Skip to main content

POLICY AND PRACTICE REVIEWS article

Front. Psychol., 16 July 2021
Sec. Gender, Sex and Sexualities
This article is part of the Research Topic Critical perspectives on gender equality policies and practices for staff in higher education View all 10 articles

Facing Racism and Sexism in Science by Fighting Against Social Implicit Bias: A Latina and Black Woman’s Perspective

  • 1Department of Neurobiology, Institute of Biology, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 2Laboratory of Neurobiology, Instituto de Biofísica Carlos Chagas Filho, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil
  • 3Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Biomedical Institute, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 4Department of Physics, Institute of Physics, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 5Department of Sociology and Methodology of Social Sciences, Institute of Human Sciences and Philosophy, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 6Department of Cellular and Molecular Biology, Institute of Biology, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 7Chemistry Institute, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 8Biomedical Institute, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil
  • 9Institute of Biology, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, Brazil

The editors of several major journals have recently asserted the importance of combating racism and sexism in science. This is especially relevant now, as the COVID-19 pandemic may have led to a widening of the gender and racial/ethnicity gaps. Implicit bias is a crucial component in this fight. Negative stereotypes that are socially constructed in a given culture are frequently associated with implicit bias (which is unconscious or not perceived). In the present article, we point to scientific evidence that shows the presence of implicit bias in the academic community, contributing to strongly damaging unconscious evaluations and judgments of individuals or groups. Additionally, we suggest several actions aimed at (1) editors and reviewers of scientific journals (2) people in positions of power within funding agencies and research institutions, and (3) members of selection committees to mitigate this effect. These recommendations are based on the experience of a group of Latinx American scientists comprising Black and Latina women, teachers, and undergraduate students who participate in women in science working group at universities in the state of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil. With this article, we hope to contribute to reflections, actions, and the development of institutional policies that enable and consolidate diversity in science and reduce disparities based on gender and race/ethnicity.

Introduction

“Science has a racism problem,” claimed an editorial of the important journal “Cell” (Edge, 2020). Editors from a variety of respected scientific journals, such as Nature and Science, have recently asserted the importance of combating racism and sexism in science. Especially after the COVID-19 pandemic, several pieces of evidence suggest that gender and racial gaps may be widened (Collins et al., 2020; Myers et al., 2020; Staniscuaski et al., 2020). For instance, Staniscuaski et al. (2021), analyzing academic productivity, showed that male academics—especially childless academics—were the group least affected by the pandemic. In contrast, female academics, especially Black women and mothers, were the most impacted group.

Although the fight against racism and sexism in science involves several aspects, socially constructed implicit bias is a key component in this fight. “Bias” is a concept that refers to analysis, judgments, or attitudes that do not adhere to the principles of impartiality. Bias against a person or group can lead to unfair assessments. This judgmental bias can be explicit or implicit (not perceived), and it can occur due to skin color, ethnicity, religion, gender, sexual orientation, weight, physical, or mental disability, among others (Greenwald and Krieger, 2006; Staats et al., 2015). Implicit (unconscious or unperceived) negative judgment bias in the academic sphere is generally associated with social stereotypes of individuals who are stigmatized as intellectually limited or incapable. Importantly, a social stereotype is a mental association of a social group or category with a characteristic or trait that may or may not be favorable (Greenwald and Krieger, 2006). In other words, stereotypes are socially constructed beliefs that do not necessarily reflect reality (Allport, 1954; Ashmore and DelBoca, 1981; Greenwald and Banaji, 1995). Such social constructions, which are determined by culture and the unequal distribution of resources and power in a community, have substantial influence on the unconscious evaluations and judgments of individuals or groups (Staats et al., 2015; Storage et al., 2016). Stereotypes that are repeatedly and imperceptibly transmitted through several information channels induce implicit beliefs that are used to organize and socially categorize the world and provide rationales for entrenched inequalities (Gaucher et al., 2011; Kang, 2012; Gálvez et al., 2019; Rivera and Tilcsik, 2019). These implicit associations are more prevalent than explicit prejudice, which means that even people who consciously believe in and defend the principles of justice and non-discrimination can have their judgment affected by implicit bias, without their knowledge (Staats et al., 2014). In fact, evidence suggests that implicit bias can be a better predictor of behavior than explicit bias (Bargh and Chartrand, 1999; Ziegert and Hanges, 2005). While explicit biases are conscious attributions that are accessible through introspection, implicit biases are more difficult to become conscious of. Nevertheless, implicit bias can be assessed through experimental paradigms using a diversity of approaches and research tools (see below).

