where the increase of kind of also points to a new application.

Figure 2 shows the distribution across year of birth, simulating a view of both corpora neatly overlapping rather than being separated by twenty years. It indicates user-specific frequencies of the variants (y-axis) alongside a density count, which indicates where most speakers are located in terms of their uses. Kind of (upper panel) shows the earliest use as a pragmatic marker (in this dataset) by speakers born in the 1920s (in comparison, sort of is used by one of the oldest speakers included in the corpus, born in 1899). Here, the development of the form becomes quite clear, with the highest uses of pragmatic marker kind of appearing among speakers born between 1970 and 1990.

Figure 2. Kind of and sort of across year of birth, per 1,000 words

The lower panels of sort of show that the form is used more often altogether, but that the high point of use based on the year of birth lies somewhere between the 1960s to 1980s. Its relative stability is further shown by few outliers and an almost indistinguishable shift between the two datasets.

The juxtaposition of change and stability of variants that are said to be functionally equivalent is further highlighted in a second observation of discourse values (see table 3). As above, the values represent the relative use of pragmatic forms against all forms (pragmatic + propositional). Unsurprisingly, the discourse value increased within each age cohort for kind of, supporting the idea that this feature has gained new uses, either in pragmatic functions or syntactic contexts. The higher discourse value also indicates higher levels of societal acceptance. Beeching (Reference Beeching2016: 194), when commenting on consistently high discourse values of sort of in her study, says it shows ‘that pragmatic marker sort of is a highly respectable way of hedging speech’. Returning to the interpretation of slightly lower uses of kind of in the youngest two age cohorts of 2014, another possibility might be that it is precisely the gained acceptability indicated through the high discourse value (84 and 68 per cent) that prevents further high uses in that age bracket. As with other pragmatic markers (e.g. intensifiers), certain functions demand a swift changeover to remain meaningful in discourse (cf. Tagliamonte Reference Tagliamonte2008).

Table 3. Discourse value of kind of and sort of across age groups (percentage)

Sort of exhibits only one notable increase in discourse values, which is located in the age group 15 to 24. This distribution is quite likely to be an indicator of age-graded structural behaviour rather than any linguistic change.

The fact that change in progress is usually spearheaded not only by adolescents, but by female adolescents, warrants a brief investigation of the distribution across gender. In figure 3, female speakers are indicated with a solid line, while male speakers are indicated with a dotted line. In 1994, we can see that for the youngest three age cohorts, female speakers are slightly leading in use of kind of, which, for the following cohorts, is reversed. By 2014, female speakers are more consistently leading in use, particularly in age groups 15 to 24 and 35 to 44. For the youngest speakers, a different pattern appears, which may be explained by my previous interpretation of social meaning attached to incoming variants. The distribution does not show any statistically significant gender differences (p > .05).

Figure 3. Kind of and sort of across age and gender, per 1,000 words

In comparison, the distribution for sort of (lower panel) appears much more unstructured. This seems likely for a feature that is not particularly marked and widely accepted across age and gender categories. It appears that male speakers in 1994 exhibit an expected stratification where the highest use can be found in the younger age cohorts (15 up to 44) before tapering off with increasing age. By 2014, this pattern has widened further to include ages up to 74, indicating that the pragmatic marker sort of is no longer age marked at all. Even more interesting is the distribution for female speakers, which almost inverts between 1994 and 2014. In 1994 we find a distribution rather typical for age-grading, with high uses throughout adolescence and a quick decrease with increasing age. By 2014, however, this pattern has shifted completely with the highest uses now recorded in the age groups 60 to 74 and 75 to 95. It seems as though, in 2014, young women have found alternative forms of hedging (certainly kind of, as indicated above, but possibly also alternative variants). Across all age groups and both corpus subsets, their use of sort of is lowest of all. This distribution is particularly remarkable considering that it is this age bracket that has shown the highest discourse value, possibly pointing to a decrease in the use of propositional sort of.

Taking the rapid change of kind of in frequencies into account, as well as possible shifts in use as indicated by discourse values, the next step is to look at the immediate context of the pragmatic markers.

4.3 Internal factors

As previously mentioned, this part of the analysis will be kept relatively short, as a more detailed and fine-grained exploration of precise functions would call for prosodic analyses. This part of the analysis thus provides a first investigation of the type of modification found with kind of (kinda) and sort of (sorta), followed by a summarising multivariate analysis.

