Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-gtxcr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:43:31.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Timing of childhood adversities and self-injurious thoughts and behaviors in adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 July 2021

Samantha J. North
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Professional Psychology (North) & Department of Psychology (Fox & Doom), University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
Kathryn R. Fox
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Professional Psychology (North) & Department of Psychology (Fox & Doom), University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
Jenalee R. Doom*
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Professional Psychology (North) & Department of Psychology (Fox & Doom), University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
*
Author for Correspondence: Jenalee Doom, Frontier Hall, 2155 S. Race Street, Denver, CO 80210, USA; E-mail: sam.north@du.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Greater childhood adversity predicts a higher likelihood of later self-injurious thoughts and behaviors (SITB). There is little research focused on whether the timing of childhood adversity predicts SITB. The current research examined whether the timing of childhood adversity predicted parent- and youth-reported SITB at age 12 and 16 years in the Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN) cohort (n = 970). We found that greater adversity at age 11–12 years consistently predicted SITB at age 12 years, while greater adversity at age 13–14 years consistently predicted SITB at age 16 years. These findings suggest there may be sensitive periods during which adversity may be more likely to lead to adolescent SITB, which can inform prevention and treatment.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Self-injurious thoughts and behaviors (SITB), both suicidal and nonsuicidal, affect millions of people around the world annually. Each year, more than 800,000 people are estimated to die by suicide worldwide (WHO, 2019). For every person that dies by suicide, many more attempt suicide (WHO, 2019). In the United States, suicide is the second leading cause of death for youth and young adults aged 10–29 years (Curtin & Heron, Reference Curtin and Heron2019). In 2017, over 7% of youth in Grades 9–12 reported attempting to die by suicide in the past year (CDC, 2020). Nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI), which falls under the umbrella of self-harm, is quite common, with nearly 5.5% of adults and 17% of adolescents reporting engaging in these behaviors (Swannell, Martin, Page, Hasking, & St John, Reference Swannell, Martin, Page, Hasking and St John2014).

One predictor of SITB is a higher number of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) (Cleare et al., Reference Cleare, Wetherall, Clark, Ryan, Kirtley, Smith and O'Connor2018), which are defined as experiences during one's childhood that are characterized as potentially traumatic (CDC, 2019). In a survey of US adults, 61% had experienced at least one instance of childhood adversity, and one in six reported experiencing four or more adversities (NCIPC, 2020). ACEs are commonly measured via 10 experiences that include physical, emotional and sexual abuse; physical and emotional neglect; household dysfunction such as mental illness; incarcerated relative; mother treated violently; substance abuse; and divorce. However, childhood adversity is not limited to these 10 ACEs and can include experiences such as homelessness, parental death, or foster care.

SITB are more common among people who have experienced greater childhood adversities (Bruwer et al., Reference Bruwer, Govender, Bishop, Williams, Stein and Seedat2014; Choi, Dinitto, Marti, & Segal, Reference Choi, Dinitto, Marti and Segal2017; Tunnard et al., Reference Tunnard, Rane, Wooderson, Markopoulou, Poon, Fekadu and Cleare2013). For example, suicide attempts are more common after experiencing childhood adversity, with one study finding them two to five times more likely varying across adversity type (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Dinitto, Marti and Segal2017). Childhood adversity is also associated with increased NSSI risk (Kaess et al., Reference Kaess, Parzer, Mattern, Plener, Bifulco, Resch and Brunner2013). Several studies demonstrate a graded association between childhood adversity and SITB, with more adversities associated with a higher risk of SITB-related outcomes (Dube et al., Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001). For example, an individual with three or more childhood adversities has a three times greater risk for suicidal ideation than an individual with no childhood adversities (Thompson, Kingree, & Lamis, Reference Thompson, Kingree and Lamis2019). Similarly, this graded association has been demonstrated between the number of abusive experiences during childhood and NSSI (Wan, Chen, Sun, & Tao, Reference Wan, Chen, Sun and Tao2015).

The developmental psychopathology framework provides insight into the pathways between childhood adversity and SITB. It is hypothesized that trauma disrupts normative development through a domino effect, where developmental delays at one stage affect development later on (Howe, Reference Howe2016). Others have described this phenomenon as a negative developmental cascade, which can unfold across the life span (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016). For example, if a negative event such as childhood adversity occurs, it can affect resilience processes and shift someone from a positive trajectory to a more negative one that could lead to psychopathology and SITB (Fergusson, Beautais, & Horwood, Reference Fergusson, Beautais and Horwood2003).

Consistent with the developmental psychopathology framework's focus on multiple levels of analysis, there are several mechanisms by which childhood adversity could increase risk for SITB. Long-term alterations in physiology following childhood adversity have been consistently demonstrated, which could increase SITB risk. For example, there is evidence that the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal system may be altered as a result of experiencing childhood abuse (Shonkoff, Reference Shonkoff2016), and these alterations are associated with increased suicide risk (McGowan et al., Reference McGowan, Sasaki, D'Alessio, Dymov, Labonté, Szyf and Meaney2009). Childhood adversity may also interact with genetic predispositions to increase risk for emotional dysregulation, suicidal ideation, and multiple forms of psychopathology, which have all been associated with SITB outcomes (Sachs-Ericsson, Rushing, Stanley, & Sheffler, Reference Sachs-Ericsson, Rushing, Stanley and Sheffler2016).

Cognitive and emotional mechanisms may also contribute to the association between childhood adversity and SITB. Childhood adversities have been associated with the development of cognitive biases, emotion regulation deficits, poorer executive functioning, and less effective coping strategies. Again, each of these are associated with increased risk for psychopathology and SITB (Burton, Vella, Weller, & Twamley, Reference Burton, Vella, Weller and Twamley2011; Sachs-Ericsson et al., Reference Sachs-Ericsson, Rushing, Stanley and Sheffler2016). Childhood adversities have also been associated with deficits in interpersonal functioning, which may leave individuals without strong social support systems, which in turn could increase SITB risk (Sachs-Ericsson et al., Reference Sachs-Ericsson, Rushing, Stanley and Sheffler2016). Fewer education and employment opportunities for those who experience greater childhood adversities may also be associated with later SITB risk (Font & Maguire-Jack, Reference Font and Maguire-Jack2016; Ports et al., Reference Ports, Merrick, Stone, Wilkins, Reed, Ebin and Ford2017).