Implicit Gender Bias

Negative implicit stereotypes are shaped by experience and are based on implicit learned associations between the culturally constructed putative characteristics of members of social categorical groups, including those based on race, gender, and socioeconomic status. The presence of these stereotypes leads to implicit bias in judgments of stigmatized individuals or groups (Greenwald and Banaji, 1995). The formation of implicit gender stereotypes, which associate characteristics of exceptional brilliance and intelligence to the male gender, seems to start early in life (Bian et al., 2017) and is reinforced by daily experiences in which members of a categorical group appear to be associated with economic precariousness and a lack of power (Tilly, 1998). In the study of Bian et al. (2017), children from 5 to 7 years old listened to a text that described a brilliant person. Then, children viewed pictures of women’s and men’s faces and were asked to indicate which person was the character in the story. Among the five-year-old children, both boys and girls chose photographs of people of their own gender. However, among children aged 6 and older, only boys continued to indicate the pictures of people of their own gender as the brilliant character in the story, while girls became less likely to choose photographs of women. Considering that children at this age generally show positive biases toward their own in-groups (e.g., those of the same gender), this result suggests that the consequences of the stereotype that brilliance is a male characteristic occur very early and that this stereotype already begins to impact girls between 5 and 6 years old (Bian et al., 2017). Interestingly, a study showed that national gender differences in science and math success are associated with national differences in implicit gender-science stereotypes. Specifically, the stronger the nation’s citizens’ implicit association of men with science and women with the liberal arts, the greater the gap between female and male adolescents’ eighth-grade science achievement in that nation (Nosek et al., 2009). There is evidence that implicit bias acts incisively in adulthood, harming women. One study showed that when university faculty (both men and women) analyzed an identical curriculum for a laboratory manager position with either a male or a female name, the faculties evaluated the curriculum with a male name as more competent and deserving a higher salary (Moss-Racusin et al., 2012). In the same vein, Reuben et al. (2014) carried out a study in which participants (men and women) who were volunteers in laboratory research were rewarded for “hiring” a good candidate to perform mathematical tests. Women were systematically less chosen than men in all three experimental conditions tested as: (1) a condition in which no skill information and only information about the physical appearance of the candidates was provided (2) a condition in which the candidates could give a speech to talk about their mathematical skills, and (3) a condition in which information about the candidates’ performance on a previous math test was provided. Interestingly, in this last experimental condition, the power of the effect of implicit bias was clearly demonstrated, as the “employers” preferred to choose men with low performance in mathematics over women with good performance. The authors also reported that in condition (2), when the candidates were allowed to talk about their skills, the male candidates overestimated their math skills, while the female candidates did the opposite.

The presence of this implicit bias against women causes considerable damage to the development of their scientific careers. Only 18.1% of articles published in high-impact journals (Nature research journals) have women as senior authors (last authorship), and the higher the journal’s impact index is the smaller the number of women listed as the principal author (Bendels et al., 2018). In addition, articles with women as the principal author are less cited than those with men as the principal author (Larivière et al., 2013). Recently, Dworkin et al. (2020) analyzed high-impact neuroscience journals and found that papers with men listed as the first or last author were cited 11.6% more than expected given the proportion of such articles in the field, and papers with women listed as the first or last author were cited 30.2% less than expected. Importantly, however, when articles are reviewed anonymously (double-blind review), the number of articles published with women listed as the first author increases (Budden et al., 2008), highlighting the impact of implicit bias in this process. Women who have authored the same number of publications with the same publication impact as men are less likely to become research leaders (Van Dijk et al., 2014). Additionally, letters of recommendation written for women use significantly fewer adjectives that represent intelligence and brilliance (Dutt et al., 2016; Kuo, 2016).

In terms of research funding, the effects of implicit bias against women are also significant. A study based on data from a Swedish funding agency reported that women need to author twice as many publications to obtain the same scientific competence score as men (Wenneras and Wold, 1997). Recently, a study based on funding provided by the NIH (a US research funding agency and one of the largest such agencies in the world) revealed that men obtain more funding renewal than women (Pohlhaus et al., 2011). A Dutch study showed no difference between men and women in the quality of the research proposal/project submitted for funding. However, in their sample, women received less funding due to lower scores in the “quality of the researcher” (Van der Lee and Ellemers, 2015). In the same vein, a Canadian study showed that the funding gap is generated by an unfavorable view of women as scientific leaders and not based on the quality of their studies (Witteman et al., 2019). Importantly, when evaluation committees of funding agencies are aware of gender bias against women, the unequal distribution of funding between men and women is less likely to occur (Régner et al., 2019).

