Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 August 2021
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding Socioemotional And Academic Adjustment During Childhood And Adolescence: Volume II View all 12 articles

Emotional Intelligence, Perceived Academic Self-Efficacy, and Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts as Predictors of Aesthetic-Musical Awareness in Late Adolescence

  • 1Department of General Didactics and Specific Didactics, University of Alicante, Alicante, Spain
  • 2Department of Didactics of Music, Plastic and Corporal Expression, University of Valladolid, Valladolid, Spain

Aesthetic-musical awareness demarcates a person's own perception of their ability to connect with music and the emotions it evokes. This may imply a benefit for the affective state of the individual. Therefore, the aim of this study was to observe whether there are statistically significant differences in emotional intelligence, perceived academic self-efficacy, and perfectionistic automatic thoughts when there are high and low scores in aesthetic-musical awareness in late adolescence. Likewise, we also aimed to determine whether emotional intelligence, perceived academic self-efficacy, and perfectionistic automatic thoughts are predictors of high aesthetic-musical awareness. To this end, a sample of 798 Spanish students between 17 and 23 years of age (Mage = 18.5 years) was used. Statistically significant differences were found for the dimensions of each variable when there were high and low scores in aesthetic-musical awareness (scores between d = −0.31 and −0.40), with higher mean scores for emotional intelligence, perceived academic self-efficacy, and perfectionistic automatic thoughts being present in the group with high aesthetic-musical awareness. Likewise, it was observed that the probability of presenting high scores in aesthetic-musical awareness was higher when there was an increase in emotional intelligence, perceived academic self-efficacy, and perceived automatic thoughts. In conclusion, the results found demonstrate that both emotional intelligence, perceived academic self-efficacy, and the presence of perfectionistic automatic thoughts influence on whether an individual has greater aesthetic-musical awareness. Taking into account previous studies that show how music influences the well-being of the person, these findings show a favorable link for the design of programs that benefit the emotional state of adolescents.

Introduction

Awareness, attention, and understanding of emotions are essential to accurately identify a person's emotional state (Chong-Leung and Cheung, 2020). This directly influences an individual's quality of life (Boden et al., 2015) and mental health (Lieberman et al., 2011), especially in the case of adolescence (Ciarrochi et al., 2011), which is characterized as a stage in which very intense emotions are experienced, but in which the skills to regulate them are not yet fully developed (Dingle et al., 2016).

Research that focuses on the emotions evoked by music is increasing due to the great adaptive power that music has for humans not only on an emotional level but also in terms of attachment and social and cultural belonging (Hunter and Schellenberg, 2010; Fritz et al., 2013; Koelsch, 2018, 2020; Ehrlich et al., 2019; Varner, 2019). Music is a medium that amplifies and alters emotions, especially for adolescents (Chong-Leung and Cheung, 2020). It influences emotion regulation (Stewart et al., 2019), while generating emotional well-being and increased awareness (Dingle et al., 2016).

According to Koelsch (2018), music can arouse different emotional, cognitive, and physiological components. These emotional components are interrelated with high Aesthetic-Musical Awareness (AMA), which is a dimension of Aesthetic Desire that is also known as one's self-awareness of the ability to connect with music and the emotions it evokes (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020a). In other words, the pleasurable affect generated by music is linked to deeper psychophysiological and cognitive aspects, ranging from emotion regulation to emotion arousal (Saarikallio et al., 2018). However, in this process, there is a conscious step involved which is the AMA.

In terms of its general conceptualization, Aesthetic Desire is understood as the emotional sensitivity that generates pleasant stimuli when observing or creating a work of art (Lundy et al., 2010; Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020a). This is a variable that has been recently studied and intervenes with the affective processes generated by plastic or architectural works of art, the beauty of one's surroundings, and musical works (Koelsch, 2018; Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020a).

Nowadays, the study of Aesthetic Desire is encouraged due to its relevance in aspects such as the frequency with which it is generated in an individual's day-to-day life and the psychological well-being it can promote for them (Saarikallio et al., 2018). The need to further understand this construct is also implied considering that a work of art, in this case pertaining to music, can be evaluated as something positive even though it triggers a negative feeling (e.g., melancholy) (Brattico et al., 2016; Koelsch, 2018). Taking this into account, it is relevant to inquire into people's AMA in order to observe whether this allows for the design of effective strategies for the regulation of an individual's emotions.

Being aware of one's feelings is greatly significant given the benefits that it can bring both at the interpersonal and intrapersonal levels (Karimi, 2021). Therefore, having high Emotional Intelligence (EI) is important because this is the ability to perceive, understand, and regulate one's emotions (Salovey and Mayer, 1990). As such, observing the link between EI and AMA can be of significance for people's daily lives, as well as for educational and therapeutic purposes.

Emotion is an affective experience that prepares humans to adapt to a particular environment, with its basic function being survival (Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019). Several studies claim that a change in emotional state can be clearly noted when listening to music, without this being conditional to whether the listener has had previous musical experience or even whether the individual has been governed by a particular personality trait (Mohn et al., 2011; Saarikallio et al., 2018; Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019). In fact, some functional neuroimaging studies claim that music acts on various brain structures that have direct involvement with emotions, such as the hippocampus, hypothalamus amygdala, and auditory cortex, among others (Koelsch, 2014, 2020; Koelsch and Skouras, 2014).

However, in the development of the teaching-learning process regarding emotional competence, and consequently EI, learning to be aware of one's own emotions is established as a fundamental step (Mayer et al., 2000; Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019). In this regard, authors such as Bisquerra (2017) note that listening to music is a way to allow this process to happen since at the same time that one discovers that an emotion has been evoked, they regulate it, which implies that it can be of great use for emotional education.

It seems as though EI is related to music listening (Londsdale, 2018). Many people listen to music to control their emotional state (Londsdale and North, 2011). To this end, these individuals must have a greater level of recognition and management of their own emotions. In other words, they need to have developed EI, with the use of music depending, in part, on the level of EI acquired (Londsdale, 2018).

