1 Introduction

The coronavirus disease-19 (COVID-19) pandemic and global climate change are two major threats to society that require tradeoffs between short-term individual preferences and long-term collective well-being. Over 3.3 million people worldwide have died from COVID-19 as of May 2021 (Johns Hopkins COVID-19 Map 2021), with deaths pronounced in regions that did not prioritize measures to contain the virus and protect vulnerable populations. Similarly, negative impacts from climate change are projected to sharply increase if action is not taken to mitigate and adapt to the threat (IPCC 2019), with approximately 143 million people expected to be displaced as a result (Rigaud et al. 2018). Given commonalities in the global scale and severity of these two threats, examining individuals’ response to COVID-19 may allow development of more effective methods of engaging the public with climate change (Bouman et al. 2021; Manzanedo and Manning 2020), as we examine here.

Converging cross-disciplinary research suggests a multidimensional approach to understanding individuals’ reactions to risks (Doherty and Webler 2016; Singleton 2010; Slovic 1987; Tannenbaum et al. 2015). Here we consider reactions to COVID-19 and climate change along three interrelated dimensions: (1) threat perceptions, (2) emotional responses, and (3) normative perceptions.

1.1 Threat perceptions

We evaluate perceived similarities across the two issues. These can highlight potential “learning experiences”: COVID-19-related experiences that could change individuals’ perceptions of climate change (Manzanedo and Manning 2020). For example, individuals could note the shared need for international collective action (Balmford et al. 2020) or society’s lack of advance threat preparation despite robust scientific evidence (Balmford et al. 2020; Kuzemko et al. 2020). Participants could also identify political polarization of both issues (Bergquist et al. 2020; Clinton et al. 2021), which in part reflects some individuals’ mistrust in the scientific evidence and media coverage of the threats (McCright and Dunlap 2011; Oreskes and Conway 2011). The two issues also influence one another, and some may note that an unstable climate is a public health threat (e.g., see Maibach et al. 2021).

We also evaluate possible perceived differences across the two issues, which might be less helpful for climate change communication efforts. Some might note that political and societal responses to date have vastly differed across the two issues. Although both represent complex dynamic systems with lags between action and effect (a potential perceived similarity), individuals may note that relatively short lags with COVID-19 (weeks for community behavior changes) and relatively long for climate (decades for emissions reductions; Howarth et al. 2020). For example, individual-level behaviors such as social distancing and mask wearing are key in fighting COVID-19, whereas addressing climate change is more dependent on system-wide changes (Attari 2021). Many Americans view climate change as geographically and temporally distant (Miniard et al. 2020, but also see Brügger et al. 2021) and may view COVID-19 as more of a present problem than climate change.

1.2 Emotional reactions

Emotional reactions to threats are increasingly recognized as key components of public responses to crises (Brosch 2021; Feldman and Hart 2016; Loewenstein et al. 2001; Lu and Schuldt 2015; Norgaard 2011; Roeser 2012; Slovic and Peters 2006). Here we consider six emotional reactions that have been identified in previous research as being particularly relevant to motivating or demotivating action across various domains: hope, despair, anxiety, anger, exhaustion, and boredom.

We focus most centrally on hope and despair, two future-oriented emotions that represent differential and sometimes overlapping emotional responses to future possibilities following traumatic experiences (Lazarus 1999) and existential threats (Pyszczynski et al. 2003). Hope is a positively valenced, approach-oriented emotion associated with a desired yet uncertain future goal (Lazarus 1999). Feeling hopeful can lower one’s guard against disease contraction (Widmann 2020), but hope-inducing messages can be persuasive and motivational toward addressing health threats (Nabi 2015). Similarly, hope can promote climate action in many contexts (e.g., Bury et al. 2019; Geiger et al. 2021b; Ojala 2012; Smith and Leiserowitz 2014) but potentially discourage it in other contexts (e.g., Hornsey and Fielding 2016; also see van Zomeren et al. 2019). Conversely, despair is a negatively valenced, avoidance-oriented emotion reflecting perceptions of hopelessness and helplessness and is associated with frequent rumination (Copeland et al. 2020). During previous pandemics (i.e., SARS, MERS), many in affected regions experienced despair (and related emotions) during and after the outbreaks (Qian et al. 2005; Yoon et al. 2016). Similarly, learning about or experiencing the effects of climate change can stimulate despair in some (Albrecht 2011; Clayton et al. 2017; Moser 2007; Taylor 2020). Preliminary work suggests that despair (and related affective states) could potentially lead to long-term disengagement with issues such as climate change (Hmielowski et al. 2019; Landry et al. 2018; Salomon et al. 2017).

