Skip to main content
Log in

Construction of subitized units is related to the construction of arithmetic units

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigates the relationship between children’s subitizing activity and their construction of arithmetic units. In particular, the study hypothesizes a positive association between children’s construction of subitized units and their construction of arithmetic units, and hypothesizes that children who can subitize larger units, such as five items, in kindergarten, are more likely to construct arithmetic units by the beginning of first grade. Data for this study were drawn from 3,660 children surveyed at the beginning of kindergarten in 2014, 2015, and 2016, and at the beginning of first grade in the following year. The children are from a single school district in the southwest United States. Logistic regression was used to model the likelihood of constructing arithmetic units based on children’s earlier construction of subitized units. Findings provide evidence of a positive relationship between children’s construction of subitized units and arithmetic units, and, on average, children who have constructed subitized units at the beginning of kindergarten are more likely to construct arithmetic units by the beginning of first grade. Based on the findings, theoretical and instructional implications are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. For our theoretical framing, we focus our attention only on the first stage of the number sequences, the INS. However, the number sequences consist of a hierarchy of four stages (Steffe, 1992; Ulrich, 2015, 2016): (1) initial number sequence, (2) tacitly nested number sequence (TNS), (3) explicitly nested number sequence (ENS), and (4) generalized number sequence (GNS). Each stage represents a more sophisticated understanding of number in terms of arithmetic units and the coordination of those units when working with the counting numbers. Briefly, the construction of an INS involves the coordination of arithmetic units of one, the TNS involves additive coordinations of units of units (composite units), the ENS includes multiplicative coordination of composite units, and finally the GNS involves the coordination of units of units of units.

  2. A comparison of kindergarteners with missing data to kindergartners without missing data by gender, ethnicity, evidence of subitizing, and evidence of arithmetic units in kindergarten found no differences in the groups by gender, ethnicity, and evidence of arithmetic units. A statistically significant difference was found for subitizing; however, the effect size associated with this relationship, Cramer’s V = .045, did not substantiate practical significance. Given no practical relationship between missingness and the demographic and study variables, we felt comfortable proceeding with the reduced sample.

References

  • Antell, S. E., & Keating, D. P. (1983). Perception of numerical invariance in neonates. Child, Perception of Numerical Invariance in Neonates Development, 54, 695–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakker, A., Cai, J., English, L., Kaiser, G., Mesa, V., & Van Dooren, W. (2019). Beyond small, medium, or large: Points of consideration when interpreting effect sizes. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 102(1), 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, S., & Norton, A. (2016). Co-construction of fractions schemes and units coordinating structures. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 41, 10–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyce, S., & Norton, A. (2019). Maddies’ units coordinating across contexts. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 55, 100696. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2019.03.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & MacDonald, B. L. (2019). Subitizing: The neglected quantifier. (pp. 13-45). In A. Norton & M. W. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing number: Merging perspectives from psychology and mathematics education. Springer.

  • De Smedt, B. (2019). The complexity of basic number processing: A commentary from a neurocognitive perspective. (pp. 123-132). In A. Norton & M. W. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing Number: Merging perspectives from Psychology and Mathematics Education. Springer.

  • Demeyere, N., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2012). The neuroanatomy of visual emmuneration: Differentiating necessary neural correlates for subitizing versus counting in a neuropsychological voxel-based morphometry study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 948–964.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackenberg, A. J. (2007). Units coordination and the construction of improper fractions: A revision of the splitting hypothesis. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26(1), 27–47.

  • Hackenberg, A. J., & Tillema, E. S. (2009). Students’ whole number multiplicative concepts: A critical constructive resource for fraction composition schemes. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 28(1), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hosmer Jr., D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (2000). Applied logistic regression (2nd ed.). Wiley.

  • Jevons, W. S. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination. Nature, 3(67), 281–282. https://doi.org/10.1038/003281a0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, E. L., Lord, M. W., Reese, T. W., & Volkmann, J. (1949). The discrimination of visual number. The American Journal of Psychology, 62(4), 498–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, B., Pack, Y. H., & Yi, S. H. (2017). Subitizing in children and adults, depending on the object individuation level of stimulus: Focusing on performance according to spacing, color, and shape of objects. Family and Environment Research, 55(5), 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D. (1973a). Quantification processes. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 3–31). Academic Press.

  • Klahr, D. (1973b). A production system for counting, subitizing, and adding. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 527–544). Academic Press.

  • Le Corre, M., Van de Walle, G. A., Brannon, E. M., & Carey, S. (2006). Re-visiting the competence/performance debate in the acquisition of counting as a representation of the positive integers. Cognitive Psychology, 52(2), 130–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, B. L. (2018). Relationships between units coordination and subitizing. In T. E. Hodges, G. J. Roy, & A. M. Tyminski (Eds.), Proceedings of the 40th Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 155–162). University of South Carolina & Clemson University.

  • MacDonald, B. L., & Shumway, J. F. (2016). Subitizing games: Assessing preschool children’s number understanding. Teaching Children Mathematics, 22(6), 340–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, B. L., & Wilkins, J. L. M. (2016). Seven types of subitizing activity characterizing young children’s mental activity (pp. 256-286). In S. Marx (Ed.), Qualitative research in STEM. Routledge.