Beyond studies documenting the association between overall childhood adversities and SITB, there has been more specific research examining the association between specific types of childhood adversity and SITB. Research suggests that certain adversities, including maltreatment and parental incarceration, are associated with increased risk for NSSI (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Kingree and Lamis2019), while sexual abuse in particular is associated with even greater risk (Serafini et al., Reference Serafini, Canepa, Adavastro, Nebbia, Belvederi Murri, Erbuto and Amore2017). Another study found that certain combinations of adversities were more predictive of suicidal ideation than others. This association was further moderated by developmental timing (i.e., adolescence vs. childhood; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a). There is less research focused on whether childhood adversity that occurs during certain chronological ages or developmental stages better predicts SITB. One study considered recency of childhood adversity and suicidal ideation of 16-year-olds using data from the Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN). The results indicated that recent adversity (defined as the period of time between 12 and 16 years of age) was associated with suicidal ideation at age 16 years (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a). While the article by Thompson and colleagues provides some insight into timing as a factor in SITB outcomes, more research is needed to develop a more nuanced understanding of the developmental timing of childhood adversity and the subsequent associations with SITB (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a).

The current research examined whether childhood adversities that occurred during specific developmental periods (measured prospectively) were more strongly associated with later SITB in adolescence. Similar to the approach of Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and FlahertyThompson et al. (Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a), the current study also used the LONGSCAN data set. Extending prior research, the present study examined cumulative adverse experiences that occur during more discrete developmental stages, as determined by chronological age groupings of 0–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–10 years, 11–12 years, 13–14 years, and 15–16 years. In addition, the present study assessed both adolescent- and parent-reported suicidal ideation and intentional self-harm as outcomes of interest during two different points in adolescence: 12 and 16 years of age. In light of prior evidence showing an association between more recent adversity and negative mental health outcomes including suicidal ideation (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a, Reference Thompson, Proctor, English, Dubowitz, Narasimhan and Everson2012b), we hypothesized that more recent childhood adversity (15–16 years for 16-year outcomes, and 11–12 years for 12-year outcomes) will have more consistent associations with SITB.

Method

The LONGSCAN study was conducted with 1,354 families in five regions throughout the United States. Participants were selected in caregiver–child dyads based on different criteria for each region. The East sample was made up of children from pediatric clinics serving low-income children that met criteria for risk through failure to thrive or prenatal drug use by the mother. The Midwest sample was derived from Child Protective Services (CPS) reports of infants aged 3–18 months. The Northwest sample came from children 0–4 years old at moderate risk based on CPS reports. The South sample was children aged 4–5 years, identified as high risk at the time of birth as determined by public health tracking. In the Southwest sample, the children were 4 years old and were placed with a relative or foster family due to confirmed maltreatment (Runyan et al., Reference Runyan, Dubowitz, English, Kotch, Litrownik and Thompson2014). Based on these criteria, children were either 4 or 6 years of age at the time of recruitment.

The sample for this study was closely split between males (49%) and females (51%). The primary race of participants was Black or African American (53%), followed by non-Hispanic White (26.3%) (descriptive statistics are provided in Table 1). Data on maltreatment were collected from face-to-face interviews and CPS records every two years. Child assessments took place at ages 4, 6, 8, 12, 14, 16, and 18 years. In addition, there were yearly phone calls with caregivers to track important life events and suicide risk between the two-year assessments (i.e., at ages 5, 7, 9, 10, 11, 13, and 15 years). CPS records were reviewed on an ongoing basis. The study began with 1,354 identified children. From baseline to 14 years old attrition was 25%, and baseline to 16 years old was 33.9% (Runyan et al., Reference Runyan, Dubowitz, English, Kotch, Litrownik and Thompson2014).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Childhood adversity

To create a childhood stressors index, all study variables were reviewed to identify childhood threat, abuse/neglect, adversity, and extreme stress (e.g., homelessness, foster care, parental death). The childhood adversity variables in this study included: physical abuse, sexual abuse, neglect, emotional maltreatment, substance use, divorce/separation, incarceration, death, foster care, homelessness, and witnessed violence. We extracted childhood adversities from several sources within the data set, including parent-report and CPS records using the Modified Maltreatment Classification System (English & LONGSCAN Investigators, Reference English1997). Table S1 of the Supplementary Material details the different categories of childhood adversity, which measures were used to assess it, and which specific questions were used for the current study. For each type of adversity, if an adversity occurred during a specific time period (ages 0–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–10 years, 11–12 years, 13–14 years, 15–16 years), a code of 1 was assigned. If it did not occur, a code of 0 was assigned. As children entered the study at various times before the age of 6 years, adversities from ages 0 to 6 years were grouped together rather than in two-year increments like the other groupings.

Physical abuse/sexual abuse/emotional maltreatment/neglect/substance use

For each of the five LONGSCAN sites, CPS records were assessed annually for maltreatment allegations regarding the child participants. Substantiated allegations were included for this study. CPS records were reviewed using the Modified Maltreatment Classification System. Physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional maltreatment, and neglect were separately coded as 1 for substantiated maltreatment or 0 for no substantiated maltreatment. Caregiver substance use problems were coded as 1 for substantiated problems and 0 for no substantiated problems.

Divorce and separation

Divorce and separation were assessed through two questions from the Child Life Events Scale (CLES) examining the past year: “Did anybody divorce?” and “Did anybody separate?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). For all ages, except for one assessment period, responses were coded as 0 = No or 1 = Yes. For ages 0–6 years, responses were coded as 0 = No, 1 = Yes, and 2 = Don't know. For this age, Don't know was counted as No, as the divorce or separation likely did not affect the child if the parent did not know about it. As a result, any time period with a divorce or separation was coded as a 1 and coded as a 0 if there was no divorce or separation.

