Back to Journals » Psychology Research and Behavior Management » Volume 14

The Complex Nature of School Violence: Attitudes Toward Aggression, Empathy and Involvement Profiles in Violence

Authors Martos Martínez Á, Molero Jurado MDM , Pérez-Fuentes MDC , Simón Márquez MDM , Barragán Martín AB, Gázquez Linares JJ

Received 1 April 2021

Accepted for publication 29 April 2021

Published 19 May 2021 Volume 2021:14 Pages 575—586

DOI https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S313831

Checked for plagiarism Yes

Review by Single anonymous peer review

Peer reviewer comments 2

Editor who approved publication: Dr Igor Elman



África Martos Martínez,1 María del Mar Molero Jurado,1 María del Carmen Pérez-Fuentes,1 María del Mar Simón Márquez,1 Ana Belén Barragán Martín,1 José Jesús Gázquez Linares1,2

1Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, 04120, Spain; 2Department of Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Providencia, 7500000, Chile

Correspondence: María del Mar Molero Jurado
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, 04120, Spain
Tel +34-950015598
Email [email protected]

Background: Aggressive behavior in adolescents has become a concern in education, where adapting to and going through high school may generate important behavior problems in adolescents.
Purpose: Analyze the relationships between parental and adolescent attitudes toward aggression and empathy. Identify profiles of direct and indirect involvement in school violence and determine differences between groups with respect to the components of empathy and attitudes toward aggression.
Methods: The sample was comprised of 1287 high school students who were administered the Beliefs about Aggression and Alternatives questionnaire, the Parental Support for Fighting and the Interpersonal Reactivity Index.
Results: The results show that beliefs in favor of the use of aggression in adolescents correlate positively with the perception of strong support from parents for aggression in response to conflict. Similarly, higher levels of support for the use of nonviolent strategies are positively related to the perception of strong support from parents. The relationships established with the components of empathy analyzed, both cognitive and emotional, were negatively correlated with favorable attitudes toward aggression. Results concerning the groups directly involved indicated that there were significant differences in the components of empathy between the groups. Furthermore, the multivariate analysis applied to the direct involvement groups showed significant differences between the groups in taking perspective. Between-group differences in empathic concern were also statistically significant for the group of active observers.
Conclusion: Taking perspective and empathic concern are moderating variables both for observers and victims and their parents in situations of violence.

Keywords: attitudes, aggression, empathy, school violence, adolescence

Introduction

In recent years, aggressive adolescent behavior has become a subject of interest in education, as the transition and adaptation to secondary education (in Spain, grade levels in this stage include 12-to-16-year-olds, and up to 18 if a year is repeated)1 can generate significant student behavior problems2 related to social,3 family and individual factors.4–6 Aggressive behavior involves violent conduct intended to physically or verbally harm or hurt others.7,8 Aggression may be an impulsive defensive instinct, motivated by anger and frustration, from a threat or conflict,9–12 or premeditated as part of planned attacks for the specific purpose of obtaining a reward as the result.13 There are different types of involvement in school violence within the framework of aggression, usually verbal in adolescents,14 in which one may have the role of victim, aggressor or observer. Adolescent victims of violence by other students usually have problems with interiorization, are introverted, and do not belong to a peer group. Youths who are the object of aggression may become emotionally distressed by the physical and psychological violence they are subjected to, as well as social exclusion and rejection by other students, increasing the probability of depressive symptomatology, suicidal ideation, stress, anxiety, substance use,15–18 and psychological adjustment problems.19 A victim-aggressor profile, in which the adolescent experiences episodes of violence, and in turn, attacks other students, instigating confrontation, has also been found.20

Another role in school violence is the aggressor, whose behavior may be associated with low levels of social and emotional competence,21 as well as lower empathy than shown by the victims.22 Often adolescents who inflict violence on others do so because they themselves are suffering or have suffered from abuse or early abandonment, or whose behavior is derived from living in a conflictive setting within the family, or where peers negatively influence their behavior.23,24

Youths who develop in a violent setting show more stable aggressive traits and belief in the use of aggression. This, along with patterns of upbringing marked by rejection, severity or overprotection can cause moral disconnection from their acts and lead them to stronger justification of the use of violence, or to attribute the reason for such behavior to factors outside themselves.25 As a result, many adolescent aggressors do not recognize having used violence on other students, and only a small percentage admit to such conduct, while a larger percentage of victims of school violence do recognize having been the subject of violence by another student.26,27 Moreover, according to the meta-analysis by Zych et al28 bullying peers is related to violent behavior in couple relations, and could be different manifestations of the same antisocial dispositions.

Observers, who view student aggression from different perspectives, may also be involved in violence, by either encouraging the aggressors to continue with their violence against other students or help and support the victims.29 In another position, observers do not want to become involved because they fear repercussions to themselves if they intervene to resolve the conflict.30–32 In this regard, a study by Bauman et al33 showed that youths who witness victimization of other students and do decide to intervene are against bullying and have more empathy. Cuevas and Marmolejo34 found the role of observer to be of special importance, because it contributes to violent episodes with intimidating behavior, or on the contrary, actions for stopping them.