Most of the coding for contextual information was enabled by the POS-tagged format of the subset corpus, which allowed me to categorise tokens in a straightforward manner. The initial step was to evaluate tokens that appeared with a determiner immediately to the left of the node as in (12), which almost always indicate propositional uses and were thus excluded from the study. Slightly more complex alternatives of propositional uses include determiner + adjective + node, as in (13), and were also excluded from the pragmatic focus.

  1. (12) the kind of books I'm reading (BNC2014, female, age 20)

  2. (13) it might erm be the right kind of book (BNC1994, female, age 33)

Pragmatic uses that were included in the study are modifications of adjectives (14), adverbs (15), nouns (16), prepositions (17) and verbs (18).

  1. (14) it's just kind of warmish (BNC1994, female, age 34)

  2. (15) he just walks kind of calmly (BNC2014, male, age 21)

  3. (16) somebody who was selling kind of kits of that (BNC2014, female, age 64)

  4. (17) she flew up in the air sort of across the kitchen (BNC2014, female, age 49)

  5. (18) we kind of made profit (BNC1994, female, age 44)

Further, pragmatic markers are also included when they appeared clause-finally (19), or were used in other pragmatic functions, such as approximation (20), quotation (21), or as a slightly more ambiguous discourse particle (22), which includes filler functions, and self-repair items.

  1. (19) he's gone home sort of (BNC1994, male, age 14)

  2. (20) I managed to get on a bus at sort of five-ish (BNC1994, female, age 28)

  3. (21) he was sort of oh god (BNC2014, male, age 66)

  4. (22) Well he just totally sort of because I think he knows (BNC1994, female, age 34)

Thus, the markers are quite varied in syntactic context and the question arises whether any of these contexts have shifted with the increased use of kind of. Gries & David (Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007: 9) found that, when looking at collocations with parts of speech, kind of showed preference in the modification of nouns and adjectives, while sort of appeared more often than expected with adverbs and verbs, as well as whole propositions. Additionally, they note that, in terms of modified lemmas, the two variants share little collocational overlap. This means that the variants are following ‘lexically determined variation’ (Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007: 11), whereby kind of, for instance, more likely modifies adjectives describing emotional states or verbs describing mental activities. This pattern might explain how kind of increased so drastically while sort of remained relatively stable. Kind of is expanding within the system; however, it is not doing so by taking over contexts that are traditionally modified by sort of as both variants seem to follow distinct modification patterns.

Table 4 summarises the modification for both variants at both points in time. Because of ambiguity in function, the variants that were coded as discourse particles (see example (22)) are not included. In 1994 kind of modified four categories of parts of speech (nouns, prepositions, verbs and adjectives) and was used in clause-final position. By 2014, it had gained uses in all remaining syntactic contexts as well, which explains the increased discourse values. The contexts of sort of appear largely unchanged. In terms of distinct uses, the two variants do not appear to be dividing their uses across modifiable and other pragmatic contexts. That means that both variants are used as modifiers across different parts of speech, albeit with different preferences; see discussion of multivariate analysis below for a statistical evaluation of expected and realised preferences.

Table 4. Syntactic application of kind of and sort of, relative value of modified instances (percentage)

Finally, in order to test the factors involved in the analysis, a multivariate regression analysis in Rbrul (Johnson Reference Johnson2009) was run to determine significance levels and overall trends in preference between the use of kind of and sort of. As previously established, both forms can be considered equivalent in function with slightly differing use preferences. As pragmatic markers, the two variants form an envelope of variation, or variable context, which allows for a variationist approach. The following analysis tests which constraints, social and linguistic, are statistically meaningful when kind of is chosen over sort of.

Factors included reflect the previous distributional analysis: age as a continuous variable, gender, syntactic contexts and of course the respective subset corpus. Speakers were included as random factors, which accounts for individual preferences that might otherwise indicate an unbalanced sample (see table 5).

Table 5. Multivariate analysis of kind of and sort of

The analysis shows that the most significant constraint is the point in time, i.e. the subset corpus, which confirms that the general change is indeed an ongoing generational change in progress. The data show that kind of is more likely to be used in the 2014 corpus. This confirms a general change in the use of kind of. Age is the second most significant predictor for the use of kind of over sort of. That is, with decreasing age, the chances of a speaker choosing kind of as a pragmatic marker are increasing. Syntactic context is also found to be significant in constraining the occurrence of kind of. Similar to the findings by Gries & David (Reference Gries, David, Pahta, Taavitsainen, Nevalainen and Tyrkkö2007), kind of favours modification of nouns and adjectives, while disfavouring verbs. Finally, gender was not found to be significant, which reflects the findings discussed previously. Men and women, at both points in time as well as across age cohorts, roughly follow the same pattern, with women leading only slightly in frequency. This conclusion echoes Beeching's (Reference Beeching2016: 209) study of sort of in British English contexts, where she discusses several social patterns, including gender, and finds no difference in frequency or function between male and female speakers. Nonetheless, the gendered use of sort of in 2014 (see figure 3) indicates a pattern that will be interesting to observe in more detail.