Incarceration

Incarceration in the past year was assessed through one question from the CLES: “Was anyone in child's family or household jailed or imprisoned?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). There was a variation to this question for the 11–12 and 13–14 year time points, with the question: “Was anyone in the child's family or household jailed, imprisoned, or kept in a juvenile residential facility for breaking the law?” For all ages except one assessment period, responses were coded 0 = No, 1 = Yes. For ages 0–6 years old, responses were coded as 0 = No, 1 = Yes, and 2 = Don't know. For this age, Don't know was reported as No. Incarceration in a particular time period was thus coded as 1, while no incarceration was coded as 0.

Death

Death was assessed through one question from the CLES examining the past year: “Did anyone who was close to child die during the past year?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). There was a slight variation to this question for ages 11–12 and 13–14 years: “Was anyone who was close to [child] die during the past year?” For all ages except one assessment period, responses were coded 0 = No, 1 = Yes. For ages 0–6 years, 0 = No, 1 = Yes, and 2 = Don't know. For this age, Don't know was recoded as No. Death during a time period was coded as 1, while no death was coded as 0.

Foster care

Foster care was assessed through a two-part question from the CLES examining the past year. Participants were first asked if they moved. If they answered yes, they were then asked either if they moved to foster care or for a number of “moves to foster care” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). If they responded No to moving, they were not asked about foster care. Responses were coded as 0 = No if they had no moves to foster care and 1 = Yes if they had one or more moves to foster care.

Homelessness

To assess homelessness, one question was used from the CLES. For age 0–6 years, the question was: “Was the child ever homeless?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). The responses were 0 = No, 1 = Yes, 2 = Don't know. Again, Don't know was reported as No. For the remaining assessment time points, the Service Utilization Scale was used and examined the past year. For ages 8–11 years, the following questions were used: “Have you or [child] used or received a service like this: A homeless shelter?” and “Did you ever need a homeless shelter in the last year?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1991). For ages 12–16 years, the following questions were used: “In the last year, did you ever need a homeless shelter as a place to stay?” and “Did you stay at a homeless shelter, in the last year?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1998). If homelessness was indicated, participants were assigned a 1 for that time period and assigned a 0 for no homelessness.

Witnessed violence

From the CLES, witnessed violence was assessed using multiple questions examining the past year. For ages 0–6 years, the following five questions were asked: (a) “Has child seen anyone physically threatened with a weapon?” (b) “Did s/he see anyone get shot or stabbed (other than on TV or movies)?” (c) “Has s/he seen someone killed or murdered?” (d) “Did s/he witness anyone being sexually abused, assaulted, or raped?” (e) “Has s/he seen anyone getting hit, kicked, or physically harmed in some other way?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 1992). These responses were assessed as 0 = No, 1 = Yes, 2 = Don't know. Don't know was recoded as No. During the assessment of 8- to 15-year-olds, the same five questions were asked. Responses were coded as 0 = No, 1 = Yes. For the 16-year assessment, the Adolescent Witnessed Violence-Short Form was utilized (LONGSCAN Investigators, 2000). The following questions were assessed: (a) “In the last year, how often have you seen someone being slapped, kicked, hit with something, or beaten up?” (b) “In the last year, how often have you seen someone pull a gun, a knife, or another life-threatening weapon on another person?” (c) “In the last year, how many times have you seen someone get stabbed or cut with some type of weapon?” (d) “In the last year, how many times have you seen someone get shot?” (e) “In the last year, how many times have you seen someone get killed by another person?” (f) “In the last year, how many times have you seen someone getting sexually assaulted, molested, or raped?” (LONGSCAN Investigators, 2000). The response options to these questions were 0 = Never, 1 = one time, 2 = two or three times, 3 = four or more times. Responses were recoded as 0 = No and 1 = Yes (for one or more times). Any witnessed violence during a particular time point was assigned a 1 and no witnessed violence was assigned a 0.

Childhood adversities composite

Composite scores for childhood adversities could range between 0 and 11. To create a composite score, anyone that responded in the affirmative to experiencing an adversity in the respective category was given a score of 1. Those who responded they had not had an experience in the category were given a 0. The mean of the categories of childhood adversities experienced at a particular time period was calculated for those who had valid data on seven or more individual adversities at each age to get a composite score for each time period. The mean was multiplied by 11, the total number of possible adversities, so every child with enough data would have a score from 0 to 11 for that time period. The time periods were divided as follows: 0–6 years old, 7–8 years old, 9–10 years old, 11–12 years old, 13–14 years old, and 15–16 years old. To create a composite of the mean number of childhood adversities experienced from 0 to 16 years, a mean was calculated of the six individual childhood adversities scores (one from each time period) for those with valid data on three or more time points. To create a composite of the mean number of childhood adversities experienced from 0 to 12 years, a mean was calculated of the four individual childhood adversity scores (one from each time period) for those with valid data on two or more time points.

Self-harm and suicide

We extracted information on child SITB engagement from both parent-report and self-report. Parent-reports of SITB outcomes came from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991a). Self-report outcomes came from the Youth Self-Report Measure (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991b) and the Adolescent Health Status and Service Utilization Questionnaire (Knight, Smith, Martin, & Lewis, Reference Knight, Smith, Martin and Lewis1998).

From the CBCL, the following items were assessed at ages 12 and 16 years: (a) “deliberately harms self or attempts suicide” (self-harm) and (b) “talks about killing self” (suicidal ideation) (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991a). The responses for these questions were 0 = Not true, 1 = Somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = Very true or often true. For these questions, both a 1 or 2 were considered Yes as a response to the question. From the Youth Self-Report, the following questions were assessed at age 12: (a) “I deliberately try to hurt or kill myself” and (b) “I think about killing myself” (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991b). The responses for these questions were 0 = Not true, 1 = Somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = Very true or often true. For these questions also, both 1 or 2 were considered Yes. From the Adolescent Health Status and Service Utilization Questionnaire, the following questions were answered by youth at age 16 years old in reference to the past year: (a) “During the last 12 months, did you ever seriously consider attempting suicide?” (b) “During the last 12 months, did you make a plan about how you would attempt suicide?” (c) “During the last 12 months, how many times did you actually attempt suicide?” For all the SITB questions, “no” or “not true” were coded as 0, and “yes” or “somewhat or very true” were coded as 1 to indicate any self-harm or suicidal ideation. Any participants younger than 11 years or older than 13.5 years at the 12-year assessment were excluded from the analyses with SITB outcomes at 12 years. Any participants younger than 15 years or older than 17.5 years at the 16-year assessment were excluded from analyses with SITB outcomes at 16 years.