Factors Involved in Violent Attitudes

Attitude toward violence and deviant behavior, has been shown to be a significant variable in predicting violent conduct of youths.35 One of the factors linked to the formation of attitudes toward violence in adolescents is the influence of the peer group.36,37 The presence of conflictive, violent students increases the possibility of others supporting and developing disruptive behavior.38 Youths who condone violent conduct often do so to gain acceptance and support from a group of conflictive peers.39,40

Nevertheless, high-quality friendships favor less violent behavior, and function as a socially and emotionally beneficial protective factor.41 Attachment relationships with a peer group that provides them with support and security37 are fundamental to diminishing violent behaviors in aggressive young people.42

Parents are another determining factor in developing attitudes favoring or opposing use of violence. The family usually influences adolescent behavior and beliefs including levels of acceptance of violence in both boys and girls and, thereby determining to a greater or lesser extent the likelihood of their developing violent behavior.43–46 Thus, parents who positively reinforce their children by giving them support and advice on how to handle the conflicts they must confront, promote alternatives to violence for solving their problems.47,48 In an environment of positive family relations, determined by attachment, inductive, open, positive and empathic communication, the possibility of adolescents having aggressive conduct diminishes.49–52

Sometimes aggressive adolescent behavior is associated with a conflictive parental relationship and inadequate parenting.53–55 Lack of family communication, absence of parental authority and low perception of parental support are associated with high levels of adolescent violence.45,56–58

Furthermore, parental approval of violence as a way of attaining goals proposed and frequent exposure to violence in the family setting cause adolescents to ignore the negative consequences of violence, normalizing aggressive action.59,60

The role of physical activity in violent behavior during adolescence should also be mentioned, having found a direct positive relationship between affective empathy and cognitive empathy, with differences between sedentary adolescents and those who usually practice some physical activity.61

Relationship Between Empathy and School Violence

In view of the repercussions that adolescent violent behavior could have, the individual factors that could lead young people to become a victim, aggressor or observer must be known.62

Among the individual factors, empathy has an essential role in violent conduct. This variable can be defined in terms of its two dimensions as cognitive empathy, or the ability to understand the mental states and feelings of others, and affective empathy, or being able to feel them.63 According to the study by Euler et al64 and Hartmann et al65 high empathy is related negatively to aggressive behaviors, and reduces unjustified violent behavior.66–68 In line with this, high levels of empathy also act as a protective factor against appearance of violent behavior and bullying, while low levels would be a risk agent.69–71 A high level of empathy also predicts parental and peer group support, which means an increase in the ability to resolve conflicts, reducing violent behavior.72 The study by Eisenberg et al73 further shows high levels of aggressiveness in youths to be related to low empathy derived from the poor relationship and conflict with parents and other students.74

A review of the literature on the subject demonstrated the increase in studies on violence in the school context, as well as the influence of empathy on violent conduct. Thus, this study sought to find out more about aggression and its link with empathy and the attitude of youths and their parents toward the use of violence.

The specific objectives of this study were to:

- Analyze the relationships between adolescent and parental attitudes toward aggression and empathy.

- Identify profiles of direct and indirect involvement in school violence.

- Determine whether there are between-group differences in the components of empathy and attitudes toward aggression.

Based on these objectives, the following hypotheses were posed: First, there is a positive association between favorable beliefs about the use of aggression and parental support for violence as a way to resolve conflicts. Thus, youths with more favorable beliefs about aggression would perceive that their parents support violence to a greater extent as a strategy in conflicts, while those with beliefs favoring the use of nonviolent strategies would be more empathic and perceive more parental support toward the use of pacific strategies (H1). We also expected to find different profiles with regard to direct or indirect involvement in school violence, where those who do not use violence toward other students and those who act when they are witness to this type of behavior would be those who would show the most empathy (H2).

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 1287 students at public high schools in the province of Almeria (Spain), aged 14 to 18 and with a mean age of 15.11 (SD=0.91), of whom 55% (n=707) were in 11th grade and 45% (577) in 12th grade, participated in this study. The sex distribution was 47.1% (n= 606) boys and 52.9% (n= 681) girls, with mean ages of 15.2 (SD=0.94) and 15.10 (SD= 0.88), respectively.

Instruments

Beliefs about Aggression and Alternatives:75 The questionnaire is comprised of a total of 12 items measuring two dimensions: adolescent beliefs about the use of aggression and approval of nonviolent response to hypothetical situations. There are seven items evaluating beliefs about aggression and another five on the use of nonviolent strategies rated on a four-point scale: 1 = Strongly agree, 2 = Agree somewhat, 3 = Disagree somewhat, 4 = Strongly disagree. The reliability found for beliefs about aggression was ω=0.787 and the GLB=0.795, and for nonviolent strategies it was ω=0.555, and GLB=0.602.

Parental Support for Fighting Scale:76 This is made up of 10 items which evaluate parental attitudes toward violence on two scales: strong support from parents for use of aggression in response to conflict, and strong support for use of nonviolent strategies by adolescents. There are five items with yes or no answer choices indicating strong parental support for aggression in response to conflict and five showing strong parental support for pacific response. Reliability found for the aggressive solution scale was ω= 0.633 and GLB= 0.689, and for non-aggressive solutions ω=0.653 and GLB= 0.676.

