Elsevier

Brain and Language

Volume 221, October 2021, 104994
Brain and Language

Positive emotion of self-referential contexts could facilitate adult’s novel word learning: An fNIRS study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2021.104994Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Emotions inherent in self or other referential contexts pose important impacts on adults’ novel word learning.

  • Besides the finding of self-promoting effects, there were a prominent self-positivity bias during novel word learning.

  • The DLPFC and inferior frontal gyrus played key roles in novel word learning under contexts.

Abstract

Learning words through contextual inference is a key way to enlarge one’s vocabulary especially for adults. However, few studies focused on the effects of different information contained in contexts on novel word learning. The present study used behavioral and fNIRS techniques to investigate the influences of positive, neutral and negative emotions inherent in self-related or other-related referential contexts. Participants were asked to perform a semantic consistency and a source judgment task after learning the relations between novel words and concepts in different contexts. The results showed that self-reference during lexical encoding could promote word learning generally. More importantly, there existed a self-positivity bias which is manifested in the significant interactions between contextual emotions and referential value. These interactions are related to the neural activities of the DLPFC and IFG. These results revealed the contextual information’s integrative contributions to semantic meaning acquisition and episodic source memory related with novel word learning.

Introduction

Words are basic elements of language, so learning words is the first step to acquire a new language (Lesage, Nailer, & Miall, 2016), which becomes increasingly necessary under the current circumstances of globalization. Therefore, more and more researchers start to focus on investigating the mechanisms of word learning, especially the learning process under different contexts (Chen et al., 2014, Dautriche and Chemla, 2014, Liu, Ding, Li, & Yang, 2019, Mestres-Missé, Muente, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2009, Wang & Mintz, 2018), since contextual learning largely mimics the natural language learning situations. Besides predictive semantic information, contexts can also contain emotional connotations or have self-referencing value for language learners. Hence the learning mechanisms of novel words are probably influenced by the contextual information. Therefore, the current study aimed at revealing the effects and neuro-mechanisms of contextual emotions and self-reference on novel word learning by employing behavioral measurement and functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) technique, which is a method to access the cortical activities by measuring the concentration change of oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO), deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb) and their sum total in the cerebral cortices.

Previous studies have shown that readers could use contextual information to infer a novel word’s meaning (Chen et al., 2014, Dautriche and Chemla, 2014, Mestres-Missé, Camara, Rodriguez-Fornells, Rotte, & Muente, 2008, Mestres-Missé, Muente, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2009). Through monitoring readers’ eye movements during reading pairs of sentences containing a target word, a correspondent context and a target-related word, Chaffin and his colleagues (2001) found that participants would spend more time on the informative contexts every time the target words were novel, and if the contexts were neutral or uninformative, they would turn to the target-related words so as to infer the meanings of the novel target words. Similarly, Mestres-Missé and her colleagues (2008) asked participants to infer the meanings of novel words embedded in contexts with different restrictions, and found that participants could only infer the meaning of new words across semantically consistent contexts but not across semantically inconsistent contexts. Furthermore, researchers compared the learning effects of concrete words versus abstract words and nouns versus verbs using similar learning paradigm, and found that learning different types of words implicated different brain areas (Mestres-Missé, Muente, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2009, Mestres-Missé, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Muente, 2010). Thus, it’s repeatedly found that adults can learn different conceptual meaning from contexts.

As for the effects of emotions contained in contexts, most of research focused on existing word processing other than novel words learning (Guo, Li, Zhang, Cui, & Wei, 2018, Smith et al., 2004, Zhang, Liu, An, Yang, & Wang, 2015). Emotional information was mainly manipulated into two dimensions: valence and arousal. Arousal is a continuous value varied from boredom/calm to anxiety/excitement, while valence is a continuous value varied from unpleasant to pleasant (Bradley, Greenwald, Petry, & Lang, 1992). The two dimensions of emotions can play important roles in learning (Kensinger, 2009). In a ERPs study, participants were asked to study neutral words superimposed onto emotional images during encoding session and performed a classic old/new judgment task in testing session. The ERPs data collected during encoding session revealed that items presented in the positive and negative high-arousing contexts relative to neutral context elicited more positive ERP deflections. During retrieval phase, FN400 old/new effects occurred for words learned in the negative low-arousing, positive, and neutral contexts but not in the negative high-arousing context, while LPC old/new effects occurred for all conditions of contexts. The results showed that emotional contexts could pose impacts on both familiarity and recollection processes, and emotional arousal irrespective of valence seemed to impair the memorization of words generally (Zhang et al., 2015). A recent study has also shown that participants paid less attention to words presented in the high-arousing contexts compared with the neutral contexts during word identification, suggesting that it might be effortful to overcome the interference of arousal contexts during the process of memory formation (Guo et al., 2018). However, Maratos and Rugg (2001) observed a larger old/new ERP effect for words studied in negative compared to neutral sentences when contextual information could be retrieved incidentally. Therefore, it seemed that both arousal and valence of emotional contexts have significant effects on cognitive processing for words.

