Blinded by guilt: Short-term relational focus and lying

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104191Get rights and content

Abstract

Guilt has often been conceptualized as a moral emotion because it stimulates transgressors to restore a damaged relationship. However, not much has been investigated about when the guilt-induced relational-promoting goal conflicts with certain moral norms (for instance, honesty). In this paper, we demonstrate that guilt increases the transgressors' lying intentions in situations where honesty is anticipated to incur immediate relational harm with the victim and that this effect is driven by the transgressors' short-term relational focus. Across three experiments (two preregistered), we provide robust evidence for our prediction. Study 1 examined the effect of guilt on inflating the feedback to the victim, measured relational focus and the focus on the instrumental value of honesty (i.e., long-term benefits brought by honesty such as career growth) as simultaneous mechanisms, and contrasted the effect of guilt on the lying intention with that of shame. Study 2 better measured the short-termism in the relational focus and examined a situation wherein guilty participants could lie to a third party for the benefit of the victim. Study 3 provided additional support for our proposed mechanism by showing that deliberation on the consequences of not being honest (i.e., indirectly increasing participants' focus on the long-term instrumental value of honesty) decreases people's lying intentions. These results contribute to our understanding of the complex interpersonal nature of guilt and, specifically, its temporal focus.

Introduction

Elicited by interpersonal harm, guilt stimulates transgressors towards relationship promoting behaviors such as cooperation, compensation, and reparation (e.g., Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1994; Cryder, Springer, & Morewedge, 2012; Graton, Ric, & Gonzalez, 2016; Hoffman, 1994; Tangney, Stuewig, & Mashek, 2007). In fact, guilt is also referred to as a moral emotion because it improves the welfare of individuals and the society as a whole (Haidt, 2003).

However, the excessive attention on interpersonal relationships brought by guilty feelings can be problematic under certain circumstances. For example, imagine one day a colleague whom you have accidentally harmed and hence felt guilty towards, came to you for suggestions on their project presentation, and suppose in your honest opinion the presentation was poorly done. In such a scenario, you need to choose between giving honest (and unpleasant) feedback that may cause immediate relational harm but may improve the presentation in the long run or giving false praise that violates the moral norm of honesty.

To our knowledge, there is limited, if any, research investigating how guilty transgressors will behave when the goal of relationship promotion is incompatible with certain moral norms. In the current study, we demonstrate that guilt can evoke violation of the highly valued and widely accepted moral norm of honesty, especially when honesty is anticipated to incur immediate relational harm with the victim. However, this effect could be attenuated by directing the guilty transgressors' attention to the instrumental value of honesty (i.e., potential long-term benefits of honesty such as meaningful growth, understanding, or change: Levine, 2021). With these findings, we broaden our understanding of the interpersonal function of guilt and its temporal focus.

As a self-conscious negative emotion, guilt often arises from transgressions such as interpersonal neglect, unfilled obligations, and selfish actions in social interactions where the transgressor has harmed another person and violated important norms (e.g., Baumeister et al., 1994; Lewis, 1971). Guilt is also referred to as a “social emotion” because it is “connected with (real or imagined) social interactions” (Barrett, 1995, p.25), and it is experienced more frequently in communal and close relationships (e.g., Baumeister, Stillwell, & Heatherton, 1995).

Accordingly, guilt serves to enhance one's interpersonal relationships and studies have consistently found that guilt is adaptive in its role in relationship maintenance (Ausubel, 1955; O'Connor, Berry, & Weiss, 1999). For instance, guilt stimulates transgressors to take reparative actions such as confession, apology, remorse, and compensation to undo the harm (Baumeister et al., 1994; Lewis, 1971; Lindsay-Hartz, 1984; Tangney, 1991). Guilt also enhances compliance and cooperation (de Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2007; Freedman & Fraser, 1966; Ketelaar & Tung Au, 2003) and fosters other-oriented empathy and perspective-taking (Hoffman, 1994; Yang, Yang, & Chiou, 2010). The goal to repair a damaged relationship can be strong enough such that a guilty transgressor may compensate the victim at the expense of a third-party's best interests (de Hooge, Nelissen, Breugelmans, & Zeelenberg, 2011).

Consistent with the previous research, we define lying as when a communicator intentionally tries to mislead the target and this could be about the objective truth or one's subjective feelings and beliefs that are independent of reality (Boles, Croson, & Murnighan, 2000; DePaulo, Kirkendol, Kashy, Wyer, & Epstein, 1996; Levine & Cohen, 2018; Levine & Schweitzer, 2015).