Implicit Racial/Ethnicity Bias

Although the studies discussed above focus on gender stereotypes, the literature also describes implicit judgment bias based on skin color and ethnicity. For example, in one study, fictitious resumes with white-sounding names received 50% more callbacks for interviews than resumes with African-American-sounding names (Bertrand and Mullainathan, 2004). Jaxon et al. (2019) demonstrated in children that the association of brilliance with male gender might depend on the race of the person being evaluated. This intersectional study showed that children associated brilliance with White men but not with Black men (Jaxon et al., 2019). Storage et al. (2016) evaluated the frequency with which college students commented whether their professors were “brilliant” or a “genius” in course reviews on a popular Web site.1 They showed that fields in which “brilliant” and “genius” appeared more often were also less likely to be pursued by African–American PhDs, predicting less diversity at the PhD level. This evidence indicates a strong racial bias that helps explain, for instance, the extremely low percentage of faculty positions and PhDs earned by African Americans in STEM (National Science Foundation, 2015; U.S. Department of Education, 2017; Bernard and Cooperdock, 2018). Baron et al. (2006) used an adaptation of the implicit association test (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998) to assess racial bias in children. Reaction time paradigms, on which the IAT is based, have been long used in studies of attention and motivation. Faster or slower response can indicate preset congruent or incongruent association in brain processing, respectively. Baron et al. (2006) tested for associations between the stereotyped group (race: Black and White) and stereotyped domain (evaluation: words with positive connotations and words with negative connotations) and showed that negative implicit race bias was already present in white children aged 6–10 years. The authors also observed that explicit beliefs about race became more egalitarian over time, but implicit race bias remained unchanged.

In a very recent interesting study, Eaton et al. (2020) probed the implicit bias for gender and its association with race/ethnicity. The authors developed an experimental design in which physics and biology professors from United States Research Universities were asked to evaluate identical curriculum vitae (CV) depicting a hypothetical doctoral graduate applying for a postdoctoral position in their field. The reviewers were asked to rate the candidate on competence, hireability, and likeability. The candidate’s name on the CV was used to manipulate race/ethnicity (Asian, Black, Latinx, and White) and gender (female or male), with all other aspects of the CV being the same across conditions. The authors found for physics reviewers an interaction between candidate gender and race/ethnicity. Black women and Latinx candidates were rated the lowest in hireability. This result suggested the robust combined effect of gender and racial/ethnicity biases.

The stereotype of being incompetent/unreliable (Fiske et al., 1999; Jimeno-Ingrum et al., 2009; Pérez, 2010) creates unfair disadvantages for Latinx scientists, especially in the context of leadership roles or to gain recognition for their studies. The persistent lack of Latinx and African representation on editorial boards is an example of the consequences of racism in the academic world (Espin et al., 2017). Latinx exclusion is so problematic that even the widely applied test used to detect/study automatic attitudes and implicit bias for putative stereotype groups, IAT, did not originally include this topic. The first study to adapt an IAT to detect implicit bias toward Latinx individuals was developed much later than the original studies (Pérez, 2010). Thus, discussions about implicit bias and stereotypes and their harmful effects are imperative in science and should consider the intersections between gender and race/ethnicity.

Stereotype Threat

Another harmful consequence of unfounded cultural stigma is low performance on cognitive tasks generated by the threat of stereotypes. Stereotype threat is a psychological phenomenon that involves people feeling at risk of conforming to negative stereotypes about their social group (Steele and Aronson, 1995; see also the review by Spencer et al., 2016). Stereotype threat makes an individual feel a sense of exclusion and lack of belonging that generates psychological stress or anxiety and impairs performance in different situations. Social bonds are necessary for survival and are extremely salient in human beings (Tomasello, 2014), which was highlighted by the COVID-19 pandemic (Bzdok and Dunbar, 2020). Human beings have a constant motivation to form and maintain lasting, positive, and significant interpersonal relationships, even in only a minimal number of these relationships (Baumeister and Leary, 1995). Likewise, perceived social isolation is one of the most pervasive threats to human wellbeing (Cacioppo and Cacioppo, 2014). Humans react to cues of social rejection or exclusion by triggering the autonomic, endocrine and immune systems similarly to when confronting physical attacks or life-threatening events (Eisenberger, 2012), leading authors to tie the word “pain” to both physical and social wounds (see Eisenberger et al., 2003). In fact, neuroimaging studies have shown an overlap of neural representations for social and physical pain (Kross et al., 2011; Eisenberger, 2012). Indeed, in the most efficient experimental protocol to study stress, participants perform speech and cognitive tasks while being ostensively evaluated by a board of trained researchers (Kudielka et al., 2007). The potentially negative evaluation and the fear of failure trigger the reactions of social pain, focusing attentional resources on the threat and weakening performance (Gruenewald et al., 2004; Angelidis et al., 2019).