However, for most people, music is a recreational and motivating activity, in which the emotions experienced while listening are not consciously analyzed (Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019). In fact, music can generate different emotions depending on the person listening to it (Kawakami et al., 2013), and even if the same emotion is felt, it can be experienced with different intensities (Schubert, 2013). For this reason, music does not only influence the reflection of the emotions felt but also can serve as a motivational strategy, a source of enjoyment (Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019), a tool to regulate emotions whatever the intensity (Stewart et al., 2019), and even as a verbal tool for the communication of such emotions (Kragness and Trainor, 2019).

Considering the role that music can have in triggering EI, and that EI is decisive for people's well-being socially (Nikooyeh et al., 2017), emotionally (Bucich and MacCann, 2019), and cognitively (Nirmala-Wijekoon et al., 2017), as well as it also being relevant at adolescence due to the characteristics of the stage and the search for one's own identity (Bucura, 2019), it is important to analyze the connection between EI and AMA in late adolescence.

On the other hand, this can be important for adolescence, being that it is a stage in which young people are immersed in their studies and are reflecting on their interests and the cognitive abilities that will improve their future career. In this sense, it is worth highlighting Academic Self-Efficacy (ASE), which is understood as one's belief about their own ability to achieve a goal, in this particular case, academic goals (García-Fernández et al., 2016b; Yokoyama, 2019) This may contribute to higher academic performance (García-Fernández et al., 2016a; Honicke and Broadbent, 2016; Yokoyama, 2019) and the adoption of metacognitive strategies (Asghar-Hayat and Shateri, 2019), as well as dispositional empathy (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020b), motivation (Ross et al., 2016), and academic resilience (Cassidy, 2015). It is therefore valuable to observe the relationship between ASE and AMA.

The essential external factors that affect individuals are aspects such as education, employment, motivation, and the need for a sense of achievement, among others (Hasanuddin and Sjahruddin, 2017). As such, it is important to obtain a high level of ASE that influences academic performance (Honicke and Broadbent, 2016; Yokoyama, 2019), as well as emotional and social aspects (Bucura, 2019; Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020c). In fact, it should be considered that a person's performance is not only observed in terms of their work capacity but also regarding their self-control, and even social relationships (Hasanuddin and Sjahruddin, 2017).

It has been shown that it is possible to regulate and control people's behavior in learning contexts (Agina et al., 2011). However, this depends on the pedagogical approach employed by educational professionals (Fernández-Rio et al., 2017). It has also been observed that the higher the self-regulated learning in students, the higher their ASE (Fernández-Rio et al., 2017). As such, there is a certain significance in nurturing students with strategies that promote the regulation of their own learning and behavior.

Adolescents tend to lead a rich and active musical life (Campbell et al., 2007). However, this does not imply that they possess a high level of efficacy in their music lessons. This is where ASE, among other components, influences the final outcome (Bucura, 2019).

Background music as a learning style, although it does not influence ASE, has a positive impact on the students' sense of achievement, bringing happiness, motivation, and greater enjoyment for a task, which has an impact on their academic success (Can and Güven, 2020) and results in less mental effort and time spent on the task (Lesiuk, 2005; Lim and Bang, 2018). Similarly, musical training influences emotional perception and mental imagery capacity, which contributes to academic learning (Commodari and Sole, 2019). Moreover, musical activity facilitates social bonding among students, promotes tolerance to diversity, and motivation for learning (Cores-Bilbao et al., 2019).

Considering the benefits of musical listening at the level of academic performance, and that musical pleasure comprises feelings of relaxation, joy, and power (Saarikallio et al., 2018), and without observing studies that analyze the direct link between AMA and ASE, it would be relevant to analyze the role of AMA on ASE in late adolescence.

Likewise, it should be considered that the demands of our current society are governed by perfect standards. In this regard, the adolescent stage may be conditioned to follow a pattern based on excellence at any level (e.g., physical, academic, social). As such, special attention should be paid to Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts (PATs), which are understood as automatic reflections that are based on the desire to be perfect (Flett et al., 1998, 2012; Esteve-Faubel et al., 2020). These thoughts have a negative influence at the academic (Desnoyers and Arpin-Cribbie, 2015; Flett et al., 2019), physical (Flett et al., 1998, 2012; Kirtley et al., 2015), social (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020c), and emotional level (Flett et al., 1998; Macedo et al., 2017; Aydin and Yerin-Güneri, 2020; Esteve-Faubel et al., 2020; Tyler et al., 2021). For this reason, it would be necessary to reflect on the connection that exists between AMA and PATs in order to determine whether AMA can positively influence strategies that enhance these perfectionistic reflections.

Perfectionism is known as a personality trait that negatively affects people who suffer from it given that it is determined by the establishment of very high and immoderate standards for the achievement of a proposed goal (Smith et al., 2021). Deriving from the more cognitive part, PATs are about specific ruminations of perfectionism (Flett et al., 2012).

A person with perfectionistic traits tends to demand an excessive quality of performance from themself in any task, therefore generating high levels of anxiety (Sarikaya and Kurtaslan, 2018). In this regard, some musicians have not been able to give any scheduled concerts throughout their professional life due to their high levels of anxiety (Kenny, 2011).

However, while music performances can generate high levels of stress and anxiety due to PATs (Flett et al., 2016; Aydin and Yerin-Güneri, 2020; Tyler et al., 2021), several studies claim that people with high levels of stress or emotionally negative experiences tend to listen more to music as a way of controlling their mood (Getz et al., 2012, 2014) because music listening has the power to decrease tension, anxiety, and stress (Lim and Bang, 2018). In other words, performing, and doing so on a crowded stage, is not the same as listening to music with the purpose of achieving a state of relaxation. Similarly, neither music performance nor listening, which have a positive or negative impact on the anxiety caused by perfectionism (Flett et al., 1998; Sarikaya and Kurtaslan, 2018; Aydin and Yerin-Güneri, 2020; Tyler et al., 2021), are the same as AMA.

There is little research based on perfectionism and music, and there is even less concrete research on the link between music and PATs. However, despite observing some research that supports this positive or negative link, depending on the case to be examined, there is a lack of research showing related findings between PATs and AMA.