Demographics

Gender was coded as male = 1, female = 2.Footnote 1 For race/ethnicity, Black or African American served as the reference group, and non-Black or African American was coded as 1. Age at the 12- or 16-year assessment was included as a covariate depending on the timing of SITB assessment for the analysis.

Data analytic plan

Only participants who had nonmissing data on the demographic, adversity, and outcome variables were included in the analyses. The primary outcomes were self-reported (a) suicidal ideation, (b) suicide plan, and (c) suicide attempt at age 16 years. Three logistic regressions were conducted to predict whether childhood adversity at different time points (0–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–10 years, 11–12 years, 13–14 years, or 15–16 years) predicted each of the three dichotomous dependent variables. Gender, age, and race/ethnicity were included as covariates.

The secondary outcomes were used to examine variations in reporter and timing of suicide outcomes. The first set of secondary outcomes included parent-report of suicide outcomes measured dichotomously at age 16 years: (a) whether youth deliberately harms self or attempts suicide and (b) whether youth talks about killing self. Two logistic regressions were conducted with childhood adversity at different time points (0–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–10 years, 11–12 years, 13–14 years, or 15–16 years) as independent variables. Covariates included gender, age, and race/ethnicity. These analyses tested whether parent-reported suicidal ideation at 16 years produced similar results to the primary outcomes self-reported at 16 years.

The second set of secondary outcomes were self- and parent-reported suicide outcomes at 12 years, which were all measured dichotomously. The self-reported outcomes are (a) whether youth deliberately tries to hurt or kill self and (b) whether youth thinks about killing self. The parent-reported outcomes are (a) whether youth deliberately harms self or attempts suicide and (b) whether youth talks about killing self. Four logistic regressions were conducted with each of the self- and parent-reported suicide outcomes as dependent variables in separate analyses, with childhood adversity at different time points (0–6 years, 7–8 years, 9–10 years, and 11–12 years) as independent variables. Covariates included gender, age, and race/ethnicity. These analyses tested whether childhood adversity at specific time points predicted self- and parent-reported suicidal outcomes at age 12 years.

Results

Descriptive statistics

A total of 970 participants were included in either the 12-year or 16-year analyses (902 in the 12-year assessment, 744 in the 16-year assessment, and 676 in both the 12- and 16-year assessments). The higher number of childhood adversities from age 0 to 6 years is consistent with both the wider age range and recruitment strategy of selecting participants with maltreatment experiences or who were at risk for maltreatment in early childhood. At age 16 years, 8.6% of youth reported seriously considering attempting suicide, 5.2% had made a plan about how to attempt suicide, and 5.5% had attempted suicide in the past year. At 16 years of age, 4.4% of parents reported their child had deliberately harmed themselves or attempted suicide, while 5.4% reported their child had talked about suicide. At age 12 years, 3.7% reported deliberately harming themselves or attempting suicide, and 4.7% had thought about attempting suicide. At the same age, 3.4% of parents reported their child had deliberately harmed themselves or attempted suicide, while 4.5% reported their child had talked about suicide. A correlation table of associations between childhood adversities at different time points is included in the Supplementary Material, as well as means of childhood adversity by timing with each of the SITB outcomes.

Primary outcomes: Self-reported SITB at 16 years

Greater childhood adversities from age 0 to 16 years predicted self-reported suicidal ideation at age 16 years, B = 0.46, SE = 0.21, χ2 (1) = 5.01, p = .025, OR = 1.59, but not making a suicide plan, p = .50, or reporting a suicide attempt in the past year, p = .72. Greater childhood adversities at age 13–14 years predicted self-reported suicidal ideation at 16 years, B = 0.45, SE = 0.16, χ2 (1) = 8.26, p = .004, OR = 1.57 (Table 2). None of the childhood adversity timing variables predicted making a suicide plan at 16 years, all p > .19. Similar to suicidal ideation, greater childhood adversities at age 13–14 years predicted the likelihood of reporting a suicide attempt in the past year, B = 0.57, SE = 0.20, χ2 (1) = 8.25, p = .004, OR = 1.76.

Table 2. Results of regression analyses with self-reported 16-year outcomes

Note: p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. The reference group for race is Black or African American.

When each time point of childhood adversity was included in models separately, childhood adversity from age 13 to 14 years predicted seriously considering attempting suicide, B = 0.47, SE = 0.14, χ2 (1) = 11.36, p = .001, OR = 1.59 (Table S12 of the Supplementary Material), and actually attempting suicide, B = 0.44, SE = 0.17, χ2 (1) = 6.86, p = .009, OR = 1.55 (Table S13 of the Supplementary Material). No other childhood adversity timing variables were significant predictors of self-reported SITB outcomes at age 16 years.

Secondary outcomes

Parent-reported SITB at 16 years

Greater childhood adversity from age 0 to 16 years predicted greater parent-reported youth deliberately harming self or attempting suicide at 16 years, B = 0.56, SE = 0.26, χ2 (1) = 4.73, p = .030, OR = 1.75, but not youth talking about suicide, p = .11. Childhood adversity timing did not predict parent-reported youth deliberately harming self or attempting suicide at 16 years, all p > .11 (Table 3). Greater childhood adversity at 9–10 years, B = 0.39, SE = 0.17, χ2 (1) = 5.55, p = .018, OR = 1.48, and 13–14 years, B = 0.41, SE = 0.19, χ2 (1) = 4.93, p = .026, OR = 1.51, predicted parent-reported youth talking about suicide at age 16 years.

Table 3. Results of regression analyses with parent-reported 16-year outcomes

Note: p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. The reference group for race is Black or African American.

