Skip to main content
Log in

Reading in multiple Arabics: effects of diglossia and orthography

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We tested the effects of diglossia and orthography on reading in Arabic, manipulating reading in Spoken Arabic (SA), using Arabizi, in which it is written using Latin letters on computers/phones, and the two forms of the conventional written form Modern Standard Arabic (MSA): vowelled (shallow) and unvowelled (deep). 77 skilled readers in 8th grade performed oral reading of single words and narrative and expository texts, and silent reading comprehension of both genres of text. Oral reading and comprehension revealed different patterns. Single words and texts were read faster and more accurately in unvoweled MSA, slowest and least accurately in vowelled MSA, and in-between in Arabizi. Comprehension was highest for vowelled MSA. Narrative texts were better than expository texts in Arabizi with the opposite pattern in MSA. The results suggest that frequency of type of texts and the way in which phonology is encoded affect skilled reading

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Gloss here is defined as one of the versions of Arabic that are used in different situations—thus being a part of ‘diglossia’. See Banerjee and Pederson (2003).

  2. In MSA, vowels represent morpho-syntactic information in addition to phonological information. However, the affixes that represent this information are deleted in SA. Thus, the similarity between Arabizi and vowelized MSA only occurs in the representation of phonological information that distinguishes between homographs—of which there are many.

  3. We thank Irit Meir for this insight.

References

  • Abdul-Mageed, M., Diab, M., & Korayem M. (2011). Subjectivity and sentiment analysis of Modern Standard Arabic. In Proceedings of the 49th annual meeting of the association for computational linguistics: Human Language Technologies (pp. 587–591). Portland, Oregon, USA, June. Association for Computational Linguistics.

  • Abou-Ghazaleh, A., Khateb, A., & Nevat, M. (2018). Lexical competition between spoken and literary Arabic: A new look into the neural basis of diglossia using fMRI. Neuroscience, 393, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Hamour, B., Al-Hmouz, H., & Kenana, M. (2013). The effect of short vowelization on curriculum-based measurement of reading fluency and comprehension in Arabic. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 18(2), 181–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. S. (1995). Different orthographies, different context effects: The effects of Arabic sentence context in skilled and poor readers. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 16, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1996). The role of vowels and context in reading of highly skilled native Arabic readers. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 25, 629–641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1999). The effect of Arabic vowels on the reading comprehension of second- and sixth-grade native Arab children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(1), 93–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2000). Effects of exposure to literary Arabic on reading comprehension in a diglossic situation. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 147–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2001). The role of vowels in reading Semitic scripts: Data from Arabic and Hebrew. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Siegel, L. (2003). Reading skills in three orthographies: The case of trilingual Arabic-Hebrew-Englishspeaking Arab children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 611–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M. (1990a). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alkawas, S. (2011). Textisms: The pragmatic evolution among students in Lebanon and its effect on English essay writing. A thesis Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Education. School of Arts and Sciences.

  • Authors. (2001). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of Arabic-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(4), 451–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2002). The Characteristics of Arabic orthography slows its processing. Neurosychology, 16, 322–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2004a). Morphological and orthographic effects on hemispheric processing of nonwords: A cross-linguistic comparison. Reading and Writing, 17, 961–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2004b). Orthography and the Hemispheres: Visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neurophysiology, 18(1), 174–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2005). Is literary Arabic a second language for native Arabic speakers?: Evidence from a semantic priming study. The Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(1), 51–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2007). Metalinguistic awareness and reading performance: A cross-language comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36(4), 297–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading off Arabic: Effects off orthography visual complexity on detection. Writ Syst Res, 3, 117–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2012). The contribution of the two hemispheres to lexical decision in different languages. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 8(3). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2014a). How does speed and accuracy in reading relate to reading comprehension in Arabic? Psicologica, 35, 251–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Authors. (2014b). Why is it Hard to Read Arabic? Handbook of Arabic Literacy, 9, 77–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Author & Aharon-Peretz, J., (2005). Is literary Arabic a second language for native Arab speakers?: Evidence from semantic priming study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34(1), 51–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ayari, S. (1996). Diglossia and illiteracy in the Arab world. Language, Culture and Curriculum, 9, 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azzam, R. (1989). Orthography and reading of the Arabic language. In P. G. Aaron & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and writing disorders in different orthographic systems (pp. 203–218). Springer, Dordrecht.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Azzam, R. (1993). The nature of Arabic reading and spelling errors of young children. Reading and Writing, 5, 355–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bashraheel, L. (2008). 9aba7 2l5air! Texting, Arab style. Arab News.(26 August 2008). [Online] Available at: http://www.arabnews.com/node/315362. Accessed 23 Apr 2016.