Interpersonal Reactivity Index.77,78 The Spanish version by Mestre et al79 was employed. This scale is comprised of a total of 28 items which measure four dimensions of global empathy: Perspective Taking, Fantasy, Empathic Concern and Personal Distress. This study used two of the four dimensions (Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern), as representative of cognitive and affective empathy. Perspective Taking (PT) refers to complex sociocognitive development enabling one to identify and understand another’s point of view. Within cognitive empathy, Perspective Taking further involves understanding the thoughts or beliefs of others.80 Reliability in this study was ω= 0.553 and GLB=0.648. Empathic Concern (EC), the emotional component of empathy, which is the ability of an individual to perceive and identify with feelings of concern and personal distress of those who experience complex harmful situations, had a reliability index of ω=0.542 and GLB=0.615. The items are rated on a five-point Likert-type scale (from 0 to 4), where 0=Does not describe me well, 1=Describes me a little, 2=Describes me well, 3=Describes me rather well, and 4=describes me very well.

Procedure

Before starting to collect data, the school principals were contacted and meetings were set up to inform them of the study objectives. They were also guaranteed confidentiality in data processing. Following the schedule of data collection sessions, two members of the research team went to the schools to implement the questionnaires. The validated tests were administered during the first half of 2019 in the usual classroom assigned to each group, in all cases, in the presence of their teacher/counselor. At the beginning of the session, the students were given the instructions for filling in the questionnaires, with time to solve any questions they might have about it, and they were assured that their answers would be anonymous, and therefore, their privacy would be respected in statistical data processing. The students filled out the tests individually in an estimated mean time of 25–30 minutes. In all cases, the ethical standards of research in the Declaration of Helsinki were complied with. Informed consent was obtained from parents/guardians and also from the participants themselves. The study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the University of Almeria (Ref: UALBIO2018/015).

Data Analysis

First the bivariate correlation matrix and descriptive statistics were calculated to establish relationships between variables. For interpretation of the magnitude of correlation coefficients, following Cohen,81 the absolute value of the coefficient was taken, regardless of the sign, such that: r < 0.10 null correlation, 0.10 ≤ r < 0.30 low, 0.30 ≤ r < 0.50 medium, 0.50 ≤ r < 1.00 large.

A two-stage cluster analysis was performed to identify the direct (victims and aggressors) and indirect (observers) involvement profiles, applying the “log-likelihood” option, which performs a probability distribution of the variables, to determine the distance or similarity between clusters. In particular, to identify the clusters or different profiles that we call “direct involvement profiles” (aggressors, victims) and “indirect involvement profiles” (observers), we used the answers to the questions: “Have you ever used violence against other students?” and, “Have you ever been the object of violence from other students?” And “Have you ever intervened when you saw someone using violence on other students?” for the indirect involvement clusters.

The “determine automatically” option was applied to determine the number of clusters. This procedure determines the “optimum” number of clusters automatically using Log-Likelihood, the criterion specified for clustering, in this case. This exploratory technique also provides data on the quality of the cluster, using the Silhouette Coefficient, which is a measure of the adequacy of cluster cohesion and separation, evaluating whether the structure resulting from forming the groups is: ≤0.25 insubstantial, 0.26–0.50 weak, 0.51–0.70 reasonable, o 0.71–1.00 strong.82 In this case, the mean silhouette was 1 (strong).

After classifying the cases, a MANOVA was performed to find between-group differences in the components of empathy (Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern). To estimate the effect size, the partial Eta squared was applied (ηp2), considering: <0.01 irrelevant, 0.01 small, 0.06 medium, and large from 0.14 on. To determine what profiles had significant differences, the post hoc Bonferroni comparison test was applied.

Results

Attitudes Toward Aggression, Perspective-Taking and Empathic Concern: Correlations

As shown in Table 1, favorable adolescent beliefs about the use of aggression (A_vs) correlated positively [r = 0.42, 95% CI (0.37, 0.46)], with a moderate association with the perception of strong support from parents for use of aggression in response to conflict (P_as). Similarly, stronger support for use of nonviolent strategies by adolescents (A_nvs) was positively related [r = 0.28, 95% CI (0.23, 0.33)] with the perception of strong support from parents for pacific solutions to conflict (P_nas), in this case, resulting in a weak association.

Table 1 Attitudes Toward Aggression, Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern. Pearson Correlation Matrix

The relationships established with both the cognitive (Perspective Taking, PT) and emotional (Empathic Concern, EC) components of empathy analyzed were negatively related to favorable adolescent attitudes toward aggression [PT: r = −0.28, 95% CI (−0.33, −0.23); EC: r = −0.30, 95% CI (−0.35, −0.24)], weak for Perspective Taking and moderate for Empathic Concern. Negative correlations were also found, although weak, with respect to the perception of parental support for aggressive solutions in response to conflict [PT: r = −0.19, 95% CI (−0.24, −0.13); EC: r = −0.14, 95% CI (−0.19, −0.08)].

Types of Involvement in School Violence

First, the following groups were identified by the possible combinations of direct involvement in school violence (Figure 1):

Figure 1 Direct involvement profiles (victims and aggressors).