Using fMRI techniques, Maratos, Dolan, Morris, Henson, and Rugg (2001) continued to investigate the dynamic activities of neural networks correlated with neutral word learning under emotional contexts. Participants were asked to perform an old/new recognition task after learning emotionally neutral words presented in sentential contexts that were either negative, neutral or positive. The results showed that the recognition of words presented in negative relative to neutral contexts was associated with increased activities in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, left amygdala and hippocampus, right lingual gyrus and posterior cingulate cortex. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is a neural base of general executive function in language processing (Raboyeau, Marcotte, Adrover-Roig, & Ansaldo, 2010), and is a part of attentional control network (Botvinick, 2007). The activations of amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex and hippocampus may reflect the retrieval and recognition of emotional contexts and items (Dolan et al., 2000, Maratos, Dolan, Morris, Henson, & Rugg, 2001), while the activations of anterior temporal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex may be related to emotional experience and episodic memory (Mesulam, 1985, Maddock, 1999). Similarly, Smith and his colleagues (2004) found that retrieving neutral images encoded in emotional contexts elicited enhanced activations of brain regions involved in episodic memory. Collectively, brain areas correlated with emotion processing, cognitive control, semantic and episodic memory probably are neural substrates for word learning under emotional contexts.

In stark contrast to abundant researches into the influential mechanisms of contextual emotions on existing words processing, there are few studies concerning about novel word learning. Although some studies confirmed that novel words’ concept could be acquired through different sentential contexts (such as Chen et al., 2014, Mestres-Missé, Muente, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2014, Mestres-Missé, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Muente, 2010, Qiao & Forster, 2013; Ye, Mestres-Missé, Rodriguez-Fornells, & Muente, 2011), they did not take the emotions of contexts into consideration. Using behavior and fMRI techniques, recent studies have examined the influences of individual’s emotional states and sentential emotional information on novel word learning. The results showed that negative contexts impeded the learning process of novel words, and contextual emotions moderated the brain functional changes induced by novel words learning (Guo et al., 2018, Guo et al., 2018). Furthermore, Snefjella, Lana, and Kuperman (2020) investigated whether novel words could acquire emotional connotations based on linguistic contexts and whether these connotations will affect the quality of word learning. During learning phases, they asked participants to learn nine novel words presented in positive, neutral or negative contexts, and tested their learning quality of novel words’ orthographic and semantic information. The results showed that participants could learn both the emotional connotations, forms and semantic information of novel words from the contexts, and participants learned novel words more accurately in the positive contexts compared to the neutral and negative contexts. Therefore, it seems that positive contexts probably facilitate the word learning but negative contexts impede.

Regarding the impeding effects of negative contexts on words learning, some researchers thought that individuals were automatically alert to negative contexts, since the information contained in negative contexts is more urgent and may threaten a person’s survival. Consequently, negative contexts would take up a lot of attention resources, so that individual’s performances in processing words in negative contexts are relatively poor (Pratto & John, 1991). Studies have also shown that individuals are more sensitive to negative information (Cacioppo & Berntson, 1994) and react to it more strongly (Taylor, 1991), which may cause individuals to spend more attention on negative contexts, thereby ignoring words. With regard to the facilitative roles of positive contexts in learning, the broaden-and-build theory can give us a reasonable explanation (Fredrickson, 2001, Fredrickson, 2013). It’s proposed that positive emotions could broaden an individual's momentary thought-action repertoires (e.g., creativity, flexibility, high efficiency), and then build the individual's personal resources (intellectual, physical, psychological and social resources), thereby bring long-term benefits to the individual (Fredrickson, 2001, Fredrickson, 2013). Taken together, negative and positive emotion probably pose opposite effects on word learning through narrowing or broadening persons’ attention resources.