Given the characteristics of AMA, which generate a specific internal and emotional knowledge of a person, it could have a beneficial influence on the design of strategies for the prevention of PATs and the regulation of other emotions of negative affect. Therefore, it is important to observe how AMA influences the PATs of the individual who presents them.

In the Current Study

The present study aims to: (a) analyze whether there are statistically significant differences in EI, ASE, and PATs in relation to high and low AMA scores in late adolescence; and (b) determine whether EI, ASE, and PATs are predictors of high AMA.

Taking into account the previous literature, it is expected that:

- Hypothesis 1: there are statistically significant differences in EI based on high and low AMA scores in late adolescence, and that EI is a predictor of high AMA (Mohn et al., 2011; Koelsch, 2014, 2020; Koelsch and Skouras, 2014; Saarikallio et al., 2018; Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019).

- Hypothesis 2: there are statistically significant differences in ASE based on high and low AMA scores in late adolescence, and that ASE is a predictor of high AMA (Lesiuk, 2005; Lim and Bang, 2018; Can and Güven, 2020).

- Hypothesis 3: there are statistically significant differences in PATs based on high and low AMA scores in late adolescence, and that PATs are predictors of high AMA (Getz et al., 2012, 2014; Lim and Bang, 2018).

Method

Participants

The participant sample was recruited under the accessibility criteria in the Faculty of Education at the University of Alicante.

A total of 824 students were selected. These were students who were in either their first or second academic year, and were studying degrees in either Early Childhood Education or Primary Education. The inclusion criteria were that the participants were future teachers so that the results are known to all of them and can apply interesting aspects in their future classrooms.

Twenty-two (2.67%) participants were excluded due to errors or omissions in the completion of the questionnaires, these being the only reasons for exclusion. Therefore, the final sample of 798 participants was achieved, with ages ranging between 17 and 23 years (Mage = 18.5; SD = 3.2), and with 70.3% being women.

Regarding the ethnicity of the participants, it should be noted that the vast majority of students were Spanish (92.23%). However, 3.88% were European, 2.01% Arab, 1.00% South American and 0.88% Asian. However, all of them had been living in Spain for more than 7 years.

Instruments

Trait Meta-Mood Scale 24 (TMMS-24; Fernández-Berrocal et al., 2004): The TMMS-24 is a Likert-type scale with 5 response points (1 = Not agree at all; 5 = Strongly agree) that assesses EI from 24 items and three dimensions: I. Attention to feelings (e.g.: I pay close attention to feelings); II. Mood repair (e.g.: I can come to understand my feelings); and III. Clarity of feelings (e.g.: When I'm sad I think of all the pleasures in life).

Reliability levels of the scale were acceptable for all dimensions (α = 0.89, 0.90, and 0.85, respectively).

Academic Situations Specific Perceived Self-Efficacy Scale (SPSASS; García-Fernández et al., 2010). The Spanish version of the SPSASS, adapted from the original scale by Palenzuela (1983), is a Likert-type scale with four response options (1 = Never; 4 = Always), consisting of 10 items and a unidimensional structure. This scale measures self-efficacy expectations in educational situations for both high school and university students (e.g.: I think I can get through a semester quite easily, and even with very good grades).

The internal consistency of the instrument for this study was α = 0.88.

Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory (PCI; Esteve-Faubel et al., 2020): The Spanish version of the PCI is an adaptation of the work done by Flett et al. (1998), which assesses PATs. The PCI is a Likert-type scale with 5 response options (1 = Not at all; 5 = All the time) and is composed of 17 items that make up three dimensions and a second-order factor. These dimensions measure: I. Perfectionistic Concerns (e.g.: I certainly have high standards); II. Perfectionistic Demands (e.g.: I've got to keep working on my goals); and III. Perfectionistic Strivings (e.g.: I expect to be perfect).

Reliability levels were acceptable for all 3 dimensions in the present study (α = 0.83, 0.71, 0.86, respectively).

Desire For Aesthetic Scale (DFAS; Aparicio-Flores et al., 2020a): The Spanish version of the DFAS is an adaptation of the original version designed by Lundy et al. (2010), which assesses Aesthetic Desire. The DFAS is a Likert-type scale with seven response options (1= Strongly Disagree; 7 = Strongly Agree) and 11 items configured in three dimensions: I. Satisfaction with Aesthetic Beauty (SAB) (e.g.: In the past, when I've mod into a new apartment, office or dorm, one of the first things I do is decorate the walls with nice artwork); II. Aesthetic-Musical Awareness (AMA) (e.g.: Hearing gorgeous songs is a major motivator for me in daily life); III. Emotional State of Aesthetic Beauty (ESAB) (e.g.: I would not to be happy in an unattractive house).

Specifically, in this study, the AMA dimension was administered, which obtained adequate reliability indices (α = 0.72).

Procedure

Firstly, a meeting was held with the teachers of the subjects in which the study questionnaires were to be administered, in order to inform them of the objectives and request their collaboration. Later, in another session, the participants were informed of the purpose of the study, they were asked for their informed consent, and they were told about their status as volunteers and about their anonymity.

Subsequently, the different tests were administered in the classroom within an estimated time of 45 min, during which a researcher was present.

The research was carried out under the recommendations of the ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki of 1964.

Data Analysis

Firstly, a Student's T-test was applied to study the differences in EI, ASE, and PATs according to the high and low AMA scores. To evaluate the magnitude of the differences found, Cohen's (Cohen, 1988) d index was included.

Secondly, to determine the predictive probability of EI, ASE, and PATs on high AMA, the statistical technique of logistic regression was applied, following the forward stepwise procedure based on the Wald statistic. The probability was estimated through the odds ratio (OR) in accordance with the following interpretation: if OR = > 1, for example, OR = 2, for each occurrence of the situation in the presence of the independent variable, there will be two times the variable present. If, on the other hand, OR = <1, for example, OR = 0.5, the probability of that situation occurring in the absence of the independent variable will be 0.5 times lower than in its presence.