We also consider how anxiety, anger, boredom, and exhaustion predict responses to COVID-19 and climate change. Anxiety is a common reaction to both threats (Taylor 2020; Wang et al. 2020). Anxiety corresponds with uncertainty about future outcomes and promotes a focus on possible aversive outcomes. Anxiety exerts inconsistent motivational effects: in many cases, anxiety (or fear) can alert individuals to health and environmental threats, thus promoting action and information seeking (Tannenbaum et al. 2015; van Zomeren et al. 2010), but it can alternatively promote psychological avoidance and defensiveness (Kim and Niederdeppe 2013; O’Neill and Nicholson-Cole 2009; Stanley et al. 2021). Anger is an approach-oriented emotion that arises in response to perceived injustice (Van Doorn et al. 2014), and can either facilitate cooperation and motivate pro-social action toward societal threats (such as when it is directed toward those seen as culpable for causing climate change; Jones and Davison 2021; Kleres and Wettergren 2017; Stanley et al. 2021) or can be associated with anti-social or aggressive action (such as when individuals experience reactance toward perceived freedom threats of public health recommendations; Dillard and Peck 2000). Feeling exhausted or bored in response to threats (e.g., “COVID fatigue”) might be associated with disengagement (a lack of action; e.g., Geiger et al. 2021b) as individuals lose motivation due to fatigue or a lack of psychological meaning associated with behaviors (Markowitz et al. 2013; Westgate and Wilson 2018).

Here we examine and compare emotional reactions to COVID-19 and climate change and how they can motivate action across the two threats.

1.3 Normative perceptions

A third dimension guiding threat responses involves individuals’ perceptions of others’ reactions. Decades of social psychological research show that when dealing with novel threats, social cues provide valuable and trusted information regarding the extent to which a threat should be taken seriously (Cialdini and Goldstein 2004). However, individuals commonly misperceive others’ behaviors, falsely perceiving that their own behavior is different from average (Miller and Prentice 1994). Although numerous factors influence these misconceptions, some work suggests that individuals may succumb to the better-than-average bias on pro-environmental behaviors, with most people perceiving that they engage in more pro-environmental behavior than the average person (Leviston and Uren 2020). This tendency can be problematic for encouraging pro-social behaviors because individuals adjust their opinions and behaviors to match their perceptions of others (Ajzen 1991; Goldberg et al. 2020); thus, if individuals perceive they are engaging in greater pro-social action than others, they may reduce their socially constructive behavior to better match the perceived norm (Schultz et al. 2007). Similarly, some have speculated that COVID-19 mitigation behaviors may be strongly influenced by perceptions of the extent to which others engage in such behaviors (Jetten et al. 2020).

There is reason to believe that most individuals might perceive that they are engaging in more socially responsible actions than others on both COVID-19 and climate change. Although compliance with health recommendations have been far from universal, most Americans self-report having extensively adjusted their behavior to contain the spread of COVID-19 (Czeisler et al. 2020). However, some COVID-19 mitigation behaviors, such as remaining at home, result in those engaging being invisible to others, while those who refuse to take such actions are highly visible to others (Jetten et al. 2020). Because of this visibility gap, many may underestimate the sacrifices that others are making to address COVID-19. Similarly, research shows that individuals underestimate others’ concern about climate change (Leviston et al. 2013; Mildenberger and Tingley 2017) and underestimate others’ engagement in green behaviors relative to their own (Leviston and Uren 2020). Here we extend previous work by examining individuals’ comparison of their behaviors to others on several COVID-19 and climate mitigation behaviors.

