Skip to main content
Log in

Continuity and Change in the Genetic and Environmental Etiology of Youth Antisocial Behavior

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Behavior Genetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 02 January 2024

This article has been updated

Abstract

Trajectories of youth antisocial behavior (ASB) are characterized by both continuity and change. Twin studies have further indicated that genetic factors underlie continuity, while environmental exposures unique to each child in a given family underlie change. However, most behavioral genetic studies have examined continuity and change during relatively brief windows of development (e.g., during childhood but not into adolescence). It is unclear whether these findings would persist when ASB trajectories are examined across multiple stages of early development (i.e., from early childhood into emerging adulthood). Our study sought to fill this gap by examining participants assessed up to five times between the ages of 3 and 22 years using an accelerated longitudinal design in the Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR). We specifically examined the etiologies of stability and change via growth curve modeling and a series of univariate and bivariate twin analyses. While participants exhibited moderate-to-high rank-order stability, mean levels of ASB decreased linearly with age. Genetic and nonshared environmental influences that were present in early childhood also contributed to both stability and change across development, while shared environmental contributions were negligible. In addition, genetic and nonshared environmental influences that were not yet present at the initial assessment contributed to change over time. Although ASB tended to decrease in frequency with age, participants who engaged in high levels of ASB during childhood generally continued to do so throughout development. Moreover, the genetic and nonshared environmental contributions to ASB early in development also shaped the magnitude of the decrease with age.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Code availability

Mplus code is available from the authors upon request.

Change history

References

  • Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA (2001) Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. University of Vermont Research Centre for Children, Youth and Families, Burlington

    Google Scholar 

  • Alink LR, Egeland B (2013) The roles of antisocial history and emerging adulthood developmental adaptation in predicting adult antisocial behavior. Aggress Behav 39(2):131–140

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 5th edn. American Psychiatric Association, Washington

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bartels M, Van den Oord EJCG, Hudziak JJ, Rietveld MJH, Van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI (2004) Genetic and environmental mechanisms underlying stability and change in problem behaviors at ages 3, 7, 10, and 12. Dev Psychol 40(5):852

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beauchaine TP, Webster-Stratton C, Reid MJ (2005) Mediators, moderators, and predictors of 1-year outcomes among children treated for early-onset conduct problems: a latent growth curve analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 73(3):371

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becker A, Woerner W, Hasselhorn M, Banaschewski T, Rothenberger A (2004) Validation of the parent and teacher SDQ in a clinical sample. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 13(2):ii11–ii16

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bongers IL, Koot HM, Van Der Ende J, Verhulst FC (2004) Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Dev 75(5):1523–1537

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner U (1988) Interacting systems in human development. Research paradigms: Present and future. Pers Context 2:25–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA (2009) Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: a meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychol Bull 135(4):608

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA (2012) How do we optimally conceptualize the heterogeneity within antisocial behavior? An argument for aggressive versus non-aggressive behavioral dimensions. Clin Psychol Rev 32(4):263–279

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Klump KL (2013) The Michigan state university twin registry (MSUTR): an update. Twin Res Hum Genet 16(1):344–350

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Klump KL (2019) The Michigan State University Twin Registry (MSUTR): 15 years of twin and family research. Twin Res Hum Genet 22(6):741

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, McGue M, Iacono WG, Krueger RF (2006) Differential parent-child relationships and adolescent externalizing symptoms: cross-lagged analyses within a monozygotic twin differences design. Dev Psychol 42(6):1289

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, McGue M, Carter LA, Iacono WG (2007) The different origins of stability and change in antisocial personality disorder symptoms. Psychol Med 37(1):27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, McGue M, Iacono WG (2009) Nonshared environmental mediation of the association between deviant peer affiliation and adolescent externalizing behaviors over time: results from a cross-lagged monozygotic twin differences design. Dev Psychol 45(6):1752

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Klahr AM, Klump KL (2015) Do non-shared environmental influences persist over time? An examination of days and minutes. Behav Genet 45(1):24–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Slawinski BL, Carsten EE, Harden KP, Hyde LW, Klump KL (2019) How should we understand the absence of sex differences in the genetic and environmental origins of antisocial behavior? Psychol Med 49(10):1600

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Clark DA, Gershoff ET, Klump KL, Hyde LW (2021) Twin differences in harsh parenting predict youth’s antisocial behavior. Psychol Sci 32(3):395–409

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Conger RD, Ge X, Elder GH Jr, Lorenz FO, Simons RL (1994) Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Dev 65(2):541–561

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eamon MK (2002) Poverty, parenting, peer, and neighborhood influences on young adolescent antisocial behavior. J Soc Serv Res 28(1):1–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eley TC, Lichtenstein P, Moffitt TE (2003) A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Dev Psychopathol 15(2):383–402

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enders CK, Bandalos DL (2001) The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct Equ Model 8(3):430–457

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganiban JM, Saudino KJ, Ulbricht J, Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D (2008) Stability and change in temperament during adolescence. J Pers Soc Psychol 95(1):222

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gard AM, Waller R, Shaw DS, Forbes EE, Hariri AR, Hyde LW (2017) The long reach of early adversity: parenting, stress, and neural pathways to antisocial behavior in adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 2(7):582–590

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner TW, Dishion TJ, Connell AM (2008) Adolescent self-regulation as resilience: resistance to antisocial behavior within the deviant peer context. J Abnorm Child Psychol 36(2):273–284