For the analysis of the quality and fit of the proposed models, the following indicators were considered: Nagelkerke's R2, which indicates the percentage of variance explained by the model (Nagelkerke, 1991); and the diagnostic efficiency, that is, the percentage of cases correctly classified by the model.

It should be noted that AMA was dichotomized according to low scores (percentile = > 25) and high scores (percentile = <75).

Results

Differences in Emotional Intelligence, Academic Self-Efficacy, and Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts in Late Adolescence With High and Low Scores in Aesthetic-Musical Awareness

Table 1 shows the statistically significant differences obtained in EI, ASE, and PATs for late adolescence in accordance with high and low AMA scores. The observed differences were statistically significant, and were also of small magnitude, for all EI, PATs, and ASE factors. Thus, the results show that all three EI factors, attention to feelings (M = 28.20; SD = 6.55), mood repair (M = 27.04; SD = 6.72) and clarity of feelings (M = 26.49; SD = 6.39), score higher in the high AMA scores compared with the low AMA group.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Differences in emotional intelligence, academic self-efficacy, and perfectionistic automatic thoughts in late adolescence according to high and low scores in aesthetic-musical awareness.

Likewise, higher ASE scores are obtained for the group with high scores as opposed to the group with low AMA scores (M = 32.25; SD = 6.63).

On the other hand, the high AMA profile vs. the low AMA profile obtained higher scores in perfectionistic concerns (M = 15.71; SD = 5.46), perfectionistic demands (M = 14.68; SD = 2.60), and perfectionistic strivings (M = 23.83; SD = 5.31).

Predictive Capacity of the Variables Emotional Intelligence, Academic Self-Efficacy, and Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts on Aesthetic-Musical Awareness

As can be seen in Table 2, two logistic regression models have been created from which correct estimates can be made regarding the probability of showing high AMA considering EI, ASE, and PATs scores.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Binary logistic regression for the probability of presenting high scores in aesthetic musical awareness based on emotional intelligence, academic self-efficacy, and perfectionistic automatic thoughts.

The findings allow us to make a correct estimate for attention to feelings in 58.7% of cases (χ2 = 11.24; p = 0.001). These data allow us to affirm that this dimension of EI is a predictor of high AMA. The components of the model revealed by the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of presenting high levels of AMA is greater, specifically 1.05 for each point increase in the attention to feelings factor of EI. Likewise, with respect to mood repair, the data allow us to make a correct estimate for 61.2% of cases (χ2 =11.82; p = 0.001), indicating that this EI factor is also a predictor of AMA. The components of the model showing the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of having high levels of AMA is 1.06 greater for each point that mood repair increases. Regarding clarity of feelings, the results found allow us to make a correct estimate in 61% of cases (χ2 = 9.87; p = 0.002), suggesting that this dimension of the TMMS-24 is a predictor of a high level of AMA. The components of the model showing the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of presenting a high AMA is 1.05 higher for each point increase in clarity of feelings.

On the other hand, with respect to the variable of ASE, the data allow for a correct estimation in 58.7% of cases (χ2 =13.70; p = < 0.001), indicating that ASE is a predictor of high levels of AMA. In this case, the components of the model that show the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of showing a high level of AMA is 1.06 higher for each point that ASE increases.

Finally, regarding PATs, the data concerning perfectionistic concerns offer a correct estimate in 60.4% of cases (χ2 =14.37; p = < 0.001), suggesting that this dimension of PATs is predictive of high AMA. The components of the model showing the odds ratio (OR) highlight that the probability of presenting high AMA is 1.07 higher for each point that perfectionistic concerns increase. Similarly, with respect to perfectionistic demands, the results obtained allow for a correct estimation in 60.6% of the cases (χ2 =16.48; p = < 0.001), indicating that this factor of the PATs is a predictor of high levels of AMA. The components of the model showing the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of presenting high levels of AMA is 1.15 higher for each point increase in perfectionistic demands. Likewise, in relation to perfectionistic strivings, a correct estimate can be made for 59.6% of cases (χ2 =10.67; p = 0.001), suggesting that this factor of PATs is a predictor of high AMA. The components of the model showing the odds ratio (OR) indicate that the probability of having a high AMA is 1.06 higher for each point that perfectionistic strivings increase.

Discussion

The findings of this study indicate statistically significant differences for all EI, ASE, and PATs factors when considering high and low AMA scores, showing higher scores in the group with high AMA in all cases. Similarly, it is observed that EI, ASE, and PATs are predictors of high AMA.

Regarding EI, the results show that the three EI factors, attention to feelings, mood repair, and clarity of feelings score higher in the group with a high AMA level compared to the profile with a low level of AMA. Likewise, all three dimensions of EI are predictors of high AMA. Therefore, all the hypotheses of the study are fulfilled.

Possessing high EI demonstrates a better perception and understanding of the emotional state the person is going through at a given moment (Salovey and Mayer, 1990; Mayer et al., 2000; Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019). Therefore, this ability to perceive emotions can also be determined by contexts in which music is the key to the altering of emotions. In other words, this means having the ability to perceive, understand, and regulate one's own emotions (these being the three factors of EI) whether from music or from any other context.

It is worth remembering that music has the ability to alter and amplify emotions (Chong-Leung and Cheung, 2020), while at the same time regulating them (Bisquerra, 2017; Londsdale, 2018; Saarikallio et al., 2018; Stewart et al., 2019). This leads to emotional well-being and greater awareness (Dingle et al., 2016), which implies achieving a greater AMA. Therefore, there is the possibility to develop EI from musical strategies.

Regarding ASE, it is the group with high AMA that scores higher compared to the group with low AMA. Similarly, the results indicate that the probability of showing a high level of AMA increases as ASE increases.