2 Present research

The present work uses mixed-methods analysis of online survey responses to explore the dimensions considered above.

3 Methods

3.1 Participants and procedure

A sample of US adults (N = 1900) was recruited online via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk) in June 2020 and were compensated $3.00 to $3.30 for participationFootnote 1. Participants who failed both of two attention checks described in Supplemental Materials (n = 116, 6% of sample) were excluded from analyses (final N = 1784).

Overall, our sample was not representative of the US public but exhibited substantial diversity in various socio-demographic measures. Median sample age was 34 years, slightly younger than the median age of the US population of 38.2 years in 2018 (including minors; US Census Bureau 2019). Forty percent of participants identified as female (50.8% in the US), 60% as male, and less than 1% as “other.” Median household income from the previous year (2019) was between $40,000 and 80,000 ($60,226 in the USA). Fifty-nine percent of participants identified as white non-Hispanic, with 13% identifying as white Hispanic and 13% as nonwhite Hispanic, 22% as black/African American, 4% as Asian, 2% as American Indian or Alaska native, and less than 1% as Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander (participants could select more than one ethnicity). Over 99% of participants held high school diplomas or GEDs, and 82% of participants held a college degree. Thirty-four percent of participants self-identified as very liberal/liberal, 39% as moderate/slightly liberal/slightly conservative, and 27% as very conservative/conservative. As shown in Figure S1 (see Supplemental Materials), participants came from all 50 US states and DC. In summary, our sample overrepresents younger adults and the highly educated, yet has sufficient heterogeneity for an initial investigation into key questions.

3.2 Measures

The full survey is available in Supplemental Materials.

3.2.1 Threat perceptions

Participants wrote descriptions of their perceptions of similarities and differences between COVID-19 and climate change, inspired by Attari et al.' (2014) work. For each topic, participants then self-categorized into one of six categories reflecting their concern about the topic (based on Swim and Geiger 2017; modified slightly to create parallel wording for COVID-19). Participants completed two blocks of questions presented in random order, with within-block statement order also randomized. In one block, participants indicated their agreement with 29 statements about COVID-19, and in the other, their agreement with a parallel set of 29 statements about climate change (both on −2 to +2 Likert scales; see statements in Table 1). Statements reflected parallel themes across the two topics that we observed through prevailing media narratives and personal conversations.

Table 1 List of parallel pairs of survey items

Participants next indicated their agreement with nine statements related to immediate climate action preferences on −2 to +2 Likert scales. Five statements suggested preferences for avoiding immediate climate action: “we need to focus on the immediate problem of the economy before we take action on climate change,” “we need to focus on the immediate problem of racial inequality before we take action on climate change,” “we should not prioritize dealing with climate change because there are more pressing issues to deal with,” “we should wait until the Coronavirus outbreak is under control before we take action on climate change,” and “society and the world need time to heal and recover before tackling the next big issue.” The other four statements indicated preferences for immediate climate action: “we need to address climate change, racial inequality, and the Coronavirus outbreak simultaneously,” “given the societal impacts of the Coronavirus outbreak, I feel motivated to tackle other big problems like climate change,” “some of the lessons we have learned from dealing with the Coronavirus outbreak can be applied to addressing climate change,” and “the Coronavirus outbreak has demonstrated our lack of preparation for global problems like climate change.”

3.2.2 Emotional reactions

Participants indicated the extent to which they felt hope, despair, boredom, anxiety, exhaustion, and angerFootnote 2 when considering (a) COVID-19 and (b) climate change on 0 “not at all” to 4 “very” scales. Participants then wrote about what makes them feel (i) hope and (ii) despair about (a) COVID-19 and (b) climate change.