Abbreviations: M, mean; SD, standard deviation.

Note: c1 = no direct involvement, c2 = victim-aggressor, c3 = victim, c4 = aggressor.

Cluster 1 (C1), labeled “no direct involvement”, was the most numerous of the groups and was made up of adolescents who said they had neither been assaulted nor used violence on other students. Cluster 2 (C2), labeled “victim-aggressor”, grouped those who responded affirmatively to both questions: “Have you ever been the object of violence from other students?” and “Have you ever used violence against other students?” Cluster 3 (C3), adolescents in the role of “victim”, was made up of those who said that they had been the object of violence from other students. Finally, Cluster 4 (C4) included adolescents who stated that they had used violence against other students, and this group had the role of “aggressor”.

Indirect involvement, based on the role of observer, was found by asking two questions: “Have you ever seen violence against other students?” which the subject only answered if he had witnessed an episode of school violence, and “Have you ever intervened when you saw someone using violence against other students?” where the answer was whether the participant had intervened or not.

The groups identified based on the participant answers to the two questions above were the following (Figure 2):

Figure 2 Indirect involvement profiles (Observers).

Abbreviations: M, mean; SD, standard deviation.

Notes: c1 = social desirability bias, c2 = active observer, c3 = non-observer, c4 = passive observer.

Cluster 1 (C1), where answers were incongruent, because the subjects stated that they had never witnessed episodes of school violence, and however, answered positively about their intervention in those cases. This group was characterized by the presence of incongruence or “social desirability bias”.

Cluster 2 (C2), the most numerous, was labeled “active observers”, and included adolescents who had intervened in episodes of school violence.

Cluster 3 (C3), labeled “non-observer”, included adolescents who said they had not witnessed episodes of school violence.

Finally, Cluster 4 (C4), called “passive observer”, included those who stated they had seen episodes of school violence, but had not intervened in them.

Differences in Empathy, by Involvement in School Violence

The MANOVA showed that there were significant differences between the direct involvement groups (Table 2) in the components of empathy (Wilk’s Λ = 0.963, F(6, 1222) = 7.76, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19). Equality between the covariance matrices was examined using Box’s M test (MBox = 9.80, F = 1.07, p = 0.377). The univariate analyses for each dependent variable revealed the existence of significant differences between the groups in Perspective Taking (F(3, 1222) = 10.96, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.26). Specifically, the group of “victims” scored significantly higher than the “no direct involvement” (pbonf < 0.001), “victim-aggressor” (pbonf < 0.01) and “aggressor” (pbonf < 0.001) profiles. Between-group differences in Empathic concern were statistically significant (F(3, 1222) = 11.28, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.27). In this case, statistically significant differences were observed in the “victims” group, who also had higher scores than the rest of the profiles: “no direct involvement” (pbonf < 0.001), “victim-aggressor” (pbonf < 0.01) and “aggressor” (pbonf < 0.001).

Table 2 Direct Involvement Profiles and Empathy. Descriptive Statistics

Furthermore, the multivariate analysis applied to the indirect involvement groups (Table 3) revealed significant differences (Wilk’s Λ = 0.966, F(6, 1222) = 7.20, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.19). Equality between covariance matrices was examined using the Box’s M test (MBox = 5.95, F = 0.66, p = 0.746). The univariate analyses found significant between-group differences in Perspective Taking (F(3, 1222) = 4.53, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.01), where the groups with the “social desirability bias” response and “active observer” scored highest. In particular, the “social desirability bias” group differed significantly from the “non-observer” (pbonf < 0.05) and “passive observer” (pbonf < 0.05) profiles. Similarly, the “active observer” group had scores significantly higher than the “non-observer” (pbonf < 0.05) and “passive observer” (pbonf < 0.05) profiles. In Empathic concern, there were also statistically significant between-group differences (F(3, 1222) = 13.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.03), where the highest mean score was in the group of “active observers”, showing statistically significant differences from the “non-observer” (pbonf < 0.001) and “passive observer” (pbonf < 0.001) profiles. The “social desirability bias” profile showed a significantly higher mean score than “passive observer” (pbonf < 0.05).

Table 3 Indirect Involvement Profiles and Empathy. Descriptive Statistics

Discussion

The results confirmed the first hypothesis of this study, since it was observed that parental approval of the use of violent strategies promotes its normalization, and therefore, supports use of this type of behavior by adolescents.60 While parental fostering of pacific solutions shows higher levels of empathy are related to less favorable attitudes of adolescents toward violence. Thus, parental promotion of strategies based on communication and socialization in the peer group,47,48,50 along with better understanding and adherence to the emotions of other students was related to support of adolescents for non-violent solutions to resolve conflicts.66–68

Furthermore, in regard to the second hypothesis of this study, the results on involvement in school violence analyzed showed the existence of different profiles within direct and indirect participation in school violence. Specifically, in direct involvement, four groups were found coinciding with what was suggested by Monteiro et al20 youths with no direct involvement, victim-aggressors, victims and aggressors. The first was made up of adolescents who said they had not used violence or been the object of school violence was the most numerous, followed by victims, aggressors and victim-aggressors, respectively. Thus, the percentage of adolescents who admitted to having used violence against other students was lower than the percentage of victims who said they had been subjected to violence, as shown in the study by Fisher et al.26

The clusters of the indirect involvement profiles, as observed, included a high percentage of active observers, characterized by helping victims of school violence.29 This was followed by the passive observers, who did not react to situations of abuse of other students due to their fear of the repercussions.30,32 And, finally, the lowest percentage was of “non-observers”, who said they had not witnessed episodes of school violence. In addition, a group with incongruent responses was observed, in which, motivated by social desirability, adolescents said they had never been witnesses of violence, but they had, however, intervened in defense of the victim.