According to previous studies, musical listening has a significant effect on positive affect, mental effort, and academic performance; however, it does not have a significant effect on perceived self-efficacy (Lim and Bang, 2018). In other words, a positive mood is able to be generated, which has an impact on the quality of a task and requires less mental effort and time spent on the task, without having a belief about higher ability (Lesiuk, 2005; Lim and Bang, 2018). However, other studies caution that, more concretely, ASE is linked to higher academic performance due to an individual having a belief about their ability to achieve an academic goal (Honicke and Broadbent, 2016; Yokoyama, 2019). There is no previous research examining the link between AMA and ASE, however, this study shows that feeling that one is capable of tackling an academic task has an impact on being more aware of one's own emotions generated by music, or at least thinking that one has a greater awareness of it. ASE is positively and significantly related to basic psychological needs, being based on competence, autonomy, and affinity needs, which contributes to optimal emotional development (Zhen et al., 2017), that is, perception, understanding, and clarity of feelings. Therefore, while there are studies that affirm the positive link established between EI and academic performance (Nirmala-Wijekoon et al., 2017) and even with quality of work-life, satisfaction, and work engagement (Karimi and Karimi, 2016), from this study, it can be stated that having the belief of greater academic ability allows one to be more aware of one's emotions, in this case musical ones. In other words, there is a greater chance of presenting more AMA.

Finally, in relation to the PATs, the group with a high level of AMA, vs. the group with low levels, scored higher in perfectionistic concerns, perfectionistic demands, and perfectionistic strivings. Furthermore, it is observed that the three PAT factors are also predictors of high AMA. In other words, the probability of high AMA increases as perfectionistic concerns, perfectionistic demands, and perfectionistic strivings increase.

In a recent study of PAT profiles of varying intensity, it was observed that the high PAT group scored higher in attention to feelings compared to the moderate and low PAT profiles. Moreover, understanding and clarity of feelings scored higher in the moderate PAT group followed by the high PAT profile (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2021). This may be due to the fact that the concern and demand to be perfect triggers a high degree of meticulousness with the purpose of achieving one's goals (Stoeber et al., 2009). This also causes a high level of alertness to one's own internal emotions. In other words, this leads to possessing greater attention to feelings, which generates greater understanding and emotion repair skills (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2021).

Despite there not being previous studies that determine a link between AMA and PATs, this study indicates that PATs, that is, perfectionistic concerns, perfectionistic demands, and perfectionistic strivings, all predict AMA. These findings may be due equally to the degree of meticulousness exhibited by individuals with PATs (Besharat and Kamali, 2016) that makes them alert to both their external and internal stimuli. On the one hand, perfectionistic concerns are worries about making mistakes. On the other hand, demands are based on the requirement for self-improvement to not only perform better but also to avoid possible negative situations. Likewise, efforts are defined by thoughts that are based on striving toward perfection (Stoeber et al., 2014; Esteve-Faubel et al., 2020). In other words, the three PAT factors, despite having their specific peculiarities, are governed by a continuous rumination toward reaching perfection or, on the contrary, toward not committing any imperfection. Besharat and Kamali (2016) stated that both PATs and ego control predicted meticulousness, which shows us that those with the presence of PATs need absolute control of their actions considering given the great psychological distress that they experience in the event of acting imperfectly or not performing in an impeccable way (Flett et al., 1998; Lyubomirsky et al., 2015). Hence, they are governed by meticulousness in all their internal and external actions, triggering greater EI (Aparicio-Flores et al., 2021), and consequently greater AMA.

Limitations and Future Research

Several limitations should be noted. Firstly, despite having a large sample of participants, the results are not generalizable to the entire adolescent population. Likewise, due to the high sample of Spanish participants, they may find different cultural connotations. However, according to Argstatter (2015), it should be that at the level of emotional classification by musical stimuli, culture has no influence. Considering that our study is driven by AMA and not by attention to feelings after musical listening, it is likely that culture does not have a significant influence. However, in order to be able to affirm this hypothesis, it would be of great benefit to carry out a similar study with a population of similar ages that belong to different cultures in order to examine whether differences exist according to culture.

Moreover, this study shows differences in EI, ASE, and PATs according to high and low AMA, as well as showing that EI, ASE, and PATs are predictor variables of high AMA in late adolescence. However, there are no studies examining whether these results can be generalized to child and adult populations. Therefore, future studies should replicate the analyses performed with different age samples.

Furthermore, this work may be limited due to the use of self-report scales. This could be solved by employing an evaluation that uses multiple sources and methods.

Practical Implications and Conclusions

Despite the aforementioned limitations, this is a novel study that contributes to the investigation of emotional, artistic, and academic aspects in the adolescent stage for several reasons. First, it is the first study to examine the differences in EI, ASE, and PATs according to high and low AMA, as well as examine whether these three variables predict high AMA. Secondly, the observed results provide specific insights into the emotional aspects of adolescents that may have a positive impact on their psychological well-being and academic performance. In other words, this direct link between these variables may be suggesting, as McGinnis (2017) stated, that teachers should orient their pedagogical perspective toward musical experiences that help the development of students' emotional self-regulation.

It is worth remembering that adolescence is framed as a stage in which very intense emotions are experienced, but also is one in which the skills to regulate them are not yet fully developed (Dingle et al., 2016). Music can have a positive influence in this, especially at this stage (Ciarrochi et al., 2011).

Music enhances both positive and negative emotions (Chong-Leung and Cheung, 2020), and being aware of the emotions it conveys can bring both internal and external benefits (Karimi, 2021). As such, it is important to include musical training in people's daily lives to consciously influence their psychological improvement and well-being (Lieberman et al., 2011; Chong-Leung and Cheung, 2020), which in turn will influence their academic performance (Lim and Bang, 2018) and quality of life (Boden et al., 2015).

For music to have special relevance in emotional education, further teacher training is urged both in the identification and regulation of emotions through musical listening, as well as in the management of specific music that allows working in the classroom depending on the situation and the feeling to be increased and/or analyzed (Lorenzo-de Reizábal, 2019).