Self-referencing value is also a key property of learning contexts. Self-reference effects refer to a better memory performance on self-relevant words and the function of self seems to be a superordinate schema in encoding process (Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker, 1977). Klein (2012) argued that self-reference could strengthen semantic links among materials, so individuals could automatically organize self-relevant materials. Many studies consistently documented self-reference effects, showing that self-relevant stimuli were more elaborate and processed faster (eg., Fan et al., 2013, Tacikowski et al., 2011). Researchers also studied self-reference effects in learning activities and found that self-reference played a key role in memory formation, organization and retrieval (Conway & Dewhurst, 2010). D'Ailly and his colleagues (1997) required children to solve mathematical problems in different referential conditions and found that in self-referential condition, children were less likely to repeat the questions and their performances were better than that in other-referential condition, indicating that self-referential encoding helped children find solutions. These findings were echoed by the Cunningham and his colleagues’ study (2018), in which children tended to learn self-owned shapes better than shapes owned by experimenter or un-owned. As for novel word learning, children showed 20% higher spelling accuracy for a novel word when it was compiled in a sentence with themselves as the subject of the sentence than with another meaningless words as the subject. It’s proposed that the self-relevant information is stored by children in their self-schema, which is connected with the learning words, stimulates learners’ interests and motivation, and makes the learning goals meaningful, thereby helps them to maintain and retrieve words (Kihlstrom, Beer, & Klein, 2003).

The medial prefrontal cortex tends to be implicated in self-referential processing (Tacikowski et al., 2011). Imafuku and his colleagues (2014) employed fNIRS to detect the brain activities of infants when they heard names and found that their own names induced greater activations in the medial prefrontal cortex. Northoff and his colleagues (2006) summarized 27 studies on self-reference and found that self-referential processing was closely related to the midline cortex, which was consistent with another study (Murray, Marie, & Martin, 2012). Additionally, studies suggested that right inferior prefrontal gyrus was crucial to self-evaluation (Morita et al., 2008) and self-related stimuli (Sugiura et al., 2000). Some researchers indicated that the inferior prefrontal gyrus was a part of the frontal-parietal attentional control network and played a crucial role in cognitive inhibition (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, 2014).

Self-reference and contextual emotion might jointly affect word processing. Fossati and his colleagues (2003) asked participants to judge whether a negative or positive personality trait could describe themselves and found that the right dorsomedial prefrontal cortex was uniquely activated in self-referential condition regardless of the valence of the words. They argued that the activations were not probably related to emotional contexts per se but were attributed to the self-referential processing of the emotional stimuli. In addition, researchers found that the positive adjectives encoded in reference to the self were better recalled than negative ones only in a retrieval task entailing cognitive control (D'Argembeau, Comblain, & Van der Linden, 2005). Moreover, researchers proposed a self-positivity bias which referred to that most individuals prefer to link self-relevant information to positive emotion (Pahl & Eiser, 2005). A recent fNIRS study detected brain activities when participants were reporting corresponding emotional feelings on evaluating art works from the perspective of themselves or artists. Oxyhemoglobin (HbO2) analysis showed a significant activation in the rostrate prefrontal cortex in other-reference condition while Deoxyhemoglobin (HHb) results displayed an activation of this region in self-positive condition (Kreplin & Fairclough, 2015). Above mentioned studies underscored the role of the prefrontal cortex in processing referential and emotional information, so the present study will focus on observing activities in this region.

Based on above reviews, the emotional information and self-referential value in contexts are very important factors for language processing and learning. However, to our knowledge, there is no study to examine the potential interactive influences of these two factors on novel words learning. Novel word learning necessitates mapping a new concept to a specific linguistic symbol, and self-referential might well facilitate this process since self-reference can strengthen semantic links among materials (Klein, 2012). Given the bias of self-reference to positive information, we expected that there would be significant interactions between contextual reference and emotions.