Lying to a partner is sometimes driven by a relational focus. For example, DePaulo et al. (1996) found that people can lie to prevent conflict, and Ennis, Vrij, and Chance (2008) found that lies to prevent interpersonal conflicts are told more in close relationships. Relatedly, people tend to believe that honesty can sometimes damage the relationship. For instance, people anticipate less liking from poorly-performing recipients if asked to provide honest feedback (Fisher, 1979) and avoid complete self-disclosure in social interactions when complete self-disclosure can hurt others (Rosenfeld, 1979). People are also reluctant to deliver an honest yet hurtful message to a partner due to concerns of being perceived negatively by their partner (Bond & Anderson, 1987). Interestingly, Levine and Cohen (2018) demonstrated that people mispredicted the relational harm brought by honesty such that honesty did less harm than what individuals expected. Irrespective of whether unpleasant honesty is indeed harmful for relationships or not, previous research suggests that lying commonly occurs with the expectation of enhancing interpersonal relationships (e.g., Metts, 1989).

In addition, lying to a third party is expected to protect the relationship between the liar and their partners. For example, Kouchaki and Kray (2018) found that females were more likely to lie to a negotiator when advocating for a partner due to greater relational sensitivity, and Zhu et al. (2020) found that gratitude motivated individuals to lie to third parties t to protect their partner and build a relationship.

Although lying might sometimes help avoid immediate relational harm, it may lead to lost benefits that could have resulted from honesty. For example, giving overly positive feedback to avoid interpersonal tension can inhibit the partner from performance improvement (e.g., Ellis, Mendel, & Aloni-Zohar, 2009; Jampol & Zayas, 2021; Lupoli, Jampol, & Oveis, 2017), and false praise may even lead to hubris that is detrimental for the partner (e.g., Hayward & Hambrick, 1997). Lying to a third party may also jeopardize the third party's best interests when making a decision requires them to obtain truthful information (e.g., Kouchaki & Kray, 2018). In addition, the detection of lies can further worsen, rather than improve, the relationship quality in the long run (e.g., Cole, 2001; Lupoli, Levine, & Greenberg, 2018). As such, situations of unpleasant honesty involve a tradeoff between lying to avoid immediate relational harm and the long-term instrumental benefits associated with honesty. Our framework is consistent with the recent work from Levine (2021), who has provided a parsimonious theoretical account of the acceptability of lying in everyday conversations, such that individuals are more likely to accept lies when the degree of the immediate harm of honesty (e.g., the harm at the moment of communication) is high and the instrumental value of honesty (e.g., the long-term benefits of honesty) is low.

Given the relational focus of guilt, one could conjecture that guilt would decrease lying because it may cause long-term relational damage. The detection that one has been lied to is linked to deleterious interpersonal outcomes such as decreased liking (Tyler, Feldman, & Reichert, 2006), trust (Schweitzer, Hershey, & Bradlow, 2006), and relational commitment (Cole, 2001). Lying detection is often more devastating in close relationships because of high expectations from each other in such relationships (Miller, Mongeau, & Sleight, 1986). Even for the lies that are out of the liar's good intentions, studies have shown that recipients held a strong distaste towards such lies (Lupoli et al., 2018). Since the focus of guilt-induced behavior is to enhance one's relationships, one could predict guilt to prevent lying.

However, in situations where honesty may create immediate relational harm, we suggest that guilt heightens short-term relational focus which, in turn, would increase lying. Functional accounts of emotions indicate that emotions direct people's attention to the problems at hand and urge people to take actions (e.g., Hajcak et al., 2007; Keltner & Gross, 1999; Keltner & Haidt, 1999; Keltner & Kring, 1998; Oatley & Jenkins, 1992). Keltner and Haidt (1999) also suggested that one basic function of emotion is to inform people about their social conditions “typically needing to be acted upon and changed” (p.509). In other words, emotions can create a sense of urgency to focus on the problem that needs to be particularly addressed based on the specific motivational functions of each emotion (Zeelenberg, Nelissen, Breugelmans, & Pieters, 2008). Accordingly, considerable evidence shows that emotions—especially the heightened ones—often make people rush into actions that are short-term focused (e.g., Cyders & Smith, 2008). For example, Lupoli et al. (2017) found that compassion increased participants' lying to the target to avoid immediate emotional harm, even though honest feedback could improve the target's chance to win a prize in a writing game. Gray (1999) found that people experiencing negative emotions preferred short-term benefits and forwent long-term consequences. Furthermore, Fischer and Roseman (2007) found that anger prompted attacking through a short-term focus on coercion while contempt prompted social exclusion through a short-term focus on derogation. In a similar vein, given that guilt engenders attention to the damaged relationship, guilty transgressors would feel a sense of urgency to repair it. Therefore, we predict that when honesty can evoke immediate relational harm with the victim, guilt should increase lying.