When each time point of childhood adversity was included in models separately, greater childhood adversity at age 0–6 years, B = 0.21, SE = 0.10, χ2 (1) = 4.38, p = .036, OR = 1.24 (Table S14 of the Supplementary Material), and 9–10 years, B = 0.32, SE = 0.16, χ2 (1) = 4.15, p = .042, OR = 1.38 (Table S15 of the Supplementary Material), predicted parent-reported youth harming self or attempting suicide at age 16 years. Greater childhood adversity at 9–10 years, B = 0.35, SE = 0.14, χ2 (1) = 5.91, p = .015, OR = 1.42 (Table S16 of the Supplementary Material), and 13–14 years, B = 0.38, SE = 0.16, χ2 (1) = 5.75, p = .016, OR = 1.46 (Table S17 of the Supplementary Material), predicted parent-reported youth talking about killing self at age 16 years.

Self- and parent-reported SITB at 12 years

The composite of childhood adversity from age 0 to 12 years did not predict self-reported deliberately harming self or attempting suicide, p = .10, self-reported thinking about killing self, p = .25, parent-reported youth self-harm or suicide attempt at age 12 years, p = .10, or parent-reported youth talking about suicide at 12 years, p = .052.

When all adversity timing variables were included in the model, no time point of childhood adversity predicted self-reported deliberately harming self or attempting suicide, all p > .16, or self-reported talking about suicide at age 12 years, all p > .14 (Table 4). Greater childhood adversity at age 9–10 years, B = 0.45, SE = 0.16, χ2 (1) = 7.49, p = .006, OR = 1.56, and at 11–12 years, B = 0.37, SE = 0.15, χ2 (1) = 5.80, p = .016, OR = 1.44, predicted greater likelihood of parent-reported youth self-harm and suicide attempt at age 12 years (Table 5). Greater childhood adversity at age 11–12 years, B = 0.35, SE = 0.14, χ2 (1) = 6.81, p = .009, OR = 1.42, predicted parent-reported youth talking about suicide at age 12 years.

Table 4. Results of regression analyses with self-reported 12-year outcomes

Note: p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. The reference group for race is Black or African American.

Table 5. Results of regression analyses with parent-reported 12-year outcomes

Note: p < .10, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. The reference group for race is Black or African American.

When each time point of childhood adversity was included in models separately, both childhood adversities at age 9–10 years, B = 0.44, SE = 0.15, χ2 (1) = 8.99, p = .003, OR = 1.55 (Table S18 of the Supplementary Material), and 11–12 years, B = 0.39, SE = 0.14, χ2 (1) = 7.96, p = .005, OR = 1.48 (Table S19 of the Supplementary Material), predicted parent-reported youth harming themselves or attempting suicide. Greater childhood adversities at age 11–12 years predicted parent-reported youth talking about killing themselves at 12 years, B = 0.37, SE = 0.13, χ2 (1) = 8.90, p = .003, OR = 1.45 (Table S20 of the Supplementary Material).

Discussion

Consistent with previous research (Bruwer et al., Reference Bruwer, Govender, Bishop, Williams, Stein and Seedat2014; Tunnard et al., Reference Tunnard, Rane, Wooderson, Markopoulou, Poon, Fekadu and Cleare2013), greater childhood adversity was associated with increased risk for SITB in this sample. Our results suggest that the timing of childhood adversities may help clarify the association between childhood adversity and SITB and thus inform prevention and intervention efforts. We found that self-reported suicidal ideation and suicide attempts at age 16 years were predicted by childhood adversity at age 13–14 years. In addition, parent-reported talking about suicide at 16 years old was predicted by childhood adversity at both 9–10 and 13–14 years old. We also found that parent-reported self-harm and suicide attempts at age 12 years were predicted by childhood adversity at 9–10 and 11–12 years old, and parent-reported talking about suicide at 12 years old was predicted by childhood adversity at 11–12 years old.

More specifically, we found that the mean level of adversity from 0 to 16 years and adversity specifically from 13 to 14 years predicted self-reported suicidal ideation at age 16 years, and only adversity from age 13 to 14 years predicted self-reported suicide attempts. These findings suggest some specificity for adversity from age 13 to 14 years for predicting suicide attempts versus ideation at age 16 years. However, when parents reported adolescents harming themselves or attempting suicide at age 16 years, the mean level of childhood adversity from age 0 to 16 years was a better predictor than adversity at any individual time point. One caveat is that the parent-reported measure did not disentangle NSSI and suicide attempts, so these self- and parent-reported outcomes cannot be directly compared. When all time points of adversity were examined in separate analyses – rather than compared directly in the same model – greater adversity from ages 0 to 6 years and from 9 to 10 years predicted parent-reported youth self-harm and suicide attempts at age 16 years. Together, these results suggest that some time periods could have greater impact on parent-reported youth SITB risk, though we will need larger samples with more specific SITB measurements to replicate and unpack this finding. Overall, the findings suggest that more recent adversities may set in motion developmental cascades of cognitive, emotional, social, and biological changes leading to greater SITB risk at age 16 years.

Interestingly, greater adversity at ages 9–10 and 13–14 years predicted parent-reported youth talking about killing themselves at age 16 years, though the mean level of adversity from age 0 to 16 years was not a predictor. This finding again supports the specificity of the timing of adversity predicting SITB outcomes rather than cumulative adversity. Finally, greater adversity at ages 9–10 and 11–12 years predicted parent-reported youth self-harm or suicide attempts at age 12 years, and greater adversity at 11–12 years predicted parent-reported youth talking about killing themselves at age 12 years. The mean level of adversity from age 0 to 12 years did not predict either of these outcomes, suggesting that more recent adversity better predicts parent-reported SITB outcomes at age 12 years than cumulative adversity. Similar to the 16-year SITB outcome analyses, developmental cascades of disrupted functioning in cognitive, emotional, social, and biological domains may result in greater SITB risk at age 12 years.