Belonging to a group stigmatized by negative stereotypes in academic domains exacerbates the pain of social isolation, causing an upward spiral of physiological and mental stress and harmful impairments to performance (Blascovich et al., 2001; Croizet et al., 2004; Allen and Friedman, 2015). Stereotype threat also reduces working memory capacity (Schmader and Johns, 2003; Rydell et al., 2009), which is extremely important to perform well in tasks. Working memory is diverted to address the survival-related threat of social exclusion through intrusive thoughts, anxiety, and stress that are imposed by stereotype threat (Schmader and Johns, 2003). Thus, unsurprisingly, even subtle situational cues for the stress due to stereotype threat can lead to a reduction in performance. In the seminal studies by Steele and Aronson (1995), the authors showed that African American college students performed worse than European American college students on a verbal task under an experimental condition of stereotype threat, in which the task was described as a “diagnostic of intellectual ability.” In the non-stereotype threat condition, in which the task was described as “a laboratory problem-solving task that was non-diagnostic of ability,” Black and white participants performed equally (Steele and Aronson, 1995). Employing a similar paradigm in France, Croizet and Claire (1998) showed that students with low socioeconomic status performed significantly worse than those with high socioeconomic status in the diagnostic condition but equally well in the non-diagnostic condition. Désert et al., 2009 observed that children with low socioeconomic status (6–9 years old) are already vulnerable to stereotype threat. Low-status children performed significantly worse under a diagnostic condition than under a non-diagnostic condition in a test of intellectual ability, whereas high-status children were unaffected. Other experimental approaches showed undermining of women’s performance in mathematical tests by inducing subtle cues of gender stereotype threat (e.g., Spencer et al., 1999; Dar-Nimrod and Heine, 2006). Indeed, math-gender cultural stereotypes seem to already affect girls, both implicitly and explicitly, at 6–10 years old (Cvencek et al., 2011).

Furthermore, Johns et al. (2005) performed a study in which men and women completed difficult math problems that were described as a problem-solving task “for a study of general aspects of cognitive processes” or a math test “for a study of gender differences in mathematics performance.” As expected, the results showed that women performed worse than men when the problems were described as a math test because of the stereotype threat created by the association between women and poor performance in math. Interestingly, when the participants were informed about the stereotype threat phenomenon, the differences in performance between women and men disappeared, indicating that “knowing is half the battle,” as the authors suggested in the paper title. Despite all the evidence showing that the stereotype threat is a robust phenomenon, some experimental paradigms have failed to replicate these data or generalize from the laboratory to real-world testing situations (Cullen et al., 2004, 2006; Sackett et al., 2004). However, as pointed out by Spencer et al. (2016), there is converging evidence that indicates that the stereotype threat is, in fact, responsible for decreases in performance in real tests. In addition, as suggested by Spencer et al. (2016), the experimental design must be carefully planned to capture the phenomenon of stereotype threat.

Considering these data, individual and institutional actions to disseminate this knowledge about stereotype threat are fundamental to reduce it among stereotyped groups. We believe these actions would be a powerful approach to fight racism, gender disparity, and the false belief of low intellectual ability of those from disadvantaged socioeconomic environments.

In sum, there is ample evidence indicating the presence of unseen forces that work to prevent the progression of women, Latinx, and Black people to positions of greater prominence and leadership, including in the academic world. In Figures 13, we suggest several actions aimed at (1) editors and reviewers of scientific journals (2) people in positions of power within funding agencies and research institutions, and (3) to members of selection committees to mitigate this effect. These recommendations are based on the experience of a group of Latinx American scientists comprising Black and Latinx women, teachers, and undergraduate students who participate in women in science working group at universities in the state of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Suggestions for people in positions of power within scientific journals.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Suggestions for people in positions of power within funding agencies and research institutions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Suggestions for people in positions of power within selection committees.