Although not explicitly stated, previous studies provided evidence that guilt might make individuals short-sighted with a preoccupation with relational focus, and this is because guilt can direct individuals' attention to reparatory means (Graton & Ric, 2017). In their comprehensive review, Graton and Mailliez (2019) suggested that upon feeling guilty, transgressors may allocate specific attention to immediate reparation that predicted further behaviors. This proposition is corroborated by empirical findings. For example, Ketelaar and Tung Au (2003) found that guilty individuals were more cooperative only during the first ten trials from a total of forty trials in repeated social bargaining games and de Hooge et al. (2007) replicated this finding and further termed the effect of guilt on cooperation as short-termed. Similarly, related literature found that guilt motivated approach tendencies when an opportunity for reparation presented itself (Amodio, Devine, & Harmon-Jones, 2007) and that once a compensation had been made to the victim, the guilty transgressor no longer exerted restoration efforts (de Hooge, 2012; de Hooge, Nelissen, et al., 2011). These results suggest that guilty transgressors will seize the proximal opportunity to repair the relationship, but they are unlikely to continue doing so.

Separately, in a consumer behavior context, Han, Duhachek, and Agrawal (2014) explored the relationship between guilt and psychological distance using construal level theory (e.g., Liberman & Trope, 1998) and found that guilt elicited lower level (i.e., more concrete) construals, which is related to a heightened focus on proximal vis-à-vis distal outcomes (e.g., Bar-Anan, Liberman, & Trope, 2006). Recently, Pounders, Royne, and Lee (2019) supported this notion by showing that guilt appeals were more persuasive when paired with a proximal frame due to higher processing fluency, which further suggested that guilt-laden individuals adopted lower-level construals.

Taken together, previous literature suggests that guilt can make individuals focus on short-term vis-à-vis more distant goals. Therefore, in situations where honesty can bring immediate relational harm, we predict that guilty transgressors are more likely to lie due to their short-term relational focus.

In three experiments, we found that in situations where honesty is anticipated to cause immediate relational harm, guilty participants demonstrated higher lying intentions than the control group. Study 1 examined the effect of guilt on inflating feedback, wherein we measured relational focus and the focus on the instrumental value of honesty as two simultaneous mechanisms and contrasted the effect of guilt on lying intentions with that of shame (another closely related negative emotion). Study 2 extended our investigation by capturing the short-termism in the measure of relational focus and by examining a situation wherein guilty participants could lie to a third party. In Study 3, we provided additional support for our proposed mechanism by asking participants to deliberate on the consequences of not being honest (thereby, indirectly increasing participants' focus on the instrumental value of honesty).

We pre-registered Study 1 and Study 3 and reported all measures, manipulations, exclusions, and sensitivity analyses for all studies. Vignettes were employed in each study to strictly control for the nature of the relationship between the transgressor and the victim, as previous literature has found that the attributes of a relationship could influence transgressors' experience of guilt (Baumeister et al., 1995; Nelissen, 2014). As such, we were only able to measure lying intentions (vis-à-vis actual lying behaviors) based on hypothetical scenarios. However, we believe it is unlikely to impact the validity of our results and we provide a more detailed explanation in the General Discussion section. All data and materials can be found in OSF (https://osf.io/kq67n/?view_only=091686b70a64491384e3965c88c0d8b9).

Section snippets

Study 1: Contrasting guilt with shame

As a starting point, Study 1 (pre-registered at http://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=tr8rz91) studied the effect of guilt on lying. We measured the degree to which participants would inflate their

Study 2: Measuring short-term relational focus

This study extended our investigation in two ways. First, instead of measuring a general sense to repair the relationship as in Study 1, we refined our measure to better capture the sense of a short-term relational focus; second, we expanded the lying context by examining whether participants are willing to lie to a third-party to protect the relationship between them and the victim. Similar to lying to the victim, under certain conditions, lying to other people can serve to protect the

Study 3: Deliberation on consequences of not being honest

In the current study (pre-registered at https://aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=n33i24), we sought to provide additional support for the mechanism of short-term relational focus by increasing the transgressor's focus on the instrumental value of honesty. We posit that if short-term relational focus mediated the relationship between guilt and lying, prompting participants to deliberate on the consequences of not being honest (thereby, redirecting their attention to the longer-term value of honesty;

General discussion

The literature on guilt has provided ample evidence that guilt prompts behaviors that undo the harm and repair the damaged relationship (e.g., Tangney et al., 2007), yet not so much is known when the goal of relational repair is incompatible with the norm of honesty. Our research investigated how guilt can motivate lying behavior when honesty is anticipated to incur immediate relational harm, and we further demonstrated that guilty transgressors focused more on the short-term relational

Conclusion

This research examines when and how guilt increases lying. Across three studies, we demonstrate that guilt increases lying out of short-term relational focus when honesty is anticipated to cause immediate relational harm, and this effect cannot be generalized to another closely related moral emotion—shame. We hope that these results contribute to a better understanding of the complex relational function of guilt and its temporal focus.