Once the different groups of involvement in school violence had been defined, the results within the direct involvement groups showed the significantly highest scores in the group of victims on Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern. Likewise, after victims, those in the “no direct involvement” group had the highest scores on the two dimensions of empathy evaluated. This seems to show that an empathic attitude promotes interpersonal relationships, lessening the use of violent behavior.9

Finally, in the multivariate analysis applied to the indirect involvement groups, the results showed a direct relationship between the components of empathy and active involvement of the observer, both in Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern. Active observers are able to put themselves in the place of the victims and feel concern for them, and then take action to help them in situations of violence. Similar results were shown in the studies by Evans et al29 and Bauman et al33 where observers who intervened when other students were subjected to violence had high scores in empathic response to conflictive situations. In view of all of the above, it is fundamental to encourage adolescents to intervene in situations of school violence.

Based on the empirical results of this study, along with the theoretical review, we can say that both transmission of parental values and individual characteristics such as empathy, have an important role in the formation of favorable or unfavorable beliefs and attitudes on the use of violent strategies for solving interpersonal conflicts in adolescence. Empathy was shown to be another important variable in the involvement of youths in school violence. Thus, showing more empathy was related to not using violence against other students and defending those who are the object of such violence.

Limitations

This study had some limitations which should be borne in mind. In the first place, it should be emphasized that the responses on the point of view of the parents about violent behavior were provided by the adolescents themselves, and therefore, these results should be compared to parents’ opinions. In the second place, the study focused on two components of empathy, Perspective Taking and Empathic Concern, which could lead to bias if the results are generalized to the rest of the components of empathy. The difficulty for generalizing these findings outside of Spain or other geographic regions should also be mentioned, as the profiles for involvement in violence may not coincide with youths in other areas.

The results of this study suggest that in future research, the sample size be increased and preuniversity [bachillerato] students added to the sample. It would also be of interest in future work to include other variables, such as emotional intelligence and self-esteem in the analysis as factors related to violent behavior.

Strategies for intervention for reducing violent behavior in schools could be designed based on the results of this study.

Conclusion

This study inquired into the role that parental perspective and empathy have in support for violence by youths, and also the levels of involvement in school violence. The results showed that parental support for the use of nonviolent strategies for resolving conflicts, as well as higher levels of cognitive and affective empathy, are related to more favorable adolescent beliefs in pacific solutions. Students who did not use violence on their classmates, and who acted when in the presence of bullying of another student had higher levels of empathy.

There is therefore a need to train parents in alternative nonviolent conflict resolution techniques so that the education youths receive at home is based on peaceful socialization. Moreover, school violence prevention programs based on training in individual variables, such as empathy, could be options promoting identification of students with their peer group. Thus, violence toward other students would be reduced and action by those who witness this type of action encouraged, creating a network of support and protection of victims of school violence.

Acknowledgments

The present study was undertaken in collaboration with the Peer violence and alcohol and tobacco use in Secondary Education program: an augmented reality program for detection and intervention (Reference: EDU2017-88139-R), funded by the State Research Program, Development and Innovation Oriented to the Challenges of Society, within the framework of the State Plan for Scientific and Technical Research and Innovation, and co-financing with Structural Funds of the European Union.

Disclosure

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest for this work.

References

1. Spain. Royal decree 1105/2014, of 26 December, setting the basic curriculum for obligatory secondary education (middle school) and bachillerato (high school). Boletín Oficial del Estado; January 3, 2015. 169–546.

2. Beharie N, Scheidell JD, Quinn K, et al. Associations of adolescent exposure to severe violence with substance use from adolescence into adulthood: direct versus indirect exposures. Subst Use Misuse. 2019;54(2):191–202. doi:10.1080/10826084.2018.1495737

3. Zhu J, Yu C, Bao Z, et al. Deviant peer affiliation as an explanatory mechanism in the association between corporal punishment and physical aggression: a longitudinal study among Chinese adolescents. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2017;45(8):1537–1551. doi:10.1007/s10802-016-0259-0

4. Verhoef R, Alsem SC, Verhulp EE, De Castro BO. Hostile intent attribution and aggressive behavior in children revisited: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 2019;90(5):e525–e547. doi:10.1111/cdev.13255

5. Levkova I. Influence of parental messages on the aggressiveness of teenagers. J Educ Cult Soc. 2018;1(1):50–63. doi:10.15503/jecs20181.50.63

6. Tian Y, Yu C, Lin S, Lu J, Liu Y, Zhang W. Parental psychological control and adolescent aggressive behavior: deviant peer affiliation as a mediator and school connectedness as a moderator. Front Psychol. 2019;10:358. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00358