3.2.3 Normative perceptions and self-reported behavior

Participants provided a written description of why some others do not perceive (a) COVID-19 and (b) climate change to be a serious threat. Next, participants self-reported their own behaviors and perceptions of behaviors of others in their community over the past month on 0 “never” to 4 “all the time” scales. We assessed three COVID-19 preventative behaviors: (1) mask wearing in public, (2) avoiding socializing, and (3) staying 6 feet from others, and three climate action behaviors: (1) eating a climate-friendly diet, (2) reducing home energy usage, and (3) political climate action.

3.2.4 Behavioral intentions

Participants indicated their intentions to engage in the three above COVID-19 behaviors (α = .76) and climate behaviors (α = .70) over the next 3 months on the same scale as above.

3.2.5 Conservatism of media consumption

Participants self-assessed the political bent of their news sources on a 7-point scale from “very liberal” to “very conservative” (see Swim et al. 2019).

4 Results

4.1 Threat perceptions

Figure 1 shows participants’ self-categorization on COVID-19 and climate change. Self-categorization on climate change is fairly similar to nationally representative data collected in April 2020 (Leiserowitz et al. 2020). As can be seen in the figure, more participants self-categorize as Alarmed about COVID-19 than climate change.

Figure 1
figure 1

Self-categorization into COVID-19 and climate change concern categories. Categories further to the left represent stronger concern and greater awareness of the issue. Created using ggplot2 package (Wickham et al. 2016) in R (R Core Team 2020)

Next, we analyzed written descriptions of similarities and differences between the two issues. One coder went through to create the original code sheet (37 to 39 unique codes) and coded the first three ideas in each response. Two coders analyzed unclear responses together. Table 2 highlights the top 15 responses for similarities and differences (See Supplemental Text for other responses).

Table 2 Perceived similarities and differences across issues

Table 2 shows that participants perceived many similarities between COVID-19 and climate change: many participants perceived both issues to be harmful to public health, politically polarizing, have global effects, are human caused, and to have solutions. Some participants highlighted the need for collective action or that climate change could exacerbate COVID-19. Some participants’ copy-pasted responses from the web instead of writing a response.

Table 2 also shows perceived differences between the two topics. Many perceived the issues to fall into different domains, perceiving COVID-19 as a medical issue and climate change as an environmental issue. Related to this perceived distinction, some participants believed COVID-19 to be naturally occurring and climate change to be human caused. Many responses related to the time horizon of both threats, reflecting perceptions of COVID-19 as a short-term problem, happening now, and with immediate impacts and short-term solutions, in contrast to climate change as a long-term problem, gradually built, and with long-term impacts and long-term solutions.

Figure 2 shows that most expressed greater agreement with statements supporting the need for immediate climate action than they did with statements suggesting not taking immediate climate action. For example, participants tended to express greater agreement with the notion that the two issues could be addressed simultaneously relative to the notion that climate change action should not be taken until after COVID-19 was addressed.

Figure 2
figure 2

Means and 95% confidence intervals for perceived connections between COVID-19 and other issues. Responses are arranged from greatest to least agreement

Next, we examined correlations for responses to each pair of parallel items that participants had completed for both COVID-19 and climate change, depicted in Figure 3. All item pairs were positively correlated with one another, yet the magnitudes of correlation drastically differed across different item pairs. On the most related end, the item pairs regarding endorsement of authoritarianism as a solution to each threat were very strongly correlated. Similarly, item pairs reflecting perceptions of (a) the extent to which the USA was cooperating internally, (b) participants’ own knowledge, and (c) scientists’ knowledge for each threat were strongly correlated between the two threats. Finally, item pairs related to mistrust, including perceptions that (a) others were overreacting to the threats, (b) the issues were under control, and (c) that actions to address the issues shouldn’t hurt the economy, were strongly correlated between the two threats. On the least related end, item pairs reflecting perceptions that threats revealed competition between communities and concern about how each problem would affect various others were only moderately correlated, suggesting a distinction between how individuals perceived these components of the two topics.