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RE-F and MA-F: article conceptualization, investigation, writing original draft, and review and editing. VC-N: formal analysis, data curation, and supervision. JE-F: methodology, formal analysis, resources, data curation, supervision and project administration. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Agina, A. M., Kommers, P. A., and Steehouter, F. (2011). The effect of the external regulator's absence on children's speech use, manifested self-regulation, and task performance during learning tasks. Comput. Hum. Behav. 27, 1118–1128. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio-Flores, M. P., Esteve-Faubel, J. M., Esteve-Faubel, R. P., and Álvarez-Teruel, J. D. (2020b). High academic self-efficacy and dispositional empathy in future teachers. Sustainability 12, 6728–6744. doi: 10.3390/su12176728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio-Flores, M. P., Esteve-Faubel, J. M., Vicent, M., Gonzálvez, C., Sanmartín, R., and García-Fernández, J. M. (2021). Dispositional empathy and emotional intelligence in terms of perfectionistic automatic thoughts. Span. J. Psychol. 24, 1–10. doi: 10.1017/SJP.2020.57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio-Flores, M. P., Vicent, M., Sanmartín, R., Gonzálvez, C., Freire-Andino, R. O., and García-Fernández, J. M. (2020c). Psychometric properties of the perfectionism cognitions inventory in Ecuador. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–12. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165834

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aparicio-Flores, M. P., Vicent, M., Sanmartín, R., Pérez-García, M. A., Álvarez-Teruel, J. D., and García-Fernández, J. M. (2020a). Propiedades psicométricas de la Desire For Aesthetics Scale (VR) en población adulta española [Psychometric properties of the Desire For Aesthetics Scale (RV) in the spanish adult population]. Revista Espacios 41, 23–32.

Google Scholar

Argstatter, H. (2015). Perception of basic emotions in music: culture-specific or multicultural? Psychol. Music 44, 674–690. doi: 10.1177/0305735615589214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asghar-Hayat, A., and Shateri, K. (2019). The role of academic self-efficacy in improving student's metacognitive learning strategies. J. Adv. Med. Edu. Prof. 7, 205–212. doi: 10.30476/jamp.2019.81200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aydin, G., and Yerin-Güneri, O. (2020). Exploring the role of psychological inflexibility, rumination, perfectionism cognitions, cognitive defusion, and self-forgiveness in cognitive test anxiety. Curr. Psychol. 1–10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00805-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besharat, M. A., and Kamali, Z. S. (2016). Predicting obsessions and compulsions according to superego and ego characteristics: a comparison between scrupulosity and non-religious obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Asian J. Psychiatry 19, 73–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2016.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisquerra, R. (2017). Música y educación emocional. Eufonía Didáctica de la música 71, 43–48.

Google Scholar

Boden, M. T., Irons, J. G., Feldner, M. T., Bujarski, S., and Bonn-Miller, M. O. (2015). An investigation of relations among quality of life and individual facets of emotional awareness and mindfulness. Mindfulness 6, 700–707. doi: 10.1007/s12671-014-0308-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brattico, E., Bogert, B., Alluri, V., Tervaniemi, M., Eerola, T., and Jacobsen, T. (2016). It's sad but i like it: the neural dissociation between musical emotions and liking in experts and laypersons. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 2–21. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucich, M., and MacCann, C. (2019). Emotional intelligence and day-to-day emotion regulation processes: examining motives for social sharing. Pers. Individ. Dif. 137, 22–26. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bucura, E. (2019). Fostering self-efficacy among adolescents in secondary general music. Gen. Music Today 32, 5–12. doi: 10.1177/1048371319834080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, P. S., Connell, C., and Beegle, A. (2007). Adolescents' expressed meanings of music in and out of school. J. Res. Music Edu. 55, 220–236. doi: 10.1177/002242940705500304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Can, Y., and Güven, M. (2020). The effects of music on achievement emotions and self-efficacy beliefs of students who have different learning styles. Sci. Edu. Art. Technol. J. 4, 95–113.

Google Scholar

Cassidy, S. (2015). Resilience building in students: the role of academic self-efficacy. Front. Psychol. 6, 1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chong-Leung, M., and Cheung, R. Y. M. (2020). Music engagement and well-being in Chinese adolescents: emotional awareness, positive emotions, and negative emotions as mediating processes. Psychol. Music 48, 105–119. doi: 10.1177/0305735618786421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciarrochi, J., Kashdan, T. B., Leeson, P., Heaven, P., and Jordan, C. (2011). On being aware and accepting: a one-year longitudinal study into adolescent well-being. J. Adolesc. 34, 695–703. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Erlbaum.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Commodari, E., and Sole, J. (2019). Music education in junior high school: perception of emotions conveyed by music and mental imagery in students who attend the standard or musical curriculum. Psychol. Music 48, 824–835. doi: 10.1177/0305735619832962