These findings are consistent with prior research examining 16-year-olds from the LONGSCAN study, where recent adversity from 12 to 16 years of age was most predictive of suicidal ideation at age 16 years (Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Litrownik, Isbell, Everson, English, Dubowitz and Flaherty2012a). Instead of using larger age groupings (e.g., 12–16 years old) as in the study by Thompson and colleagues, we examined childhood adversity in two-year intervals (e.g., 9–10 years old) to allow for a more nuanced assessment of the timing of adversity and SITB. Building on prior research, the present study included more comprehensive assessments of SITB across both adolescent self-report and parent-report at ages 12 and 16 years. The findings contradict the idea of early childhood being the most impactful period for later mental health (Zeanah, Gunnar, McCall, Kreppner, & Fox, Reference Zeanah, Gunnar, McCall, Kreppner and Fox2011). Although early childhood adversity predicts a range of psychopathology, it might not be as predictive for SITB at ages 12 and 16 years compared to more recent adversity. This research sheds light both on the potential power of recent adversity for predicting SITB and the potential for adversity occurring during or around the sensitive period of early adolescence and puberty for predicting SITB.

The current study can inform suicide prevention efforts. For example, if someone has experienced a great deal of adversity at age 11–12 years, it may be especially important to check in with them about SITB around age 12 to ensure they are supported and can access the services they need. Similarly, knowing someone has experienced a great deal of adversity at age 13–14 years, it may be particularly important to assess SITB around age 16 to ensure that these adolescents are getting the treatment they need.

The developmental psychopathology framework uses an approach with multiple levels of analysis to understand why childhood adversities may be associated with greater risk for SITB. This framework postulates that childhood adversity may negatively affect multiple domains of functioning soon after experiencing adversity, which can spread across even more domains of functioning as development proceeds – a process termed “developmental cascades” (Masten & Cicchetti, Reference Masten and Cicchetti2010). One downstream outcome of these negative developmental cascades following adversity may be greater risk for SITB. Potential mechanisms between childhood adversities and SITB in these developmental cascades include alterations in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis and brain development, cognitive biases, poorer executive functioning, emotion dysregulation, deficits in interpersonal functioning, fewer education and employment opportunities, and onset of psychopathology (Burton et al., Reference Burton, Vella, Weller and Twamley2011; Font & Maguire-Jack, Reference Font and Maguire-Jack2016; Ports et al., Reference Ports, Merrick, Stone, Wilkins, Reed, Ebin and Ford2017; Sachs-Ericsson et al., Reference Sachs-Ericsson, Rushing, Stanley and Sheffler2016; Shonkoff, Reference Shonkoff2016). Our results suggest that more recent adversities are associated with greater SITB risk at age 12 and 16 years, which points to mechanisms occurring closer in time to SITB outcomes as potentially being particularly important to target in interventions and future research.

Strengths

The present study surveyed youth and parents about childhood adversity prospectively rather than retrospectively in order to reduce problems with memory or reporting biases later in development. In addition, the sample was racially diverse, with more than half of the participants identifying as Black or African American. The sample primarily included children who had experienced maltreatment or who were at higher risk for maltreatment, which makes these findings particularly applicable to populations experiencing higher levels of adversity. Finally, there were multiple reporters of SITB in order to compare findings for both parent- and self-report.

Limitations

Although the study has numerous strengths, there are also notable limitations. The first limitation of the study was missing data. The present study intentionally included a large sample of youth at high risk for childhood maltreatment, with a primary focus on topics not related to SITB. As a result, it is not surprising that some adversity and SITB measures were missing or measured inconsistently across ages. For example, the time period of assessment (e.g., the past 2 weeks) for the CBCL was not defined in the measure, while some measures clearly defined the time period of assessment. An additional expected limitation was the limited sample size of those endorsing SITB. This limitation follows the push and pull of prediction, where researchers have to choose between large, general populations and populations at particularly high risk for suicide and self-harm. Future research seeking to better understand the prospective relationship between childhood adversity and SITB should consider using a larger sample, and perhaps a sample with familial risk for SITB. Such choices could increase statistical power to detect meaningful associations. In addition, several questions used assessed SITB outcomes not distinguished by suicidal intent, as is currently recommended. Future longitudinal studies should use updated measures of SITB outcomes that are in line with current recommendations in the field.

Like many longitudinal prediction studies (Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Ribeiro, Fox, Bentley, Kleiman, Huang and Nock2016), another limitation of this work is that we observed only small associations between childhood maltreatment and future suicidal thoughts and behaviors. Results continue to suggest that self-harm and suicidal thoughts and behaviors are complex outcomes that likely will not be strongly predicted by any given factor, including childhood abuse, which is often discussed as a critical predictor regardless of the age at which it is experienced. Lastly, many children were recruited because they experienced early maltreatment, so our sample tended to have greater adversity scores in early childhood rather than later in childhood or in adolescence. The oversampling for early adverse experiences may limit the variability of childhood adversities and affect the generalizability of findings.

Future directions

To better determine the strength and reliability of these findings and to replicate and extend the results, several future directions are needed. The present study intentionally included a large sample of youth who had experienced maltreatment or who were at high risk for childhood maltreatment. Although this study is unique in that it assessed risk for SITB over time, it was limited by inconsistent SITB measurement. Several different SITB measures were used across assessments, and several of these items conflated suicidal threats and behaviors. Future research should consider carefully assessing NSSI (i.e., intentional self-harm enacted without suicidal intent) separately from suicidal thoughts and behaviors in light of evidence showing critical distinctions in the etiology and course of these outcomes (e.g., Nock, Reference Nock2009). Future longitudinal studies should consider using consistent and precise language to assess engagement in suicidal and self-harming thoughts and behaviors to reduce error. Prior research demonstrates that the way questions about suicide are asked can greatly alter rates of endorsement (e.g., Nock & Kessler, Reference Nock and Kessler2006). Future research should also consider asking participants at younger ages about their own suicidal and self-harming thoughts and behaviors. Contrary to the common misconception, asking about suicide and self-harm does not increase the likelihood that people will have or act on those thoughts (e.g., DeCou & Schumann, Reference DeCou and Schumann2017); moreover, there is evidence that children as young as 4 years old endorse suicidal thinking and often understand key aspects of death (Hennefield, Whalen, Wood, Chavarria, & Luby, Reference Hennefield, Whalen, Wood, Chavarria and Luby2019). Finally, frequent assessment of suicidal and self-harming thoughts and behaviors may be useful as there is evidence that longer-term retrospective reports may miss prior experiences with these outcomes (Goldney, Smith, Winefield, Tiggerman, & Winefield, Reference Goldney, Smith, Winefield, Tiggerman and Winefield1991).