Figure 3
figure 3

Point estimates and 95% confidence intervals for inter-item correlations between responses to parallel items about COVID-19 and climate change. Items are arranged from strongest to weakest correlations. Dashed vertical lines represent cutoffs for small (r = .1), medium (r = .3), and large (r = .5) correlations as defined by Cohen (1988). Clim., climate. Created using the forestplot package (Gordon et al. 2019) in R.

4.2 Emotional reactions

Following Smith and Leiserowitz’s (2014) work examining links between emotions and climate policy support, we used multiple regression examine the how each emotional appraisal uniquely predicts topic-relevant behavioral intentions. In Table 3, we regressed climate action intentions onto emotion reactions to climate change, and in a parallel regression, we regressed COVID-19 action intentions onto emotional reactions to COVID-19. In both, we control for media conservatism. See Tables S1 and S2 in Supplemental Materials for zero-order correlations and supplemental analyses.

Table 3 Emotional reactions to each risk predicting topic-relevant action intentions

All six emotional reactions predicted climate action intentions, while COVID-19 action intentions were predicted by despair, anxiety, and (negatively) boredom, but not hope, exhaustion, or anger. Overall, emotional reactions were much stronger predictors of intentions to act on climate (30% variance explained) than COVID-19 (8% variance explained).

As shown in Table 4, a variety of topics fostered hope about COVID-19. In particular, scientific advances and the possibility of a medical solution made many participants hopeful, as did contemplating an end to the outbreak. Collective and individual actions and efficacy also made many participants feel hopeful. A relatively small number of participants indicated not feeling hopeful regarding the topic.

Table 4 Participants’ descriptions of elicitors of hope regarding each threat

Table 4 also showed that responses were relatively similar for climate change: as with COVID-19, many participants (albeit a smaller percentage) reported feeling hopeful due to possible future science and technology to address the issue. Collective and individual actions and efficacy also emerged as a source of hope for climate change. Unlike with COVID-19, some participants reported feeling hopeful based on a perception that climate change is exaggerated.

As shown in Table 5, there were both similarities and differences in sources of despair for COVID-19 and climate change. Two key themes emerged for both topics: incompetent government response and the perception that others were not taking the issue seriously. In contrast, more participants reported feeling no despair about climate change than regarding COVID-19, and a small number of people stated that other people’s belief in climate change resulted in their feelings of despair. The direct health and economic consequences of COVID-19 emerged as key themes inciting despair. A relatively small percentage of participants cited poor mental health as a source of despair.

Table 5 Participants’ descriptions of elicitors of despair regarding each of the two threats

4.3 Normative perceptions

Table 6 shows codes for why participants believe that others do not regard COVID-19 and climate change as serious threats (shown in Table 6). Many participants reasoned that others did not believe COVID-19 was serious because it seemed “invisible” (i.e., many others had not personally experienced or witnessed an infection), elite cues (e.g., then-President Trump downplaying the threat), or ignorance. Other participants explained that others downplay the threat because they were uninformed about the science, mistakenly perceived COVID-19 to be comparable to the flu, or perceived low mortality rates and a strong possibility of recovery. Additionally, some participants noted politicization of the topic and lack of trust in media and government as potential causes of others’ lack of caution.

Table 6 Respondents’ beliefs about why others believe that COVID-19 and climate change are not serious threats

Participants’ reasoning for why others did not take climate change seriously echoed many similar themes to COVID-19. Specifically, many participants reasoned that the perceived invisibility of climate change and lack of information explained others’ lack of concern about the issue. Also, participants attributed some blame to inaccurate information provided by the government and then-President Trump, politicization, and lack of trust in media and scientific authorities. In contrast, with climate change, some emphasized monetary interests and propaganda spread by powerful interest groups (e.g., fossil fuel companies) and corrupt politicians. Moreover, some perceived that others may attribute climate change to natural causes or believe it to be a hoax.