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cores-Bilbao, E., Fernández-Corbacho, A., Machancoses, F. H., and Fonseca-Mora, M. C. (2019). A music-mediated language learning experience: student's awareness of their socio-emotional skills. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desnoyers, A., and Arpin-Cribbie, C. A. (2015). Examining cognitive performance: do perfectionism and rumination matter? Pers. Indiv. Dif. 76, 94–98. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dingle, G. A., Hodges, J., and Kunde, A. (2016). Tuned in emotion regulation program using music listening: effectiveness for adolescents in educational settings. Front. Psychol. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrlich, S., Agres, K. R., Guan, C., and Cheng, G. (2019). A closed-loop, music-based brain-computer interface for emotion mediation. PLoS ONE 14, 1–24. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteve-Faubel, J. M., Aparicio-Flores, M. P., Vicent, M., Gonzálvez, C., Sanmartín, R., and García-Fernández, J. M. (2020). Validation of Spanish version of the perfectionism cognitions Inventory: profiles of automatic perfectionism thoughts and their association with social anxiety. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 51, 268–277. doi: 10.1037/pro0000290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Berrocal, P., Extremera, N., and Ramos, N. (2004). Validity and reliability of the Spanish modified version of the trait meta-mood scale. Psychol. Rep. 94, 751–755. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3.751-755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández-Rio, J., Cecchini, J. A., Méndez-Giménez, A., Méndez-Alonso, D., and Prieto, J. A. (2017). Self-regulation, cooperative learning, and academic self-efficacy: interactions to prevent school failure. Front. Psychol. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Blankstein, K. R., and Gray, L. (1998). Psychological distress and the frequency of perfectionistic thinking. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 75, 1363–1381. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Molnar, D. S., Nepon, T., and Hewitt, P. L. (2012). A mediational model of perfectionistic automatic thoughts and psychosomatic symptoms: the roles of negative affect and daily hassles. Pers. Individ. Dif. 52, 565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., Hewitt, P. L., and Fitzgerald, K. (2016). Perfectionism, components of stress reactivity, and depressive symptoms. J. Psychopatol. Behav. Assess. 38, 645–654. doi: 10.1007/s10862-016-9554-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., Hewitt, P. L., Zaki-Azat, J., Rose, A. L., and Swiderski, K. (2019). The mistake rumination scale: development, validation and utility of a measure of cognitive perfectionism. J. Psychoedu. Assess. 38, 84–98. doi: 10.1177/0734282919879538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, T. H., Schumde, P., Jentschke, S., Friederici, A. D., and Koelsch, S. (2013). From understanding to appreciating music cross-culturally. PLoS ONE 8, 1–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Fernández, J. M., Inglés, C. J., Torregrosa, M. S., Ruiz-Esteban, C., Díaz-Herrero, A., Pérez Fernández, E., et al. (2010). Propiedades psicométricas de la Escala de Autoeficacia Percibida Específica de Situaciones académicas en una muestra de estudiantes españoles de Educación Secundaria Obligatoria. Eur. J. Edu. Psychol. 3, 61–74. doi: 10.30552/ejep.v3i1.51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Fernández, J. M., Inglés, C. J., Vicent, M., Gonzálvez, C., Pérez-Sánchez, A. M., and Lagos-San Martín, N. (2016b). Validación de la escala de Autoeficacia Percibida Específica de Situaciones Académicas en Chile y su relación con las estrategias de aprendizaje [Validation of the Academic Situations Specific Perceived Self-efficacy Scale in Chile and its relation with learning strategies]. Revista Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación- e Avaliação Psicológica 41, 118–131.

Google Scholar

García-Fernández, J. M., Inglés-Saura, C. J., Vicent, M., Gonzálvez, C., Lagos-San Martín, N., and Pérez-Sánchez, A. M. (2016a). Relación entre autoeficacia y autoatribuciones académicas en estudiantes chilenos [Relationship between self-efficacy and academic self-attributions in Chilean students]. Universitas Psychologica 15, 15–24. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy15-1.raaa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getz, L. M., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Roy, M. M., and Devroop, K. (2012). The relationship between affect, uses of music, and music preferences in a sample of South African adolescents. Psychol. Music 40, 164–178. doi: 10.1177/0305735610381818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Getz, L. M., Marks, S., and Roy, M. (2014). The influence of stress, optimism, and music training on music uses and preferences. Psychol. Music 42, 71–85. doi: 10.1177/0305735612456727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasanuddin, R., and Sjahruddin, H. (2017). The structure of emotional intelligence, spiritual intelligence and its relationship with work enthusiasm and auditor performance. World J. Bus. Manage. 3, 67–85. doi: 10.5296/wjbm.v3i1.11321

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honicke, T., and Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on academic performance: a systematic review. Edu. Res. Rev. 17, 63–84. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2015.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, P. G., and Schellenberg, E. G. (2010). “Music and emotion,” in Music Perception, eds En M.R. Jones, R.Fay, y A.Pooper (New York, NY: Springer handbook of auditory research), 129–163. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-6114-3_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi, M. (2021). A review of emotions makes up the bulk of life. Int. J. Adv. Stud. Hum. Soc. Sci. 10, 156–160. doi: 10.22034/IJASHSS.2021.278791.1050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karimi, Z., and Karimi, F. (2016). The structural models of relationship between spiritual intelligence and emotional intelligence with quality of work life and work engagement of employees. Int. J. Manage. Edu. 10, 278–292. doi: 10.1504/IJMIE.2016.077508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawakami, A., Furukawa, K., Katahira, K., and Okanoya, K. (2013). Sad music induces pleasant emotion. Front. Psychol. 4, 1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kenny, D. T. (2011). The Psychology of Music Performance Anxiety. Oxford: Oxford University Presss. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586141.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirtley, O. J., O'Connor, R. C., and O'Carroll, R. E. (2015). Hurting inside and out? emotional and physical pain in self-harm ideation and enactment. Int. J. Cogn. Ther. 8, 156–171. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2015.8.2.156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2014). Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 170–180. doi: 10.1038/nrn3666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2018). Investigating the neural encoding of emotion with music. Neuron 98, 1075–1079. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S. (2020). A coordinate-based meta-analysis of music-evoked emotions. NeuroImage 223:117350. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., and Skouras, S. (2014). Functional centrality of amygdala, striatum and hypothalamus in a “small-world” network underlying joy: an fMRI study with music. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3485–3498. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kragness, H. E., and Trainor, L. J. (2019). Nonmusicians express emotions in musical productions using conventional cues. Music Sci. 2, 1–13. doi: 10.1177/2059204319834943

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesiuk, T. (2005). The effect of music on work performance. Psychol. Music 33, 173–191. doi: 10.1177/0305735605050650