  • MacDonald, B. L., & Wilkins, J. L. M. (2019). Subitising activity relative to units construction: A case study. Research in Mathematics Education, 21(1), 77–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, B. L., Moss, D. L., & Hunt, J. H. (2020). Dominoes: Promoting units construction and coordination. Mathematics Teacher: Learning and Teaching Pre-K–12, 113(7), 551–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., & Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111(1), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nabors, W. K. (2003). From fractions to proportional reasoning: A cognitive schemes of operation approach. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 22(2), 133–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, A., & Wilkins, J. L. M. (2012). The splitting group. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43(5), 557–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, J. (1999). From fractions to rational numbers of arithmetic: A reorganization hypothesis. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 1(4), 279–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivier, J., May, W. L., & Bell, M. L. (2017). Relative effect sizes for measures of risk. Communications in statistics: Theory and methods, 46(14), 6774–6781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, C.-Y. J., Lee, K. L., & Ingersoll, G. M. (2002). An introduction to logistic regression analysis and reporting. Journal of Educational Research, 96(1), 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1942). The child’s conception of number. Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. Columbia University Press.

  • Piazza, M., Mechelli, A., Butterworth, B., & Price, C. J. (2002). Are subitizing and counting implemented as separate or functionally overlapping processes? Neuroimage, 15(2), 435–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisier, M. A., Tiest, W. M. B., & Kappers, A. M. (2010). Range dependent processing of visual numerosity: Similarities across vision and haptics. Experimental brain research, 204, 525–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Revkin, S. K., Piazza, M., Izard, V., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2008). Does subitizing reflect numerical estimation? Psychological Science, 19(6), 607–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarama, J., & Clements, D. H. (2009). Early childhood mathematics education research: Learning trajectories for young children. Routledge.

  • SAS Institute, & Inc. (2012). SAS for windows [Version 9.4]. SAS Institute, Inc..

  • Sayers, J., Andrews, P., & Boistrup, L. B. (2016). The role of conceptual subitising in the development of foundational number sense. In Mathematics education in the early years (pp. 371–394). Springer.

  • Shin, J., Lee, S. J., & Steffe, L. P. (2020). Problem solving activities of two middle school students with distinct levels of units coordination. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 59, 100793. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2020.100793

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, S., & Castellan Jr., N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). McGraw Hill.

  • Simpson, A. (2020). On the misinterpretation of effect size. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 103(1), 125–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sophian, C., & Crosby, M. E. (2008). What eye fixation patterns tell us about subitizing. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 394–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., & Cooper Jr., R. G. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 399–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, P., Spelke, E. S., & Gelman, R. (1990). Numerical abstraction by human infants. Cognition, 36, 97–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (1991a). The learning paradox: A plausible counterexample. In L. P. Steffe (Ed.), Epistemological foundations of mathematical experience (pp. 26–44). Springer.

  • Steffe, L. P. (1991b). Operations that generate quantity. Learning and individual differences, 3(1), 61–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (1992). Schemes of action and operation involving composite units. Learning and Individual Differences, 4(3), 259–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (1994). Children’s multiplying schemes. In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The development of multiplicative reasoning in the learning of mathematics (pp. 3–39). SUNY Press.

  • Steffe, L. P. (2002). A new hypothesis concerning children’s fractional knowledge. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20(3), 267–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (2010). Operations that produce numerical counting schemes. In L. P. Steffe & J. Olive (Eds.), Children’s fractional knowledge (pp. 27–47). Springer.

  • Steffe, L. P., & Olive, J. (2010). Children’s fractional knowledge. Springer.

  • Steffe, L. P., von Glasersfeld, E., Richards, J., & Cobb, P. (1983). Children’s counting types: Philosophy, theory, and application. Praeger Scientific.

  • Trick, L. M., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. Psychological Review, 101, 80–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, C. (2015). Stages in constructing and coordinating units additively and multiplicatively (part 1). For the Learning of Mathematics, 35(3), 2–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, C. (2016). Stages in constructing and coordinating units additively and multiplicatively (part 2). For the Learning of Mathematics, 36(1), 34–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Loosbroek, E., & Smitsman, A. W. (1990). Visual perception of numerosity in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 916–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1981). An attentional model for the conceptual construction of units and number. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 83–94.

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1982). Subitizing: The role of figural patterns in the development of numerical concepts. Archives de Psychologie.

  • Wagner, S. H., & Walters, J. (1982). A longitudinal analysis of early number concepts: From numbers to number. Action and thought: From sensorimotor schemes to symbolic operations, 137–161.

  • Wilkins, J. L. M., Woodward, D., & Norton, A. (2020). Children’s number sequences as predictors of later mathematical development. Mathematics Education Research Journal. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13394-020-00317-y

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank David Woodward for making it possible to obtain and use the data in this study and for helping with understanding the interview protocols and data collection.

Code availability

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jesse L. M. Wilkins.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

Approval for human subject research was obtained from the first author’s institutional review board prior to beginning work on this research.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wilkins, J.L.M., MacDonald, B.L. & Norton, A. Construction of subitized units is related to the construction of arithmetic units. Educ Stud Math 109, 137–154 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-021-10076-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10649-021-10076-7

Keywords

Navigation