Hence, reference and emotions of contexts were orthogonally manipulated to explore their interactive effects on novel word learning with behavioral and fNIRS techniques in the current study. Participants were required to learn the meanings of novel words which could be inferred from sentential contexts during learning session. During test session, they were asked to perform a semantic consistency task and a source judgement task. The semantic consistency task required participants to determine whether the learned novel words were consistent with the corresponding real words so as to check the learning quality of semantic information. We expected that there would be a self-referential advantage on learning novel words compared with other-referential condition especially for words learned in positive contexts. Furthermore, self-reference might involve enhanced activations in the prefrontal cortex compared to other-reference, and the emotional effects might be more prominent in self-referential contexts. Episodic memory is an ability to remember the bindings of the to-be-recalled items and the aspects of contexts (Andrews et al., 2020, Ngo, Horner, Newcombe, & Olson, 2019). Since self-referential contexts probably activate self-schema of participants, it’s reasonable to expect that self-reference would facilitate the memory of learning background. Therefore, after the semantic consistency judgment task, participants were also asked to perform a source judgment task, in which they were asked to recall the referential contexts in which the novel words were learned. We predicted that individuals could better recall the reference information of novel words in the self-referential and positive contexts.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

In experiment 1, we examined the potential effects of contextual reference and emotions on novel words learning behaviorally.

Experiment 2

In experiment 2, we adopted fNIRS technology to detect the neural activities related with the learning process.

Semantic consistency judgment task

The accuracy analyses showed a main effect of emotion, F (2, 56) = 3.818, p = 0.028, η2 = 0.120. Participants were less accurate in judging the novel words learned in negative contexts (0.814 ± 0.029) relative to positive contexts (0.860 ± 0.022), p = 0.022; the differences between negative context(0.814 ± 0.029) and neutral context (0.843 ± 0.028) were marginally significant, p = 0.065. However, the differences between neutral context (0.843 ± 0.028) and positive context (0.860 ± 0.022) were

Discussion

In the semantic consistency judgment task, self-reference effects and interactions of reference and emotion were observed from fNIRS results. It’s revealed that novel words learned in self-referential contexts induced an enhanced activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). On the basis of the self-schema theory (Markus & Ruvolo, 1989), we assumed that once the schema (an enhanced elaboration and organization, containing the past, present and future, and even beliefs,

General discussion

The present study explored the effects and neural bases of contextual reference and emotions on novel words learning. The results indicated that firstly, self-reference could promote lexical learning, and self-relevant context probably formed stronger and more elaborate semantic representations; Secondly, emotional effects on word learning were modulated by contextual reference, showing a self-positivity bias; finally, fNIRS data analyses revealed that the left DLPFC was implicated in the

Limitations and future work

There are limitations in the current work. Firstly, to control for the gender bias of emotional effects, participants in our study were all females, so findings must be treated cautiously and might be problematical to be generalized to a group level. Future work shall concern gender effects on novel words learning. Secondly, we only investigated the effects at sentence level, while paragraph and discourse are another two kinds of contexts which are rich in information (Hao, Liang, Wang, Liu, &

Conclusion

In summary, the current work showed that words learned in self-reference contexts showed a semantic retrieving advantage, indicating that self-reference could promote lexical encoding and form more elaborate semantic representations, so self-referencing is an effective encoding strategy in word learning. Secondly, the right DLPFC and the right inferior prefrontal gyrus showed enhanced activations in negative self-reference condition in semantic consistency judgement task, and the left

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Acknowledgements

Funding: This work was supported by project of Humanities and Social Sciences of General MOE (Ministry of Education in China) [grant number 18YJA190002]; Major project of medicine science and technology of PLA [grant number AWS17J012]; and Social Science Funds of Shaanxi Province [2019M031].

References (101)

  • M.R. Dulas et al.

    The effects of aging on ERP correlates of source memory retrieval for self-referential information

    Brain Research

    (2011)
  • B. Dunst et al.

    Neural efficiency as a function of task demands

    Intelligence

    (2014)
  • C.L. Fales et al.

    Altered emotional interference processing in affective and cognitive-control brain circuitry in major depression

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • B.L. Fredrickson

    Positive Emotions Broaden and Build

  • X. Guo et al.