7. Blair RJ. The neurobiology of impulsive aggression. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2016;26(1):4–9. doi:10.1089/cap.2015.0088

8. Vicent M, Inglés CJ, Sanmartín R, Gonzálvez C, García-Fernández JM. Aggression profiles in the Spanish child population: differences in perfectionism, school refusal and affect. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:12. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00012

9. Denson TF, DeWall CN, Finkel EJ. Self-control and aggression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):20–25. doi:10.1177/0963721411429451

10. Girard LC, Tremblay RE, Nagin D, Côté SM. Development of aggression subtypes from childhood to adolescence: a group-based multi-trajectory modelling perspective. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2019;47(5):825–838. doi:10.1007/s10802-018-0488-5

11. Pérez-Fuentes MC, Molero MM, Carrión JJ, Mercader I, Gázquez JJ. Sensation-seeking and impulsivity as predictors of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1447. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01447

12. Wrangham RW. Two types of aggression in human evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115:245–253. doi:10.1073/pnas.1713611115

13. Babcock JC, Tharp ALT, Sharp C, Heppner W, Stanford MS. Similarities and differences in impulsive/premeditated and reactive/proactive bimodal classifications of aggression. Aggress Violent Behav. 2014;19(3):251–262. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2014.04.002

14. Espejo T, Chacón R, Zurita F, Castro M. Victimización en edad escolar desde la perspectiva de la actividad física. Sportis. Sports Sci J. 2016;2(3):379–389. doi:10.17979/sportis.2016.2.3.1729

15. Chan KL. Victimization and poly-victimization among school-aged Chinese adolescents: prevalence and associations with health. Prev Med. 2018;56(3–4):207–210. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.12.018

16. Rivers I, Noret N. Potential suicide ideation and its association with observing bullying at school. J Adolesc Health. 2013;53(1):S32–S36. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.10.279

17. Rodway C, Tham SG, Ibrahim S, et al. Suicide in children and young people in England: a consecutive case series. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):751–759. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30094-3

18. Romero-Abrio A, Martínez-Ferrer B, Sánchez-Sosa JC, Musitu G. A psychosocial analysis of relational aggression in Mexican adolescents. Psicothema. 2019;31(1):88–93. doi:10.7334/psicothema2018.151

19. Cava MJ, Buelga S, Musitu G, Murgui S. School violence between adolescents and their implications in the psychosocial adjustment: a Longitudinal Study. Rev Psicodidáctica. 2010;15:21–34.

20. Monteiro RP, Medeiros ED, Pimentel CE, Soares AK, Medeiros HA, Gouveia VV. Valores humanos e bullying: idade e sexo moderam essa relação? Tenden Psicol. 2017;25:1317–1328. doi:10.9788/tp2017.3-18en

21. Zych I, Beltrán-Catalán M, Ortega-Ruiz R, Llorent VJ. Social and emotional competencies in adolescents involved in different bullying and cyberbullying roles. Rev Psicodidact. 2018;23(2):86–93. doi:10.1016/j.psicoe.2017.12.001

22. Jolliffe D, Farrington DP, Roesch SC. Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? J Adolesc. 2011;34(1):59–71. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.001

23. Gázquez J, Pérez-Fuentes M, Carrión J, Luque de la Rosa A, Molero M. Interpersonal value profiles and analysis to adolescent behavior and social attitudes. Rev Psicodidac. 2015;20(2):321–337. doi:10.1387/RevPsicodidact.12978

24. Wilkins N, Myers L, Kuehl T, Bauman A, Hertz M. Connecting the dots: state health department approaches to addressing shared risk and protective factors across multiple forms of violence. J Public Health Manag Pract. 2018;24(Suppl 1):S32–S41. doi:10.1097/PHH.0000000000000669

25. Li J, Qian D, Gao X. Adolescent aggression and violent video games: the role of moral disengagement and parental rearing patterns. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;118:105370. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105370

26. Fisher BS, Coker AL, Garcia LS, Williams CL, Clear ER, Cook-Craig PG. Statewide estimates of stalking among high-school students in Kentucky: demographic profile and sex differences. Violence Against Women. 2014;20(10):1258–1279. doi:10.1177/1077801214551574

27. Pérez-Fuentes MC, Molero MM, Barragán AB, Gázquez JJ. Family functioning, emotional intelligence, and values: analysis of the relationship with aggressive behavior in adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(3):478. doi:10.3390/ijerph16030478

28. Zych I, Viejo C, Vila E, Farrington DP. School bullying and dating violence in adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2021;22(2):397–412. doi:10.1177/1524838019854460

29. Evans C, Smokowski PR, Rose RA, Mercado MC, Marshall KJ. Cumulative bullying experiences, adolescent behavioral and mental health, and academic achievement: an integrative model of perpetration, victimization, and bystander behavior. J Child Fam Stud. 2018;27. doi10.1007/s10826-018-1078-4

30. Hamby S, Weber MC, Grych J, Banyard V. What difference do bystanders make? The association of bystander involvement with victim outcomes in a community sample. Psychol Violence. 2016;6(1):91–102. doi:10.1037/a0039073