  • Beesley, K.R. (1998). Romanisation, Transcription and Transliteration, Xerox, viewed. Retrieved 15 May 2015 from https://open.xerox.com/Services/arabicmorphology/Pages/romanization

  • Bentin, S., & Frost, R. (1987). Processing lexical ambiguity and visual word recognition in a deep orthography. Memory & Cognition, 15, 13–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S. (1989). Electrophysiological studies of visual word perception, lexical organization and semantic processing: A tutorial review. Lang-Speech, 32, 205–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berkowitz, S. J., & Taylor, B. M. (1981). The effects of text type and familiarity on the nature of information recalled by readers. In M. Kamil (Ed.), Directions in reading: Research and instruction (pp. 157–161). National Reading Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Katzenberger, I. (2004). Form and function in introducing narrative and expository texts: A developmental perspective. Discourse Processes, 38(1), 57–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R., & Slobin, D. (Eds.). (1994). Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study. Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnsson, J. (2010). Egyptian Romanized Arabic: A Study of Selected Features from Communication Among Egyptian Youth on Facebook. MA Thesis, Univeristy of Oslo.

  • Boudelaa, S. (2014). Is the Arabic mental lexicon morpheme-based or stem-based? Implications for spoken and written word recognition. Handbook of Arabic literacy (pp. 31–54). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bourisly, A. K., Haynes, C., Bourisly, N., & Mody, M. (2013). Neural correlates of diacritics in Arabic: An fMRI study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 26(1), 195–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing accounts. Psychological Review, 100, 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwish, K. (2013). Arabizi detection and conversion to Arabic. Computation and language. https://arxiv.org/abs/1306.6755

  • Ehri, L. C., & McCormick, S. (1998). Phases of word learning: Implications for instruction with delayed and disabled readers. Reading and Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 14(2), 135–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy ability and phonological processing skills amongst dyslexic and non-dyslexic speakers of Arabic. Reading and Writing, 20, 273–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elmahdy, M., Gruhn, R., Abdennadher, S., & Minker, W. (2011). Rapid phonetic transcription using everyday life natural chat alphabet orthography for dialectal Arabic speech recognition. In The IEEE international conference on acoustics, speech, and signal processing (ICASSP) (pp. 4936–4939).