Why is Diversity Important for Science?

Diversity in science can promote new discoveries, as it expands the points of view, issues, and areas addressed by researchers (Nielsen et al., 2017). Scientists from different backgrounds may choose to investigate different questions, and more importantly, they may approach the same question in different ways. For instance, historically, bird song has been associated with males seeking to attract females. However, a deeper look at this question performed by women researchers showed that female song is common and that both sexes probably sang in the common ancestor of modern songbirds (Riebel et al., 2019). Hong and Page (2004) showed that when participants try to solve complex problems, the ability to see the problem differently, not simply “being smart,” often is the key to discovery. Indeed, when groups of different individuals are working to solve difficult problems, the diversity of the problem-solvers matters more than their individual ability. Another important example of the importance of diversity in the coordination of scientific research concerns the understanding of physiological differences related to health problems. There is evidence that diversity among doctors and health professionals improves access to care for underprivileged groups, develops culturally informed care, and expands the health research agenda (Cohen et al., 2002; Jackson and Gracia, 2014; Valantine and Collins, 2015). Then, diversity promotes perspectives from different angles, contributing to a more complete understanding of the topic.

Despite the importance of diversity in science, research conducted by underrepresented groups is frequently underestimated. Hofstra et al. (2020) showed that underrepresented groups produce higher rates of scientific novelty. Surprisingly, this study showed that the innovative and disruptive contributions made by underrepresented groups are undervalued and are less accepted by other scholars than are new contributions by gender and racial majorities. In addition, they showed that equally impactful contributions from gender and racial minorities are less likely to result in successful scientific careers. This evidence shows the inequality and injustice that is perpetuated in science. For the building of a fair and truly excellent scientific community, we need efficient policies that promote gender and racial/ethnicity equity.

Conclusion

Converging evidence in the literature suggests that explicit and implicit biases related to gender and race/ethnicity are powerful forces that foster the disparities and inequalities found in our society. Cognitive control can allow individuals to more easily refute explicit bias as they consciously perceive it. However, implicit bias is more prevalent than explicit bias. Therefore, it is crucial to increase awareness of the commonly ignored implicit biases so that each of us can cognitively resignify them. Additionally, institutions must submit proposals to mitigate this problem. With this article, we hope to contribute to reflections, actions, and the development of institutional policies that enable and consolidate diversity in science and reduce disparities in gender, race/ethnicity, which is essential to improve innovation and, therefore, the progress of inclusive science. If we want to combat racism and sexism in science, we need to combat socially constructed implicit bias. This issue is especially important now, as the COVID-19 pandemic may widen the gender and racial gap. Implicit bias is an unseen force that prevents us from moving toward the construction of a more inclusive and diverse science.

Author Contributions

KC, LO, FE, and EV conceived the presented idea of writing a manuscript on this subject. All authors contributed to the literature review and discussion of recommendations. All authors contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported in part by federal and state Brazilian research agencies (CNPq; CAPES 614 001; CAPES/PRINT; FAPERJ; CNPQ/Institutos Nacionais de ciência e tecnologia/ Instituto Nacional de Neurociência Translacional CNPq/INCT/ INNT; FINEP). KCC, LO, MPG, and EV thank CNPQ and FAPERJ for individual research fellowship.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to especially thank all the members of the “Women in Science” working group at Universidade Federal Fluminense.

Footnotes

References

Allen, B., and Friedman, B. H. (2015). Threatening the heart and mind of gender stereotypes: can imagined contact influence the physiology of stereotype threat? Soc. Psychophysiol. Res. 53, 105–112. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allport, G. W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Google Scholar

Angelidis, A., Solis, E., Lautenbach, F., van der Does, W., and Putman, P. (2019). I’m going to fail! Acute cognitive performance anxiety increases threat-interference and impairs WM performance. PLoS One 14:e0210824. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210824

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashmore, R. D., and Del Boca, F. K. (1981). “Conceptual approaches to stereotypes and stereotyping” in Cognitive Processes in Stereotyping and Intergroup Behavior. ed. D. L. Hamilton (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 1–36.

Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A., and Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. Am. Psychol. 54, 462–479. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, A. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 17, 53–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117:497. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bendels, M. H., Müller, R., Brueggmann, D., and Groneberg, D. A. (2018). Gender disparities in high-quality research revealed by nature index journals. PLoS One 13:e0189136. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0189136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernard, R. E., and Cooperdock, E. H. (2018). No progress on diversity in 40 years. Nat. Geosci. 11, 292–295. doi: 10.1038/s41561-018-0116-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrand, M., and Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. Am. Econ. Rev. 94, 991–1013. doi: 10.3386/w9873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, L., Leslie, S. J., and Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests. Science 355, 389–391. doi: 10.1126/science.aah6524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., and Kowai-Bell, N. (2001). Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 253–267. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budden, A. E., Tregenza, T., Aarssen, L. W., Koricheva, J., Leimu, R., and Lortie, C. J. (2008). Double-blind review favours increased representation of female authors. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 4–6. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2007.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bzdok, D., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2020). The neurobiology of social distance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 24, 717–733. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J. T., and Cacioppo, S. (2014). Social relationships and health: the toxic effects of perceived social isolation. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 8, 58–72. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. J., Gabriel, B. A., and Terrell, C. (2002). The case for diversity in the health care workforce. Health Aff. 21, 90–102. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.5.90

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, C., Landivar, L. C., Ruppanner, L., and Scarborough, W. J. (2020). COVID-19 and the gender gap in work hours. Gend. Work. Organ. 28, 101–112. doi: 10.1111/gwao.12506

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croizet, J.-C., and Claire, T. (1998). Extending the concept of stereotype threat to social class: the intellectual underperformance of students from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Soc. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 24, 588–594. doi: 10.1177/0146167298246003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croizet, J. C., Després, G., Gauzins, M.-E., Huguet, P., Leyens, J.-P., and Méot, A. (2004). Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 721–731. doi: 10.1177/0146167204263961

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullen, M. J., Hardison, C. M., and Sackett, P. R. (2004). Using SAT-grade and ability-job performance relationships to test predictions derived from stereotype threat theory. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 220–230. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullen, M. J., Waters, S. D., and Sackett, P. R. (2006). Testing stereotype threat theory predictions for math-identified and non-math-identified students by gender. Hum. Perform. 19, 421–440. doi: 10.1207/s15327043hup1904_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., and Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Dev. 82, 766–779. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dar-Nimrod, I., and Heine, S. J. (2006). Exposure to scientific theories affects women’s math performance. Science 314, 435. doi: 10.1126/science.1131100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Désert, M., Préaux, M., and Jund, R. (2009). So young and already victims of stereotype threat: socio-economic status and performance of 6 to 9 years old children on Raven’s progressive matrices. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 24, 207–218. doi: 10.1007/BF03173012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dutt, K., Pfaff, D. L., Bernstein, A. F., Dillard, J. S., and Block, C. J. (2016). Gender differences in recommendation letters for postdoctoral fellowships in geoscience. Nat. Geosci. 9, 805–808. doi: 10.1038/ngeo2819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dworkin, J. D., Linn, K. A., Teich, E. G., Zurn, P., Shinohara, R. T., and Bassett, D. S. (2020). The extent and drivers of gender imbalance in neuroscience reference lists. Nat. Neurosci. 23, 918–926. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0658-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eaton, A. A., Saunders, J. F., Jacobson, R. K., and West, K. (2020). How gender and race stereotypes impact the advancement of scholars in STEM: professors’ biased evaluations of physics and biology post-doctoral candidates. Sex Roles 82, 127–141. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01052-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edge, L. (2020). Science has a racism problem. Cell 181, 1443–1444. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2020.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The neural bases of social pain: evidence for shared representations with physical pain. Psychosom. Med. 74, 126. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182464dd1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espin, J., Palmas, S., Carrasco-Rueda, F., Riemer, K., Allen, P. E., Berkebile, N., et al. (2017). A persistent lack of international representation on editorial boards in environmental biology. PLoS Biol. 15:e2002760. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2002760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Xu, J., Cuddy, A. C., and Glick, P. (1999). (dis) respecting versus (dis) liking: status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. J. Soc. Issues 55, 473–489. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gálvez, R. H., Tiffenberg, V., and Altszyler, E. (2019). Half a century of stereotyping associations between gender and intellectual ability in films. Sex Roles 81, 643–654. doi: 10.1007/s11199-019-01019-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaucher, D., Friesen, J., and Kay, A. C. (2011). Evidence that gendered wording in job advertisements exist and sustains gender inequality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 109–128. doi: 10.1037/a0022530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol. Rev. 102, 4–27. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., and Krieger, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: scientific foundations. Calif. Law Rev. 94, 945–967. doi: 10.2307/20439056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruenewald, T. L., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., and Fahey, J. L. (2004). Acute threat to the social self: shame, social self-esteem, and cortisol activity. Psychosom. Med. 66, 915–924. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143639.61693.ef