On average, participants believed that they were doing more than others in their community (i.e., the “better-than-average” effect) on both COVID-19 and climate. As shown in Table 7, this effect was particularly pronounced for COVID-19 behaviors: participants, on average, believed that they were doing moderately (as defined by Cohen 1988) more than others on all three COVID-19 behaviors assessed here. Similarly, on climate change, participants, on average, believed that they engaged in moderately more efforts to reduce home energy consumption than others. In contrast, participants, on average, believed that they only engaged in slightly more climate-friendly diet efforts than others and, on average, did not exhibit the better-than-average effect regarding political climate action.

Table 7 Differences in self-reported behavior and perception of others’ behaviors

5 Discussion

5.1 Threat perceptions

Respondents indicated many similarities in threat perceptions of COVID-19 and climate change. Doubting the severity of COVID-19 (e.g., believing that the risk was exaggerated) was highly correlated with similar doubts about climate change, suggesting the possibility that mistrust might reflect a deeper worldview and mistrust of risk communicators. Further, the extent to which participants believed that authoritarian government was needed to address COVID-19 was closely related to a similar perception for climate change, suggesting the possibility that COVID-19 might foster a defensive or authoritarian mindset for some (Stanley et al. 2019) that could extend to other threats. Participants’ agreement with statements indicating that lessons from COVID-19 could be applied to climate change suggests that COVID-19 has been a useful “learning experience” for the need to take climate action, at least when individuals were explicitly asked about the connection. Qualitative results also showed that many recognized that both issues would create negative health impacts, although this similarity was not universally recognized as many others described climate change as an “environmental issue” (vs. COVID-19 as a “health issue”).

We also found differences in threat perceptions between the two threats. COVID-19 risk perceptions (concern about effects on self and others) were only moderately correlated with climate change risk perceptions. Many also perceived the two threats as unfolding on different timescales: unlike COVID-19, many viewed climate change as a “future problem” (see Miniard et al. 2020; Brügger et al. 2021). These differences suggest that communicating about COVID-19 risks may not necessarily promote awareness of the climate risks. Perhaps communication efforts could focus on explaining connections between these risks in a way that is more salient to lay individuals.

5.2 Emotional reactions

Participants indicated both substantive similarities in sources of hope and despair to the two topics. Considering individual and collective action made some participants hopeful about solving both issues. This leads us to an intriguing speculation: if perceptions of individual and collective action are increased on COVID-19, would that also increase hope in finding climate solutions? We also found that perceived apathy and denial made participants feel despair about both topics. Future work should examine whether decreasing perceptions of others’ apathy and denial on COVID-19 might also decrease despair on climate change.

In understanding possible links between feelings and climate engagement, some have focused primarily on distinguishing positive from negative feelings (e.g., O’Neill and Nicholson-Cole 2009), while other work suggests that the meaning that individuals ascribe to emotional experiences might be more important than affective valence (Geiger et al. 2021a; Geiger et al. 2021b; also see Chapman et al. 2017) Consistent with the latter perspective, we found that two negative emotions that commonly (although not always) correspond to deeply embedded psychological meaning (i.e., anxiety and despair) predicted greater action intentions on both threats, while boredom, which typically reflects lack of psychological meaning, predicted lesser action intentions on both threats. Yet, not all patterns were similar across the two issues. Two other emotions commonly associated with psychological meaning (anger and hope) predicted greater climate action intentions, but not COVID-19 action intentions. The former could align with van Zomeren et al.' (2019) argument that hope can serve a motivational role in some cases but in other cases simply function to improve mood. Specifically, they argue that hope is likely to be motivational when individuals also perceive that their own action can contribute to a better future. Thus, it is possible that many participants might have been better able to envision how their actions could bring about climate progress than how their actions would address COVID-19. Unexpectedly, exhaustion, which could be associated with a lack of meaning, predicted greater climate action intentions (no relationship with COVID-19 action intentions). Overall, it appears that climate action intentions are more closely related to emotional experiences, and with a more diverse array of emotions, than are COVID-19 action intentions.