  • Englert, C. S., & Hiebert, E. (1984). Children’s developing awareness of text structures in expository materials. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 65–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enwall, J. (2001). Di- and trigraphia among the A-Hmao in Yunnan Province, China. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 150, 95–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Essawy, R. (2010). Arabic in Latin Script: Who is using it and why in the Egyptian Society. In E.A.-I.S. Dahan (Ed.), Global English: Issues of Language, Culture, and Identity in the Arab World. Peter Lang Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., Goldstein, Z., Iraqi, J., & Share, D. L. (1993). Effects of listening to story reading on aspects of literacy acquisition in a diglossic situation. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., Kita, B., & Goldstein, Z. (1986). Effects of listening to series stories on first graders’ comprehension and use of language. Research in the Teaching of English, 20, 339–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fender, M. (2008). Spelling knowledge and reading development: Insights from Arab ESL learners. Reading in a Foreign Language, 20(1), 19–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishman, J. (2001). Digraphia maintenance and loss among Eastern European Jews: Intertextual and interlingual print conventions in Ashkenazic linguistic culture since 1800. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 150, 27–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. Patterson, M. Coltheart, & J. Marshall (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1994). Prelexical and postlexical strategies in reading: Evidence from a deep and a shallow orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 20, 116–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1995). Phonological computation and missing vowels: Mapping lexical involvement in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 398–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., & Katz, L. (1989). Orthographic depth and the interaction of visual and auditory processing in word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 17, 302–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M. K., & Jenkins, J. R. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 239–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., Hoover, W. A., & Peterson, C. L. (1996). Some observations on a simple view of reading. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties (pp. 1–13). Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., & Louwerse, M. M. (2003). What do readers need to learn in order to process coherence relations in narrative and expository text? In A. P. Sweet & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension (pp. 82–98). Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A., Golding, J. M., & Long, D. L. (1991). Narrative representation and comprehension. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. II, pp. 171–205). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grivelet, S. (2001). Digraphia: Writing Systems and Society. In J. Fishman (Ed.), International Journal of the Sociology of Language. Mouton De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayadre, W. M., Kurzon, D., Peleg, O., & Zohar, E. (2015). Ambiguity resolution in lateralized Arabic. Reading and Writing, 28, 395–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R. F., Lane, H. B., & Pullen, P. C. (2005). Reading fluency assessment and instruction: What, Why, and how? The Reading Teacher, 58(8), 702–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, R.F., Mercer, C.D., & Lane, H. B. (2000). Exploring reading fluency: A paradigmatic overview, Unpublished manuscript, University of Florida, Gainesville.

  • Hussien, A. M. (2014). Reading Arabic shallow and deep genres in the fifth and tenth grades: Indispensible variables to science of reading. Journal of Education and Learning, 3(1), 60–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, M. H. (1983). Linguistic distance and literacy in Arabic. Journal of Pragmatics, 7(5), 507–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iraqi, J. (1990). Reading to Arabic-speaking kindergarten children compared to alternative enrichment activities as a means of improving listening comprehension and language skills. Unpublished Master's Thesis, Haifa, Israel: University of Haifa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iraqi, J., Feitelson, F., & Share, D. (1992). Effects of listening to story reading in aspects of literacy acquisition in a diglossic situation. Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 31–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated cognitive psychology of atypical reading. Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. Elsevier North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, R. (2001). The poinonous potency of script: Hindi and Urdu. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 150, 43–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., & Stahl, S. A. (2000). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial practices. Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. R., Schwanenflugel, P. J., & Meisinger, E. B. (2010). Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 45(2), 230–251. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.45.2.4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maamouri, M. (1998). Language Education and Human Development: Arabic diglossia and its impact on the quality of education in the Arabic region. Discussion paper prepared for the World Bank Mediterranean Development Forum, Marrakech. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania International Literacy Institute.

  • Maamouri, M., Bies, A., Kulick, S., Ciul, M., Habash, N. and Eskander, R. (2014). Developing an Egyptian Arabic Treebank: Impact of Dialectal Morphology on Annotation and Tool Development. In Proceedings of the ninth international conference on language resources and evaluation. Reykjavik, Iceland

  • Magner, T. (2001). Digraphia in the territories of the Croats and Serbs. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 150, 11–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masmoudi, A., Habash, N., Ellouze, M., Est, Y., & Belguith, L. H. (2016). Arabic transliteration of romanized tunisian dialect text: A preliminary investigation. In: Computational Linguistics and Intelligent Text Processing (Vol. 9041, pp. 608–619). Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

  • Mazzie, C. A. (1987). An experimental investigation of the determinants of implicitness in spoken and written discourse. Discourse Processes, 10, 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muhammed, R., Farrag, M., Elshamly, N. & Abdel-Ghaffar, N. (2011). Arabizi or Romanization: The dilemma of writing Arabic texts. Paper presented at Jīl Jadīd Conference, University of Texas at Austin, TX.