5 Conclusion

The analysis shows that the pragmatic marker kind of has increased considerably over the span of just twenty years, from 1994 to 2014. The approach of combining apparent-time and real-time studies further highlights that age-based uses of the marker correlate with patterns of age-grading, generational change, as well as lifespan change. Sort of, as a marker serving similar functions, has previously been established as the preferred British English variant. While this preference is still upheld in the 2014 subset corpus, the data suggest that the two variants, in British English contexts, are no longer distinguished. Surprisingly, general frequencies of sort of have not been affected as might be expected (although it appears as though the marker is losing its prominence with younger speakers). The recent increase of kind of also raises questions of possible indexed social meaning, as became clear in the visualisations of development across time and gender.

In terms of the linguistic structures that the variants modify, the markers appear to have slight preferences for certain parts of speech. A cautionary glance into the future might include structural separation and more pronounced linguistic differentiation. A more detailed follow-up study of functional and structural preferences might untangle further patterns of pragmatic language change.

The study not only sheds light on the development of a pragmatic marker variable in spoken British English but also highlights the usefulness of comparable larger-sized corpora that provide full speaker meta-data (detailed information on chronological age alongside year of birth). By being able to put together subsets of material to allow for the type of approach that fits the research questions, scholars can trace language change more thoroughly. Second and third-generation corpora, such as the two BNC sets, enable linguists to investigate language change in more detail and with more rigour.

Footnotes

This study was funded as part of the project ‘The British National Corpus (BNC) as a sociolinguistic dataset: Exploring individual and social variation’, ESRC grant no. EP/P001559/1.