Funding

Research reported in this paper was partially funded by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MCIU), State Research Agency (AEI) and European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) Grant No. PGC2018-098767-B-C22.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Open practices

All data and survey materials can be found on Open Science Framework using the following link: https://osf.io/kq67n/?view_only=091686b70a64491384e3965c88c0d8b9

Declaration of Competing Interest

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

References (85)

  • M.J. Lupoli et al.

    Paternalistic lies

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

    (2018)
  • M.E. Schweitzer et al.

    Promises and lies: Restoring violated trust

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

    (2006)
  • J.M. Tyler et al.

    The price of deceptive behavior: Disliking and lying to people who lie to us

    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • S.S. Wiltermuth

    Cheating more when the spoils are split

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

    (2011)
  • D.M. Amodio et al.

    A dynamic model of guilt

    Psychological Science

    (2007)
  • D.P. Ausubel

    Relationships between shame and guilt in the socializing process

    Psychological Review

    (1955)
  • Y. Bar-Anan et al.

    The association between psychological distance and construal level: Evidence from an implicit association test

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2006)
  • K.C. Barrett

    A functionalist approach to shame and guilt

  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Guilt: An interpersonal approach

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1994)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Personal narratives about guilt: Role in action control and interpersonal relationships

    Basic and Applied Social Psychology

    (1995)
  • M. Chen et al.

    Relaxing moral reasoning to win: How organizational identification relates to unethical pro-organizational behavior

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (2016)
  • T. Cole

    Lying to the one you love: The use of deception in romantic relationships

    Journal of Social and Personal Relationships

    (2001)
  • C.E. Cryder et al.

    Guilty feelings, targeted actions

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2012)
  • M.A. Cyders et al.

    Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: Positive and negative urgency

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2008)
  • B.M. DePaulo et al.

    Lying in everyday life

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • S. Ellis et al.

    The effect of accuracy of performance evaluation on learning from experience: The moderating role of after-event reviews

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (2009)
  • E. Ennis et al.

    Individual differences and lying in everyday life

    Journal of Social and Personal Relationships

    (2008)
  • A.H. Fischer et al.

    Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2007)
  • C.D. Fisher

    Transmission of positive and negative feedback to subordinates: A laboratory investigation

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1979)
  • J. Förster et al.

    Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: Consequences for insight and creative cognition

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • J.L. Freedman et al.

    Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1966)
  • N. Gausel et al.

    Concern for self-image and social image in the management of moral failure: Rethinking shame

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • N. Gausel et al.

    Resolving the paradox of shame: Differentiating among specific appraisal-feeling combinations explains pro-social and self-defensive motivation

    Motivation and Emotion

    (2016)
  • F. Gino et al.

    Robin hood under the hood: Wealth-based discrimination in illicit customer help

    Organization Science

    (2010)
  • A. Graton et al.

    A theory of guilt appeals: A review showing the importance of investigating cognitive processes as mediators between emotion and behavior

    Behavioral Sciences

    (2019)
  • A. Graton et al.

    How guilt leads to reparation? Exploring the processes underlying the effects of guilt

    Motivation and Emotion

    (2017)
  • J.R. Gray

    A bias toward short-term thinking in threat-related negative emotional states

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (1999)
  • J. Haidt

    The moral emotions

  • G. Hajcak et al.

    Emotion facilitates action: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of motor cortex excitability during picture viewing

    Psychophysiology

    (2007)
  • D. Han et al.

    Emotions shape decisions Through construal level: The case of guilt and shame

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (2014)
  • A.F. Hayes

    Methodology in the social sciences.Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach

    (2013)
  • M.L.A. Hayward et al.

    Explaining the premiums paid for large acquisitions: Evidence of CEO hubris

    Administrative Science Quarterly

    (1997)
  • Cited by (3)

    • My Family Accounts Much for Me: How Does Work-to-Family Conflict Lead to Unethical Pro-Family Behavior

      2023, International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health

    This paper has been recommended for acceptance by Vanessa Bohns.

    View full text