In addition to improving measures of suicidal and self-harming thoughts and behaviors, future studies should consider using more intensive assessments of childhood adversity. For example, multiple annual assessments of adversity may help to reduce memory biases that can occur after traumatic experiences (Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury, & Danese, Reference Baldwin, Reuben, Newbury and Danese2019). However, it is important to note that retrospective reports of experiences of childhood adversity have unique associations with health that do not always parallel concurrent reports (Danese & Widom, Reference Danese and Widom2020). In addition, although we were not sufficiently powered to examine both timing and type of childhood adversities in relation to SITB outcomes, we encourage researchers with access to larger samples of longitudinal, well-characterized participants to address these important questions.

Another consideration for the study is how demographic information is collected. Given that LGBTQ+ populations are at disproportionately high risk for SITB outcomes (CDC, 2016), future research should expand the binary coding of male/female for gender and incorporate a wide range of gender identities and sexual orientations.

Conclusion

The results of this study suggest that childhood adversity predicts suicide and self-harming outcomes in adolescence and the timing of these exposures impacts the strength of the association. Future research probing these associations using a more systematic assessment of childhood adversity and SITB will be critical to better understand potential mechanisms and implications for interventions.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000808

Acknowledgments

The data used in this publication were made available by the National Data Archive on Child Abuse and Neglect, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA, and were used with permission. Data from the LONGSCAN assessments were originally collected by Desmond K. Runyan, Howard Dubowitz, Diana J. English, Jonathan Kotch, Alan Litrownik, Richard Thompson, Terri Lewis, and the LONGSCAN Investigator Group.

Funding Statement

Funding for the project was provided by the Office on Child Abuse and Neglect (OCAN), Children's Bureau, Administration for Children and Families, Department of Health and Human Services. [The National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (NCCAN), under the Office of Human Services, funded this consortium of studies during the early years of data collection from 04/01/1991 until NCCAN became part of OCAN in 1998.] Award numbers: 90CA1467, 90CA1481, 90CA1466, 90CA1458, 90CA1572, 90CA1569, 90CA1568, 90CA1566, 90CA1678, 90CA1681, 90CA1680, 90CA1676, 90CA1677, 90CA1679, 90CA1744, 90CA1745, 90CA1746, 90CA1747, 90CA1748, and 90CA1749. The collector of the original data, the funder, National Data Archive on Child Abuse and Neglect, Cornell University, and their agents or employees bear no responsibility for the analyses or interpretations presented here. The research reported in this publication was supported by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health under award number K01HL143159 (PI: Doom). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Footnotes