References

Aijmer, Karin. 1984. ‘Sort of’ and ‘kind of’ in English conversation. Studia Linguistica 38(2), 118–28.Google Scholar
Aijmer, Karin. 2002. English discourse particles: Evidence from a corpus. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
BNC Consortium. 2007. The British National Corpus, version 3 BNC XML edition. Distributed by Bodleian Libraries, University of Oxford, on behalf of the BNC Consortium. www.natcorp.ox.ac.ukGoogle Scholar
Bailey, Guy. 2002. Real and apparent time. In Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, Peter & Schilling, Natalie (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change, 312–32. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Barbieri, Federica. 2008. Patterns of age-based linguistic variation in American English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(1), 5888.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beeching, Kate. 2016. Pragmatic markers in British English: Meaning in social interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brems, Lieselotte & Davidse, Kristin. 2010. The grammaticalisation of nominal type noun constructions with kind/sort of: Chronology and paths of change. English Studies 91(2), 180202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J. 1996. Pragmatic markers in English: Grammaticalization and discourse functions. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle. 2006. Social stereotypes, personality traits and regional perception displaced: Attitudes towards the ‘new’ quotative in the UK. Journal of Sociolinguistics 10(3), 362–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchstaller, Isabelle & D'Arcy, Alexandra. 2009. Localized globalization: A multilocal, multivariate investigation of quotative be like. Journal of Sociolinguistics 13(1), 291331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, J. K. 2003. Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social significance, 2nd edn. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Cheshire, Jenny. 2007. Discourse variation, grammaticalisation and stuff like that. Journal of Sociolinguistics 11(2), 155–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coupland, Nikolas. 2001. Age in social and sociolinguistic theory. In Coupland, Nikolas, Sarangi, Srikant & Candlin, Christopher N. (eds.), Sociolinguistics and social theory, 185211. London: Pearson.Google Scholar
Coupland, Nikolas. 2004. Age in social and sociolinguistic theory. In Nussbaum, Jon F. & Coupland, Justine (eds.), Handbook of communication and aging research, 6990. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Denison, David. 2002. History of the sort of construction family. Presented at the Second International Conference on Construction Grammar (ICCGS), University of Helsinki, Finland. www.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/medialibrary/llc/files/david-denison/Helsinki_ICCG2.pdf (accessed 20 May 2021).Google Scholar
D'Arcy, Alexandra. 2007. Like and language ideology: Disentangling fact from fiction. American Speech 82(4), 386419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Arcy, Alexandra. 2017. Discourse-pragmatic variation in context: Eight hundred years of LIKE. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2008. Variation and the indexical field. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(4), 453–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erman, Britt. 2001. Pragmatic markers revisited with a focus on you know in adult and adolescent talk. Journal of Pragmatics 33(9), 1337–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tree, Fox, Jean, E. & Schrock, Josef C.. 2002. Basic meanings of you know and I mean. Journal of Pragmatics 34(6), 727–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fung, Loretta & Carter, Ronald. 2007. Discourse markers and spoken English: Native and learner use in pedagogic settings. Applied Linguistics 28(3), 410–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. & David, Caroline. 2007. This is kind of / sort of interesting: Variation in hedging in English. In Pahta, Päivi, Taavitsainen, Irma, Nevalainen, Terttu & Tyrkkö, Jukka (eds.), Studies in variation, contacts and change in English, vol. 2: Towards multimedia in corpus studies. www.helsinki.fi/varieng/journal/volumes/02/gries_david/Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. & Traugott, Elizabeth Closs. 1993. Grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Daniel Ezra. 2009. Getting off the GoldVarb standard: Introducing Rbrul for mixed-effects variable rule analysis. Language and Linguistics Compass 3(1), 359–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kilgarriff, Adam, Baisa, Vít, Bušta, Jan, Jakubíček, Miloš, Kovář, Vojtěch, Michelfeit, Jan, Rychlý, Pavel & Suchomel, Vit. 2014. The Sketch Engine: Ten years on. Lexicography 1(1), 736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 2001. Principles of language change: Social factors. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1975. Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts. In Hockney, D. J., Harper, William & Freed, B. (eds.), Contemporary research in philosophical logic and linguistic semantics, 221–71. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Love, Robbie, Dembry, Claire, Hardie, Andrew, Brezina, Vaclav & McEnery, Tony. 2017. The Spoken BNC2014: Designing and building a spoken corpus of everyday conversations. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 22(3), 319–44.Google Scholar
Macaulay, Ronald. 2002. You know, it depends. Journal of Pragmatics 34(6), 749–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauranen, Anna. 2004. ‘They're a little bit different.’ Observations on hedges in academic talk. In Aijmer, Karin & Stenström, Anna (eds.), Discourse patterns in spoken and written corpora, 173–97. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miskovic-Lukovic, Mirjana. 2009. Is there a chance that I might kinda sort of take you out to dinner? The role of the pragmatic particles kind of and sort of in utterance interpretation. Journal of Pragmatics 41(3), 602–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pichler, Heike, Wagner, Suzanne Evans & Hesson, Ashley. 2018. Old-age language variation and change: Confronting variationist ageism. Language and Linguistics Compass 12(6), e12281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian. 2004. Adolescents, young adults and the critical period: Two case studies from ‘Seven Up’. In Fought, Carmen (ed.), Sociolinguistic variation: Critical reflections, 121–39. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Britta. 1990. Pauses in monologue and dialogue. In Svartvik, Jan (ed.), The London–Lund Corpus of Spoken English: Description and research, 211–52. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Britta. 1999. He was really gormless – she's pretty crap: Girls, boys, and intensifiers. In Hasselgård, Hilde & Oksefjell, Signe (eds.), Out of corpora: Studies in honour of Stig Johansson, 6978. Amsterdam and Atlanta, GA: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Stenström, Anna-Britta. 2002. Trends in teenage talk: Corpus compilation, analysis and findings. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 2008. So different and pretty cool! Recycling intensifiers in Toronto, Canada. English Language and Linguistics 12(2), 361–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 2016. Teen talk: The language of adolescents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. & D'Arcy, Alexandra. 2009. Peaks beyond phonology: Adolescence, incrementation, and language change. Language 85(1), 58108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Suzanne Evans. 2012. Age grading in sociolinguistic theory. Language and Linguistics Compass 6(6), 371–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiltschko, Martina, Denis, Derek & D'Arcy, Alexandra. 2018. Deconstructing variation in pragmatic function: A transdisciplinary case study. Language in Society 47(4), 569–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Subset corpus distribution across gender and age

Figure 1

Table 2. Distribution of kind of and sort of in the subset corpus

Figure 2

Figure 1. Kind of and sort of across age, per 1,000 words

Figure 3

Figure 2. Kind of and sort of across year of birth, per 1,000 words

Figure 4

Table 3. Discourse value of kind of and sort of across age groups (percentage)

Figure 5

Figure 3. Kind of and sort of across age and gender, per 1,000 words

Figure 6

Table 4. Syntactic application of kind of and sort of, relative value of modified instances (percentage)

Figure 7

Table 5. Multivariate analysis of kind of and sort of