31. Hillis S, Mercy J, Amobi A, Kress H. Global prevalence of past-year violence against children: a systematic review and minimum estimates. Pediatrics. 2016;137(3):e20154079. doi:10.1542/peds.2015-4079

32. Liebst LS, Philpot R, Bernasco W, et al. Social relations and presence of others predict bystander intervention: evidence from violent incidents captured on CCTV. Aggress Behav. 2019;45(6):598–609. doi:10.1002/ab.21853

33. Bauman S, Yoon J, Iurino C, Hacckett L. Experiences of adolescent witnesses to peer victimization: the bystander effect. J Sch Psychol. 2020;80:1–14. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2020.03.002

34. Cuevas MC, Marmolejo MA. Observadores: un rol determinante en el acoso escolar. Pensam Psicol. 2016;14:89–102. doi:10.11144/Javerianacali.PPSI14-1.orda

35. Juárez P. Screening for violent tendencies in adolescents. In: Morelli V, editor. Adolescent Health Screening: An Update in the Age of Big Data. Elsevier; 2019:115–134.

36. Lynn K, Boswell C, Zheng J. Causes and consequences of social exclusion and peer rejection among children and adolescents. Rep Emot Behav Disord Youth. 2017;17(3):71–75.

37. Zulauf CA, Sokolovsky AW, Grabell AS, Olson SL. Early risk pathways to physical versus relational peer aggression: the interplay of externalizing behavior and corporal punishment varies by child sex. Aggress Behav. 2018;44(2):209–220. doi:10.1002/ab.21744

38. Powers CJ, Bierman KL; Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. . The multifaceted impact of peer relations on aggressive-disruptive behavior in early elementary school. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(6):1174–1186. doi:10.1037/a0028400

39. Henneberger AK, Coffman DL, Gest SD. The effect of having aggressive friends on aggressive behavior in childhood: using propensity scores to strengthen causal inference. Soc Dev. 2017;26(2):295–309. doi:10.1111/sode.12186

40. Levey E, Garandeau CF, Meeus W, Branje S. The longitudinal role of self-concept clarity and best friend delinquency in adolescent delinquent behavior. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(6):1068–1081. doi:10.1007/s10964-019-00997-1

41. Malti T, McDonald K, Rubin KH, Rose-Krasnor L, Booth-LaForce C. Developmental trajectories of peer-reported aggressive behavior: the role of friendship understanding, friendship quality, and friends’ aggressive behavior. Psychol Violence. 2015;5(4):402–410. doi:10.1037/a0039685

42. Cuadros O, Berger C. The protective role of friendship quality on the wellbeing of adolescents victimized by peers. J Youth Adolesc. 2016;45(9):1877–1888. doi:10.1007/s10964-016-0504-4

43. Caravita S, Blasio PD, Salmivalli C. Early adolescents’ participation in bullying: is ToM involved? J Early Adolesc. 2010;30(1):138–170. doi:10.1177/0272431609342983

44. Hidalgo MV, Jiménez L, López-Verdugo I, Lorence B, Sánchez J. Family education and support program for families at psychosocial risk: the role of implementation process. Psychosoc Interv. 2016;25(2):79–85. doi:10.1016/j.psi.2016.03.002

45. Moreno-Ruiz D, Estévez E, Jiménez TI, Murgui S. Parenting style and reactive and proactive adolescent violence: evidence from Spain. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(12):2634. doi:10.3390/ijerph15122634

46. Pérez-Martínez V, Sanz-Barbero B, Ferrer-Cascales R, et al. The role of social support in machismo and acceptance of violence among adolescents in Europe. lights violence baseline results. J Adolesc Health. 2020;In Press. doi10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.09.007

47. Labella MH, Ann S, Masten AS. Family influences in the development of aggression and violence. Curr Opin Psychol. 2018;19:11–16. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.028

48. Ozer EJ, Lavi I, Douglas L, Wolf JP. Protective factors for youth exposed to violence in their communities: a review of family, school, and community moderators. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2017;46(3):353–378. doi:10.1080/15374416.2015.1046178

49. Jiménez TI, Estévez E. School aggression in adolescence: examining the role of individual, family and school variables. Int J Clin Health Psychol. 2017;17(3):251–260. doi:10.1016/j.ijchp.2017.07.002

50. Lindstrom-Johnson SR, Finigan NM, Bradshaw CP, Haynie DL, Cheng TL. Examining the link between neighborhood context and parental messages to their adolescent children about violence. J Adolesc Health. 2011;49(1):58–63. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.10.014

51. Rodríguez-Fernández A, Antonio-Agirre I, Ramos-Díaz E, Rebolledo-Rebolledo L. The role of affect-communication and rule setting in perceived family support and school adjustment. Eur J Educ Psychol. 2020;13:5–18. doi:10.30552/ejep.v13i1.288

52. Tur-Porcar AM, Doménech A, Mestre V. Family linkages and social inclusion. Predictors of prosocial behavior in childhood. An Psicol Spain. 2018;34(2):340–348. doi:10.6018/analesps.34.2.308151

53. Gallego R, Novo M, Fariña F, Arce R. Child-to-parent violence and parent-to-child violence: a meta-analytic review. Eur J Psychol Appl Leg. 2019;11(2):51–59. doi:10.5093/ejpalc2019a4