1 Although this measure is labeled as gender in the codebook and dataset, we believe it may actually be a measure of biological sex at birth.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991a). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist 4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Retrieved from http://www.aseba.orgGoogle Scholar
Achenbach, T. M. (1991b). Manual for the Youth Self-Report and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Retrieved from http://www.aseba.orgGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. R., Reuben, A., Newbury, J. B., & Danese, A. (2019). Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76, 584593. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruwer, B., Govender, R., Bishop, M., Williams, D. R., Stein, D. J., & Seedat, S. (2014). Association between childhood adversities and long-term suicidality among South Africans from the results of the South African stress and health study: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open, 4, e004644. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2013-004644CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, C. Z., Vella, L., Weller, J. A., & Twamley, E. W. (2011). Differential effects of executive functioning on suicide attempts. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 23, 173179. doi:10.1176/jnp.23.2.jnp173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
CDC. (2019). Vital signs: Adverse childhood experiences. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/vitalsigns/aces/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
CDC. (2020). Data & statistics fatal injury report for 2018. Retrieved from https://afsp.org/suicide-statistics/Google Scholar
CDC (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention). (2016). Sexual identity, sex of sexual contacts, and health-risk behaviors among students in grades 9-12: Youth risk behavior surveillance. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/65/ss/pdfs/ss6509.pdfGoogle Scholar
Choi, N. G., Dinitto, D. M., Marti, C. N., & Segal, S. P. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and suicide attempts among those with mental and substance use disorders. Child Abuse & Neglect, 69, 252262. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2017.04.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 187211. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleare, S., Wetherall, K., Clark, A., Ryan, C., Kirtley, O. J., Smith, M., & O'Connor, R. C. (2018). Adverse childhood experiences and hospital-treated self-harm. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15, 1235. doi:10.3390/ijerph15061235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtin, S. C., & Heron, M. (2019). Death rates due to suicide and homicide among persons aged 10–24: United States, 2000–2017. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/data/databriefs/db352-h.pdfGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danese, A., & Widom, C. S. (2020). Objective and subjective experiences of child maltreatment and their relationships with psychopathology. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 811818. doi:10.1038/s41562-020-0880-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCou, C. R., & Schumann, M. E. (2017). On the iatrogenic risk of assessing suicidality: A meta-analysis. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48, 531543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dube, S. R., Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Chapman, D. P., Williamson, D. F., & Giles, W. H. (2001). Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: Findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 30893096. doi:10.1001/jama.286.24.3089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
English, D. J., & LONGSCAN Investigators. (1997). Modified Maltreatment Classification System (MMCS). Retrieved from http://www.iprc.unc.edu/longscan/Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M., Beautais, A. L., & Horwood, L. J. (2003). Vulnerability and resiliency to suicidal behaviours in young people. Psychological Medicine, 33, 6173. doi:10.1017/S0033291702006748CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Font, S. A., & Maguire-Jack, K. (2016). Pathways from childhood abuse and other adversities to adult health risks: The role of adult socioeconomic conditions. Child Abuse & Neglect, 51, 390399. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.05.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J. C., Ribeiro, J. D., Fox, K. R., Bentley, K. H., Kleiman, E. M., Huang, X., … Nock, M. K. (2016). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 187232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldney, R. D., Smith, S., Winefield, A. H., Tiggerman, M., & Winefield, H. R. (1991). Suicidal ideation: Its enduring nature and associated morbidity. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 83, 115120. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb07375.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hennefield, L., Whalen, D. J., Wood, G., Chavarria, M. C., & Luby, J. L. (2019). Changing conceptions of death as a function of depression status, suicidal ideation, and media exposure in early childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58, 339349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howe, T. R. (2016). A quiet revolution: Developmental psychopathology as a cutting edge framework for understanding abuse and neglect. PsycCRITIQUES, 61(35). 10.1037/a0040493.Google Scholar
Kaess, M., Parzer, P., Mattern, M., Plener, P. L., Bifulco, A., Resch, F., & Brunner, R. (2013). Adverse childhood experiences and their impact on frequency, severity, and the individual function of nonsuicidal self-injury in youth. Psychiatry Research, 206, 265272. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.10.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, E. D., Smith, J. B., Martin, L., Lewis, T., & LONGSCAN Investigators. (1998). Measures for assessment of functioning and outcomes in longitudinal research on child abuse. Volume 3: Early adolescence (ages 12–14). Retrieved from http://longscan.research.unc.edu/Google Scholar
LONGSCAN Investigators. (1991). LONGSCAN service utilization [instrument]. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Injury Prevention Research Center. Retrieved from http://longscan.research.unc.edu/Google Scholar
LONGSCAN Investigators. (1992). LONGSCAN child's life events [instrument adapted from the Coddington Child Life Events Scales]. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Injury Prevention Research Center. Retrieved from http://longscan.research.unc.edu/Google Scholar
LONGSCAN Investigators. (1998). LONGSCAN service utilization [instrument]. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Injury Prevention Research Center. Retrieved from http://longscan.research.unc.edu/Google Scholar
LONGSCAN Investigators. (2000). LONGSCAN adolescent witnessed violence [instrument]. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Retrieved from: Retrieved from http://longscan.research.unc.edu/Google Scholar
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491495. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000222CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGowan, P. O., Sasaki, A., D'Alessio, A. C., Dymov, S., Labonté, B., Szyf, M., … Meaney, M. J. (2009). Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nature Neuroscience, 12, 342348. doi:10.1038/nn.2270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
NCIPC (National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, Division of Violence Prevention). (2020). Preventing adverse childhood experiences. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/childabuseandneglect/aces/fastfact.htmlGoogle Scholar
Nock, M. K. (2009). Understanding nonsuicidal self-injury: Origins, assessment, and treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nock, M. K., & Kessler, R. C. (2006). Prevalence of and risk factors for suicide attempts versus suicide gestures: Analysis of the national comorbidity survey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ports, K. A., Merrick, M. T., Stone, D. M., Wilkins, N. J., Reed, J., Ebin, J., & Ford, D. C. (2017). Adverse childhood experiences and suicide risk: Toward comprehensive prevention. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 53, 400403. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2017.03.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runyan, D., Dubowitz, H., English, D. J., Kotch, J. B., Litrownik, A., Thompson, R., & the LONGSCAN Investigator Group. (2014). Longitudinal Studies of Child Abuse and Neglect (LONGSCAN) assessments 0–18. Retrieved from http://www.ndacan.cornell.eduGoogle Scholar
Sachs-Ericsson, N. J., Rushing, N. C., Stanley, I. H., & Sheffler, J. (2016). In my end is my beginning: Developmental trajectories of adverse childhood experiences to late-life suicide. Aging & Mental Health, 202, 139165. doi:10.1080/13607863.2015.1063107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serafini, G., Canepa, G., Adavastro, G., Nebbia, J., Belvederi Murri, M., Erbuto, D., … Amore, M. (2017). The relationship between childhood maltreatment and non-suicidal self-injury: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 149. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P. (2016). Capitalizing on advances in science to reduce the health consequences of early childhood adversity. JAMA Pediatrics, 170, 10031007. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2016.1559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swannell, S. V., Martin, G. E., Page, A., Hasking, P., & St John, N. J. (2014). Prevalence of nonsuicidal self-injury in nonclinical samples: Systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 44, 273303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, M. P., Kingree, J. B., & Lamis, D. (2019). Associations of adverse childhood experiences and suicidal behaviors in adulthood in a U.S. nationally representative sample. Child: Care, Health and Development, 45, 121128. doi:10.1111/cch.12617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R., Litrownik, A. J., Isbell, P., Everson, M. D., English, D. J., Dubowitz, H., … Flaherty, E. G. (2012a). Adverse experiences and suicidal ideation in adolescence: Exploring the link using the LONGSCAN samples. Psychology of Violence, 2, 211225. doi:10.1037/a0027107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R., Proctor, L. J., English, D. J., Dubowitz, H., Narasimhan, S., & Everson, M. D. (2012b). Suicidal ideation in adolescence: Examining the role of recent adverse experiences. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 175186. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tunnard, C., Rane, L., Wooderson, S., Markopoulou, K., Poon, L., Fekadu, A., & Cleare, A. (2013). The impact of childhood adversity on suicidality and clinical course in treatment-resistant depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 152–154, 122130. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.037Google ScholarPubMed
Wan, Y., Chen, J., Sun, Y., & Tao, F. (2015). Impact of childhood abuse on the risk of non-suicidal self-injury in mainland Chinese adolescents. PLoS One, 10, e0131239. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0131239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO (World Health Organization). (2019). Suicide. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/suicideGoogle Scholar
Zeanah, C. H., Gunnar, M. R., McCall, R. B., Kreppner, J. M., & Fox, N. A. (2011). Sensitive periods. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76, 147162. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00631.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Table 2. Results of regression analyses with self-reported 16-year outcomes

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of regression analyses with parent-reported 16-year outcomes

Figure 3

Table 4. Results of regression analyses with self-reported 12-year outcomes

Figure 4

Table 5. Results of regression analyses with parent-reported 12-year outcomes

Supplementary material: File

North et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S20

Download North et al. supplementary material(File)
File 48.3 KB