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofstra, B., Kulkarni, V. V., Galvez, S. M.-N., He, B., Jurafsky, D., and McFarland, D. A. (2020). The diversity–innovation paradox in science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 9284–9291. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1915378117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hong, L., and Page, S. E. (2004). Groups of diverse problem solvers can outperform groups of high-ability problem solvers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 16385–16389. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403723101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, C. S., and Gracia, J. N. (2014). Addressing health and health-care disparities: the role of a diverse workforce and the social determinants of health. Public Health Rep. 129, 57–61. doi: 10.1177/00333549141291S211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaxon, J., Lei, R. F., Shachnai, R., Chestnut, E. K., and Cimpian, A. (2019). The acquisition of gender stereotypes about intellectual ability: intersections with race. J. Soc. Issues 75, 1192–1215. doi: 10.1111/josi.12352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jimeno-Ingrum, D., Berdahl, J. L., and Lucero-Wagoner, B. (2009). Stereotypes of Latinos and whites: do they guide evaluations in diverse work groups? Cult. Divers. Ethn. Minor. Psychol. 15, 158. doi: 10.1037/a0015508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns, M., Schmader, T., and Martens, A. (2005). Knowing is half the battle: teaching stereotype threat as a means of improving women's math performance. Psychol. Sci. 16, 175–179. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00799.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, J. (2012). “Communications law: bits of bias,” in Implicit Racial Bias Across the Law I. eds. J. D. Levinson and R. J. Smith (Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press), 132–145.

Google Scholar

Kross, E., Berman, M. G., Mischel, W., Smith, E. E., and Wager, T. D. (2011). Social rejection shares somatosensory representations with physical pain. PNAS 108, 6270–6275. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1102693108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., and Kirschbaum, C. (2007). “Ten years of research with the trier social stress test—revisited” in Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior. eds. E. Harmon-Jones and P. Winkielman (London, New York: The Guilford Press), 56–83.

Google Scholar

Kuo, M. (2016). Recommendation letters reflect gender bias. Science doi: 10.1126/science.caredit.a1600139

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B., and Sugimoto, C. R. (2013). Bibliometrics: global gender disparities in science. Nat. News 504:211. doi: 10.1038/504211a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., and Handelsman, J. (2012). Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 16474–16479. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211286109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, K. R., Tham, W. Y., Yin, Y., Cohodes, N., Thursby, J. G., Thursby, M. C., et al. (2020). Unequal effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on scientists. Nat. Hum. Behav. 8, 880–883. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0921-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Science Foundation (2015). National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics. Science and Engineering Degrees, By Race/Ethnicity of Recipients: 2002–12. Arlington, VA: Author.

Google Scholar

Nielsen, M. W., Alegria, S., Börjeson, L., Etzkowitz, H., Falk-Krzesinski, H. J., Joshi, A., et al. (2017). Gender diversity leads to better science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 1740–1742. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1700616114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosek, B. A., et al. (2009). National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. PNAS 106, 10593–10597. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809921106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, E. O. (2010). Explicit evidence on the import of implicit attitudes: the IAT and immigration policy judgments. Polit. Behav. 32, 517–545. doi: 10.1007/s11109-010-9115-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pohlhaus, J. R., Jiang, H., Wagner, R. M., Schaffer, W. T., and Pinn, V. W. (2011). Sex differences in application, success, and funding rates for NIH extramural programs. Acad. Med. 86, 759–767. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0b013e31821836ff