5.3 Normative perceptions

Extending other work demonstrating that individuals often misperceive others’ opinions and behaviors (Geiger and Swim 2016; Lambert et al. 2003; Leviston and Uren 2020; Miller and Prentice 1994), we found preliminary evidence for the “better-than-average” effect across many behaviors with both threats (Leviston and Uren 2020). Interestingly, and counter to expectations, this effect was approximately equally pronounced for both relatively invisible (avoiding social interaction) and visible (wearing a face mask) COVID-19 behaviors. In contrast, the better-than-average effect seemed to vary as expected based on visibility of climate change behaviors: participants exhibited a substantial better-than-average effect regarding perceptions of others’ attempts to save energy (largely invisible) but less so for climate-related food choices (somewhat visible), and not at all for political participation (fairly visible). Perhaps the novelty of COVID-19 (and the fact that many had less social exposure than usual) led to a greater reliance on media coverage rather than personal experience to assess perceptions of others’ behaviors, thus rendering behavioral visibility moot on this topic (see Chia and Lee 2008). Qualitative results add clarity to the reasoning behind perceptions that others lack concern: many believe that politicization, intentional misinformation, threat invisibility, and ignorance explain others’ lack of concern regarding both topics.

Although it is possible that these better-than-average perceptions were accurate (i.e., it is possible that MTurk workers may be engaging in more COVID-19 and climate change mitigation behaviors than others in their communities), these findings provide preliminary evidence for public misperceptions regarding other's engagement with many COVID-19 and climate change mitigation behaviors (although not for the publicly visible behavior of political climate action). Future work should consider providing individuals with better insight into others’ behaviors and the motives behind them, such as communicating that others use less energy in their homes than many believe (see Schultz et al. 2007), might be useful in promoting action toward these threats.

6 Limitations and future directions

Our findings may not generalize to all populations. Although our sample provides substantial heterogeneity for an initial investigation of key questions, it is not fully representative of the US adult population. Future work should also consider cross-cultural heterogeneity, as the USA has exhibited high media coverage of the climate denial movement (Painter and Ashe 2012) relative to most other industrialized countries. Additionally, future researchers examining the better-than-average effect may consider asking participants to compare themselves to a group of which the sample is representative (e.g., asking MTurk workers about perceptions of other MTurk workers). Our findings may also be specific to the time of data collection (fairly early in the COVID-19 pandemic). Risk perceptions of COVID-19 might increase as more people know someone who has been severely affected by COVID-19 (Verdery et al. 2020) or as case numbers rise sharply, and decrease as “COVID fatigue” spreads (Godlee 2020) or as case numbers fall sharply and more become vaccinated against the virus. Despite limitations in generalizability, this work provides a useful preliminary picture into responses to these two societal threats.

Future work could examine how contextual information surrounding emotional reactions to threats influences their motivational effects. As Chapman et al. (2017) note, emotions are not “simple levers for behavior change” and rather part of more complex psychological processes. Future work could examine questions such as whether anxiety arising in response to the subjective uncertainty of COVID-19 could promote COVID-mitigation actions, while anxiety arising due to economic insecurity caused by the outbreak (Witteveen and Velthorst 2020) could discourage COVID-mitigation actions. Additionally, longitudinal work could examine emotion-behavior links on different timescales, as it is possible, for example, that despair could promote action in the short term (consistent with our findings) but lead to disengagement over time (as suggested by Hmielowski et al. 2019).

7 Conclusion

As COVID-19 vaccines are mass-distributed in 2021 and onwards, a key question is whether we will aspire to return to “normal” or whether society will learn from the crisis and work to “build back better.” Communicators and other social scientists are seeking to understand how the unspeakable social tragedies caused by the COVID-19 pandemic may have also created an opportunity to better engage the public with other societal threats such as climate change (Bouman et al. 2021). Our work represents an initial exploration into identifying learning opportunities from COVID-19 that can be used to more effectively engage people on climate change. We find significant overlaps in threat perceptions, emotional reactions, and normative perceptions across the two issues, although these overlaps are far from universal. We hope that our findings can be used to identify fruitful avenues of communication for public engagement on both topics. As the severity of the climate crisis becomes more apparent, researchers, communicators, and practitioners will need a more diverse toolbox to better engage the public.