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, M. D., Inagaki, T. K., Tabibnia, G., and Crockett, M. J. (2011). Subjective responses to emotional stimuli during labeling, reappraisal, and distraction. Emotion 11, 468–480. doi: 10.1037/a0023503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lim, H. A., and Bang, E. (2018). The effects of music listening on affect, self-efficacy, mental exertion, and task performance of online learners. J. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Christians High. Edu. 8, 13–37. doi: 10.31380/sotlched.8.1.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Londsdale, A. J. (2018). Emotional intelligence, alexithymia, stress, and people's reasons for listening to music. Psychol. Music 47, 680–693. doi: 10.1177/0305735618778126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Londsdale, A. J., and North, A. C. (2011). Why do we listen to music? a uses and gratifications analysis. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 108–134. doi: 10.1348/000712610X506831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-de Reizábal, A. (2019). Música para el desarrollo de la conciencia emocional [Music for the development of emotional awareness]. Publicaciones 49, 191–213. doi: 10.30827/publicaciones.v49i2.8384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundy, D. E., Schenkel, M. B., Akrie, T. N., and Walker, A. M. (2010). How important is beauty to you? the development of the desire for aesthetics scale. Empir. Stud. Arts 28, 73–92. doi: 10.2190/EM.28.1.e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyubomirsky, S., Layous, K., Chancellor, J., and Nelson, K. S. (2015). Thinking about rumination: the scholarly contributions and intellectual legacy of Susan Nolen-Hoeksema. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 11, 1–22. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macedo, A., Marques, C., Quaresma, V., Soares, M. J., Amaral, A. P., Araújo, A. I., et al. (2017). Are perfectionism cognitions and cognitive emotion regulation strategies mediators between perfectionism and psychological distress? Pers. Individ. Dif. 119, 46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., and Caruso, D. R. (2000). “Models of emotional intelligence,” in Handbook of Intelligence, 2nd Edn, ed En R.J. Sternberg (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 396–420. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511807947.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGinnis, E. J. (2017). Developing the emotional intelligence of undergraduate music education majors: an exploratory study using bradberry and greave's (2009) emotional intelligence 2.0. J. Music Teach. Edu. 27, 1–12. doi: 10.1177/1057083717723919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohn, C., Argstatter, H., and Wilker, F. W. (2011). Perception of six basic emotions in music. Psychol. Music 39, 503–517. doi: 10.1177/0305735610378183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagelkerke, N. J. D. (1991). A note on a general definition of the coefficient of determination. Biometrika 78, 691–692. doi: 10.1093/biomet/78.3.691

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nikooyeh, E., Zarani, F., and Fathabadi, J. (2017). The mediating role of social skills and sensation seeking in the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and school adjustment in adolescents. J. Adolesc. 59, 45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nirmala-Wijekoon, C., Amaratunge, H., De Silva, Y., Senanayake, S., Jayawardane, P., and Senarath, U. (2017). Emotional intelligence and academic performance of medical undergraduates: a cross-sectional study in a selected university in Sri Lanka. BMC Med. Edu. 17, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/s12909-017-1018-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palenzuela, D. (1983). Construcción y validación de una escala de autoeficacia percibida específica de situaciones académicas. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta 9, 185–219. doi: 10.33776/amc.v9i21.1649

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, M., Perkins, H., and Bodey, K. (2016). Academic motivation and information literacy self-efficacy: the importance of a simple desire to know. Libr. Infor. Sci. Res. 38, 2–9. doi: 10.1016/j.lisr.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saarikallio, S. H., Maksimainen, J. P., and Randall, W. M. (2018). Relaxed and connected: insights into the emotional-motivational constituents of musical pleasure. Psychol. Music 47, 644–662. doi: 10.1177/0305735618778768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salovey, P., and Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imag. Cogn. Personal. 9, 185–211. doi: 10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarikaya, M., and Kurtaslan, Z. (2018). Prediction of musical performance anxiety according to music teacher candidates' perfectionism and self-efficacy beliefs. Int. Online J. Edu. Sci. 10, 183–198. doi: 10.15345/iojes.2018.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schubert, E. (2013). Emotion felt by the listener and expressed by the music: literature review and theoretical perspectives. Front. Psychol. 4, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00837

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. M., Sherry, S. B., Ge, S. Y., Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., and Lee-Baggley, D. (2021). Multidimensional perfectionism turns 30: a review of know knowns and known unknowns. Can. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/cap0000288. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, J., Garrido, S., Hense, C., and McFerran, K. (2019). Music use for mood regulation: self-awareness and conscious listening choices in young people with tendencias to depression. Front. Psychol. 10, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., Kobori, O., and Brown, A. (2014). Examining mutual supression effects in the assessment of perfectionism cognitions: evidence supporting multidimensional assessment. Assessment 21, 647–660. doi: 10.1177/1073191114534884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeber, J., Otto, K., and Dalbert, C. (2009). Perfectionism and the Big Five: conscientiousness predicts longitudinal increases in self-oriented perfectionism. Pers. Individ. Dif. 47, 363–368. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, J., Mu, W., McCann, J., Belli, F., and Asnaani, A. (2021). The unique contribution of perfectionistic cognitions to anxiety disorder symptoms in a treatment-seeking sample. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 50, 121–137. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2020.1798497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varner, E. (2019). General music learning is also social and emotional learning. Gen. Music Today 33, 74–78. doi: 10.1177/1048371319891421

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yokoyama, S. (2019). Academic self-efficacy and academic performance in online learning: a mini review. Front. Psychol. 9, 1–4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhen, R., Liu, R. D., Ding, Y., Wang, J., Liu, Y., and Xu, L. (2017). The mediating roles of academic self-efficacy and academic emotions in the relation between basic psychological needs satisfaction and learning engagement among Chinese adolescent students. Learn. Indiv. Dif. 54, 210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aesthetic-musical awareness, aesthetic desire, emotional intelligence, academic self-efficacy, perfectionistic automatic thoughts

Citation: Esteve-Faubel RP, Aparicio-Flores MP, Cavia-Naya V and Esteve-Faubel JM (2021) Emotional Intelligence, Perceived Academic Self-Efficacy, and Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts as Predictors of Aesthetic-Musical Awareness in Late Adolescence. Front. Psychol. 12:733025. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.733025

Received: 29 June 2021; Accepted: 14 July 2021;
Published: 12 August 2021.

Edited by:

Nelly Lagos San Martín, University of the Bío Bío, Chile

Reviewed by:

Inmaculada Méndez, University of Murcia, Spain
Cruz López De Rego Fernández, Autonomous University of Madrid, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Esteve-Faubel, Aparicio-Flores, Cavia-Naya and Esteve-Faubel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: María Pilar Aparicio-Flores, pilar.aparicio@ua.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.