  • Peereman, R., Lété, B., & Sprenger-Charolles., L. . (2007). Manulex-infra: Distributional characteristics grapheme-phoneme mapping, and infralexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 593–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rao, C., Vaid, J., Srinivasan, N., & Chen, H. C. (2011). Orthographic characteristics speed Hindi word naming but slow Urdu naming: Evidence from Hindi/Urdu biliterates. Reading and Writing, 24(6), 679–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roman, G., & Pavard, B. (1987). A comparative study: How we read Arabic and French. In J. K. O’Regan & A. Levy-Schoen (Eds.), Eye movements: From physiology to cognition (pp. 431–440). North Holland Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 115–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of word and pseudowords in a diglossic context. Apply Psycholinguist, 25, 495–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing, 18, 559–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Henkin-Roitfarb, R. (2014). The structure of Arabic language and orthography. Handbook of Arabic literacy (pp. 3–28). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Spolsky, B. (2014). Acquiring literacy in a diglossic context: Problems and prospects. Handbook of Arabic literacy (pp. 225–240). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Schiff, R. (2016). The impact of diglossia on voweled and unvoweled word reading in Arabic: A developmental study from childhood to adolescence. Scientific Studies of Reading, 20(4), 311–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Taha, H. (2017). The role of morphological and phonological awareness in the early development of word spelling and reading in typically developing and disabled Arabic readers. Dyslexia, 23(4), 345–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samules, S. J., Schermer, N., & Reinking, D. (1992). Reading fluency: Techniques for making decoding automatic. In S. J. Samuels & A. E. Farstrup (Eds.), What research has to say about reading instruction (2nd ed., pp. 124–144). International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiff, R., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2017). When diglossia meets dyslexia: The effect of diglossia on voweled and unvoweled word reading among native Arabic-speaking dyslexic children. Reading and Writing, 30(5), 1089–1113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seraye, A. M. (2004). The Role of Short Vowels and Context in the Reading of Arabic, Comprehension, and Word Recognition of Highly Skilled Reader. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, Pittsburgh.

  • Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, I. Y. (1972). Misreading: A search for causes. In J. F. Kavanaugh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by eye and by ear (pp. 293–317). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. (1999). The role of vowel signs in Hebrew: Beyond word recognition. Reading and Writing, 11, 301–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. (2006). Reading Hebrew: The language and the psychology of reading it. Routledge.

  • Shoufan, A., Al-Ameri, S. (2015). Natural language processing for dialectical Arabic: A survey. In Proceedings of the second workshop on Arabic natural language processing (pp. 36–48). Beijing, China.

  • Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language and the challenge of reading for learning about science. Science, 328, 450–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taha, H., & Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2016). The role of phonological versus morphological skills in the development of Arabic spelling: An intervention study. Journal of psycholinguistic research, 45(3), 507–535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taouk, M., & Coltheart, M. (2004). The cognitive processes involved in learning to read in Arabic. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 27–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. M., & Beach, R. W. (1984). The effects of text structure instruction on middle-grade students’ comprehension and production of expository text. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 134–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, B. M., & Samuels, S. J. (1983). Children’s use of text structure in recall of expository material. American Educational Research Journal, 20, 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warschauer, M. (2001). Online communication. In R. Carter & D. Nunan (Eds.), The Cambridge guide to teaching English to speakers of other languages (pp. 207–212). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, C. A., & Kintsch, W. (1991). Expository text. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, 2 (pp. 230–245). Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, Y., Katzir, T., & Bitan, T. (2015). Many ways to read your vowels—Neural processing of diacritics and vowel letters in Hebrew. NeuroImage, 121, 10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werry, C. (1996). Linguistic and interactional features of Internet Relay Chat. In S. Herring (Ed.), Computer-mediated communication: Linguistic, social and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 47–63). John Beniamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Yaghan, M. (2008). ‘‘Arabizi”: A Contemporary Style of Arabic Slang. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Design Issues, 24(2), 39–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yates, S. J. (1996). Oral and written linguistic aspects of computer conferencing. In S. Herring (Ed.), Computer-mediated communication: Linguistic, social and cross-cultural perspectives (pp. 9–46). John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aula Khatteb Abu-Liel.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 22 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abu-Liel, A.K., Ibrahim, R. & Eviatar, Z. Reading in multiple Arabics: effects of diglossia and orthography. Read Writ 34, 2291–2316 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10143-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-021-10143-8

Keywords

Navigation