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith HH (1993) Nature–nurture issues in the behavioral genetic context: overcoming barriers to communication. In: Plomin R (ed) Nature, nurture, and psychology. American Psychological Association, Washington, pp 325–339

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman R (2001) Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Youthinmind, London

    Google Scholar 

  • He JP, Burstein M, Schmitz A, Merikangas KR (2013) The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): the factor structure and scale validation in US adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 41(4):583–595

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopwood CJ, Donnellan MB, Blonigen DM, Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono WG, Burt SA (2011) Genetic and environmental influences on personality trait stability and growth during the transition to adulthood: a three-wave longitudinal study. J Pers Soc Psychol 100(3):545

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hox J, Stoel RD (2014) Multilevel and SEM approaches to growth curve modeling. Wiley, Hoboken

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2002) Sex differences in the genetic and environmental influences on the development of antisocial behavior. Dev Psychopathol 14(2):395–416

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kind AJ, Buckingham WR (2018) Making neighborhood-disadvantage metrics accessible—the neighborhood atlas. N Engl J Med 378(26):2456

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kupersmidt JB, Griesler PC, DeRosier ME, Patterson CJ, Davis PW (1995) Childhood aggression and peer relations in the context of family and neighborhood factors. Child Dev 66(2):360–375

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lacourse E, Boivin M, Brendgen M, Petitclerc A, Girard A, Vitaro F, Tremblay RE (2014) A longitudinal twin study of physical aggression during early childhood: evidence for a developmentally dynamic genome. Psychol Med 44(12):2617

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey BB, Loeber R, Burke JD, Applegate B (2005) Predicting future antisocial personality disorder in males from a clinical assessment in childhood. J Consult Clin Psychol 73(3):389

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J (2000) The neighborhoods they live in: the effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychol Bull 126(2):309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacCallum RC (2009) Factor analysis. In: Millsap RE, Maydeu-Olivares A (eds) The SAGE handbook of quantitative methods in psychology. SAGE Publications Ltd, London, pp 123–147

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Martino SC, Ellickson PL, Klein DJ, McCaffrey D, Edelen MO (2008) Multiple trajectories of physical aggression among adolescent boys and girls. Aggress Behav 34(1):61–75

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGue M, Bouchard TJ (1984) Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behav Genet 14(4):325–343

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monahan KC, Steinberg L, Cauffman E (2009) Affiliation with antisocial peers, susceptibility to peer influence, and antisocial behavior during the transition to adulthood. Dev Psychol 45(6):1520

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Muris P, Meesters C, van den Berg F (2003) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12(1):1–8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén LK, Muthén B (2019) Mplus. The comprehensive modelling program for applied researchers: user’s guide 5

  • Neale MC, Cardon LR (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Odgers CL, Moffitt TE, Broadbent JM, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A (2008) Female and male antisocial trajectories: from childhood origins to adult outcomes. Dev Psychopathol 20(2):673

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters H, Van Gestel S, Vlietinck R, Derom C, Derom R (1998) Validation of a telephone zygosity questionnaire in twins of known zygosity. Behav Genet 28(3):159–163

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pingault JB, Rijsdijk F, Zheng Y, Plomin R, Viding E (2015) Developmentally dynamic genome: evidence of genetic influences on increases and decreases in conduct problems from early childhood to adolescence. Sci Rep 5(1):1–9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R, Daniels D (1987) Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behav Brain Sci 10(1):1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porsch RM, Middeldorp CM, Cherny SS, Krapohl E, Van Beijsterveldt CE, Loukola A, Bartels M (2016) Longitudinal heritability of childhood aggression. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 171(5):697–707

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhemtulla M, Hancock GR (2016) Planned missing data designs in educational psychology research. Educ Psychol 51(3–4):305–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer E, Gottesman II (1991) Individual differences and the canalization of human behavior. Dev Psychol 27:18–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuvblad C, Narusyte J, Grann M, Sarnecki J, Lichtenstein P (2011) The genetic and environmental etiology of antisocial behavior from childhood to emerging adulthood. Behav Genet 41(5):629–640

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Beijsterveldt CEM, Bartels M, Hudziak JJ, Boomsma DI (2003) Causes of stability of aggression from early childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal genetic analysis in Dutch twins. Behav Genet 33(5):591–605

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Windle M (2000) A latent growth curve model of delinquent activity among adolescents. Appl Dev Sci 4(4):193–207

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This project was supported by Grant Nos. R01-MH081813, UG3/UH3 MH114249, and UH3-MH114249-04S1 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and R01-HD066040, F31HD102094, and R01-HD093334 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIMH, NICHD, or the National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: SLC, SAB. Formal analysis: SLC, DAC. Funding acquisition: SAB, KLK, LWH, SLC. Writing – original draft: SLC. Writing – review and editing: SLC, DAC, LWH, KLK, SAB.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Alexandra Burt.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Sarah L. Carroll, D. Angus Clark, Luke W. Hyde, Kelly L. Klump, and S. Alexandra Burt declare no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

The described study has been approved by the Michigan State University IRB.

Consent to participate

Parents provided written consent for their children to participate (if under 18), and adult twins provided written consent for their own participation.

Human and animal rights and informed consent

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Participants were provided with information about the study, including their rights as participants. Parents provided informed consent, and children provided informed assent. This article does not include any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 39 KB)

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carroll, S.L., Clark, D.A., Hyde, L.W. et al. Continuity and Change in the Genetic and Environmental Etiology of Youth Antisocial Behavior. Behav Genet 51, 580–591 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10066-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10066-8

Keywords

Navigation