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Régner, I., Thinus-Blanc, C., Netter, A., Schmader, T., and Huguet, P. (2019). Committees with implicit biases promote fewer women when they do not believe gender bias exists. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 1171–1179. doi: 10.1038/s41562-019-0686-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reuben, E., Sapienza, P., and Zingales, L. (2014). How stereotypes impair women’s careers in science. PNAS 111, 4403–4408. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314788111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riebel, K., Odom, K. J., Langmore, N. E., and Hall, M. L. (2019). New insights from female bird song: towards an integrated approach to studying male and female communication roles. Biol. Lett. 15:20190059. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2019.0059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rivera, L. A., and Tilcsik, A. (2019). Scaling down inequality: rating scales, gender bias, and the architecture of evaluation. Am. Soc. Rev. 84, 248–274. doi: 10.1177/0003122419833601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., and Beilock, S. L. (2009). Multiple social identities and stereotype threat: imbalance, accessibility, and working memory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 949–966. doi: 10.1037/a0014846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackett, P. R., Hardison, C. M., and Cullen, M. J. (2004). On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American–white differences on cognitive tests. Am. Psychol. 59, 7–13. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmader, T., and Johns, M. (2003). Converging evidence that stereotype threat reduces working memory capacity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 440–452. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, S. J., Logel, C., and Davies, P. G. (2016). Stereotype threat. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 415–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., and Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 35, 4–28. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staats, C., Capatosto, K., Wright, R. A., and Contractor, D. (2015). State of the Science: Implicit Bias Review 2015. Vol. 3. Columbus, OH: Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity.

Google Scholar

Staats, C., Capatosto, K., Wright, R., and Jackson, V. (2014). 2014 State of the Science: Implicit Bias Review. Columbus, OH: Kirwan Institute for the Study of Race and Ethnicity.

Google Scholar

Staniscuaski, F., Kmetzsch, L., Soletti, R. C., Reichert, F., Zandonà, E., Ludwig, Z. M. C., et al. (2021). Gender, race and parenthood impact academic productivity during the COVID-19 pandemic: from survey to action. Front. Psychol. 12:663252. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staniscuaski, F., Reichert, F., Werneck, F. P., de Oliveira, L., Mello-Carpes, P. B., Soletti, R. C., et al. (2020). Impact of COVID-19 on academic mothers. Science 368:724. doi: 10.1126/science.abc2740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steele, C. M., and Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 797–811. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storage, D., Horne, Z., Cimpian, A., and Leslie, S. J. (2016). The frequency of “brilliant” and “genius” in teaching evaluations predicts the representation of women and African Americans across fields. PLoS One 11:e0150194. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0150194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tilly, C. (1998). Durable Inequality. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 187–194. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Department of Education (2017). Table 318.45. Number and percentage distribution of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) degrees/certificates conferred by postsecondary institutions, by race/ethnicity, level of degree/certificate, and sex of student: 2008–09 through 2015–16. Available at: https://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d17/tables/dt17_318.45.asp (Accessed April 25, 2019).

Google Scholar

Valantine, H. A., and Collins, F. S. (2015). NIH addresses the science of diversity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 12240–12242. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515612112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Lee, R., and Ellemers, N. (2015). Gender contributes to personal research funding success in the Netherlands. PNAS 112, 12349–12353. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510159112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Dijk, D., Manor, O., and Carey, L. B. (2014). Publication metrics and success on the academic job market. Curr. Biol. 24, 516–517. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.04.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wenneras, C., and Wold, A. (1997). Nepotism and sexism in peer review. Nature 387, 341–343. doi: 10.1038/387341a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witteman, H. O., Hendricks, M., Straus, S., and Tannenbaum, C. (2019). Are gender gaps due to evaluations of the applicant or the science? A natural experiment at a national funding agency. Lancet 393, 531–540. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32611-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegert, J. C., and Hanges, P. J. (2005). Employment discrimination: the role of implicit attitudes, motivation, and a climate for racial bias. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 553–562. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.3.553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: implicit bias, stereotype threat, gender inequalities, diversity, underrepresented groups

Citation: Calaza KC, Erthal FS, Pereira MG, Macario KD, Daflon VT, David IA, Castro HC, Vargas MD, Martins LB, Stariolo JB, Volchan E and de Oliveira L (2021) Facing Racism and Sexism in Science by Fighting Against Social Implicit Bias: A Latina and Black Woman’s Perspective. Front. Psychol. 12:671481. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.671481

Received: 23 February 2021; Accepted: 10 June 2021;
Published: 16 July 2021.

Edited by:

Sarah Barnard, Loughborough University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

David Stuart Smith, Robert Gordon University, United Kingdom
Fernando Salinas-Quiroz, Tufts University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Calaza, Erthal, Pereira, Macario, Daflon, David, Castro, Vargas, Martins, Stariolo, Volchan and de Oliveira. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Leticia de Oliveira, oliveira_leticia@id.uff.br; Karin C. Calaza, kcalaza@id.uff.br; Eliane Volchan, evolchan@biof.ufrj.br

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.