    Weakening of recognition memory caused by task-irrelevant emotional encoding context can be modulated by individuals’ inhibitory control

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2018)
  • G.P. Hallam et al.

    Charting the effects of TMS with fMRI: Modulation of cortical recruitment within the distributed network supporting semantic control

    Neuropsychologia

    (2016)
  • M. Imafuku et al.

    “Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers

    Neuroimage

    (2014)
  • U. Kreplin et al.

    Effects of self-directed and other-directed introspection and emotional valence on activation of the rostral prefrontal cortex during aesthetic experience

    Neuropsychologia

    (2015)
  • E. Lesage et al.

    Cerebellar BOLD signal during the acquisition of a new lexicon predicts its early consolidation

    Brain and Language

    (2016)
  • W. Liu et al.

    Metaphorical meaning learning in contexts: An event-related potential study

    Journal of Neurolinguistics

    (2019)
  • R.J. Maddock

    The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: New insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (1999)
  • E.J. Maratos et al.

    Neural activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • A. Mestres-Missé et al.

    Neural differences in the mapping of verb and noun concepts onto novel words

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • N. Molinaro et al.

    Combinatorial semantics strengthens angular-anterior temporal coupling

    Cortex

    (2015)
  • R.J. Murray et al.

    Degrees of separation: A quantitative neuroimaging meta-analysis investigating self-specificity and shared neural activation between self- and other-reflection

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2012)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and neural efficiency

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2009)
  • G. Northoff et al.

    Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • R. Okada et al.

    Brain areas associated with sentence processing: A functional MRI study and a lesion study

    Journal of Neurolinguistics

    (2013)
  • M. Petrides

    Lateral frontal cortical contribution to memory

    Seminars in the Neurosciences

    (1996)
  • K.L. Phan et al.

    Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: A trial-related fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • G. Raboyeau et al.

    Brain activation and lexical learning: The impact of learning phase and word type

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • H. Sato et al.

    Temporal cortex activation during speech recognition: An optical topography study

    Cognition

    (1999)
  • A.P.R. Smith et al.

    fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • M. Stein et al.

    Reduced Frontal Activation with Increasing 2nd Language Proficiency

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • M. Sugiura et al.

    Passive and active recognition of one's own face

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • X. Sun et al.

    Age-dependent brain activation during forward and backward digit recall revealed by fMRI

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • P. Tacikowski et al.

    How multiple repetitions influence the processing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: An ERP study

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • D.J. Turk et al.

    Selfish learning: The impact of self-referential encoding on children's literacy attainment

    Learning and Instruction

    (2015)
  • G. Vigliocco et al.

    Nouns and verbs in the brain: A review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2011)
  • F.H. Wang et al.

    The role of reference in cross-situational word learning

    Cognition

    (2018)
  • L.A. Watson et al.

    Seeing yourself in a positive light: Brain correlates of the self-positivity bias

    Brain Research

    (2007)
  • E.L. Wilding

    Separating retrieval strategies from retrieval success: An event-related potential study of source memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • A.R. Aron et al.

    Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: One decade on

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2014)
  • J.S. Bedwell et al.

    Functional neuroanatomy of subcomponent cognitive processes involved in verbal working memory

    International Journal of Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • S. Bishop et al.

    Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: Controlling attention to threat-related stimuli

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2004)
  • R.D. Bleser et al.

    The organisation of the bilingual lexicon: A PET study

    Journal of Neurolinguistics

    (2003)
  • M.M. Botvinick

    Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function

    Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory & Cognition

    (1992)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • R. Chaffin et al.

    Learning new word meanings from context: A study of eye movements

    Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition

    (2001)
  • Cited by (6)

    • Cost or advantage? Intra-sentential language switching could facilitate L2 emotional words' comprehension in auditory modality

      2023, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The emotional valence and arousal of sentence contexts were evaluated in a scale ranging from 1 to 5 and the predictability of sentences were evaluated by additional participants through a sentence completion task. To control the confounding of self-reference effect (Li et al., 2021), sentences were subjected with I or We. All these sentences were translated into English, and then the target words of both Chinese and English sentences were converted into the other language, so there were 360 sentences in total.

    View full text