54. Lo TW, Cheng C. Predicting effects of the self and contextual factors on violence: a comparison between school students and youth offenders in Macau. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15:258. doi:10.3390/ijerph15020258

55. Tsaousis I. The relationship of self-esteem to bullying perpetration and peer victimization among schoolchildren and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Aggress Violent Behav. 2016;31:186–199. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2016.09.005

56. Cutrín O, Maneiro L, Sobral J, Gómez-Fraguela JA. Longitudinal effects of parenting mediated by deviant peers on violent and non-violent antisocial behaviour and substance use in adolescence. Eur J Psychol Appl Leg. 2019;11(1):23–32. doi:10.5093/ejpalc2018a12

57. Jiménez TI, Estévez E, Velilla CM, Martín-Albo J, Martínez ML. Family communication and verbal child-to-parent violence among adolescents: the mediating role of perceived stress. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(22):4538. doi:10.3390/ijerph16224538

58. Suárez-Relinque C, Del Moral G, León-Moreno C, Callejas JE. Child-to-parent violence: which parenting style is more protective? A study with Spanish adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(8):1320. doi:10.3390/ijerph16081320

59. Meter DJ, Ehrenreich SE, Underwood MK. Relations between parent psychological control and parent and adolescent social aggression. J Child Fam Stud. 2019;28(1):140–151. doi:10.1007/s10826-018-1240-z

60. Su W, Mrug S, Windle M. Social cognitive and emotional mediators link violence exposure and parental nurturance to adolescent aggression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2010;39(6):814–824. doi:10.1080/15374416.2010.517163

61. Arufe-Giráldez V, Zurita-Ortega F, Padial-Ruz R, Castro-Sánchez M. Association between level of empathy, attitude towards physical education and victimization in adolescents: a multi-group structural equation analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(13):2360. doi:10.3390/ijerph16132360

62. Martínez-Monteagudo MC, Delgado B, García-Fernández JM, Rubio E. Cyberbullying, aggressiveness, and emotional intelligence in adolescence. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(24):5079. doi:10.3390/ijerph16245079

63. Jolliffe D, Farrington DP. Development and validation of the basic empathy scale. J Adolesc. 2006;29(4):589–611. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010

64. Euler F, Steinlin C, Stadler C. Distinct profiles of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents: associations with cognitive and affective empathy. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2017;11(1):1. doi:10.1186/s13034-016-0141-4

65. Hartmann T, Toz E, Brandon M. Just a game? Unjustified virtual violence produces guilt in empathetic players. Media Psychol. 2010;13(4):339–363. doi:10.1080/15213269.2010.524912

66. Álvarez-garcía D, Núñez J, García T, Barreiro-Collazo A. Individual, family, and community predictors of cyber-aggression among adolescents. Eur J Psychol Appl Leg. 2018;10:79–88. doi:10.5093/ejpalc2018a8

67. Espejo-Siles R, Zych I, Farrington DP, Llorent VJ. Moral disengagement, victimization, empathy, social and emotional competencies as predictors of violence in children and adolescents. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2020;118:105337. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105337

68. Vachon DD, Lynam DR. Fixing the problem with empathy: development and validation of the affective and cognitive measure of empathy. Assessment. 2016;23(2):1–15. doi:10.1177/1073191114567941

69. Richaud MC, Mesurado B. Las emociones positivas y la empatía como promotores de las conductas prosociales e inhibidores de las conductas agresivas [Positive emotions and empathy as promoters of prosocial behaviors and inhibitors of aggressive behaviors]. Acción Psicol. 2016;13(2):31–41. doi:10.5944/ap.13.2.17808. Galician.

70. Zych I, Gómez-Ortiz O, Touceda LF, Nasaescu E, Llorent VJ. Parental moral disengagement induction as a predictor of bullying and cyberbullying: mediation by children’s moral disengagement, moral emotions, and validation of a questionnaire. Child Indic Res. 2020;13(3):1065–1083. doi:10.1007/s12187-019-09670-2

71. Zych I, Ttofi MM, Farrington DP. Empathy and callous–unemotional traits in different bullying roles: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2019;20(1):3–21. doi:10.1177/1524838016683456

72. Van Lissa CJ, Hawk ST, Branje S, Koot HM, Meeus WHJ. Common and unique associations of adolescents’ affective and cognitive empathy development with conflict behavior towards parents. J Adolesc. 2016;47:60–70. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2015.12.005

73. Eisenberg N, Eggum ND, Di Giunta L. Empathy-related responding: associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Soc Issues Policy Rev. 2010;4(1):143–180. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x

74. Boele S, Van der Graaff J, de Wied M, Van der Valk IE, Crocetti E, Branje S. Linking parent-child and peer relationship quality to empathy in adolescence: a multilevel meta-analysis. J Youth Adolesc. 2019;48(6):1033–1055. doi:10.1007/s10964-019-00993-5

75. Farrell AD, Meyer AL, White KS. Evaluation of responding in peaceful and positive ways (RIPP): a school-based prevention program for reducing violence among urban adolescents. J Clin Child Psychol. 2001;30(4):451–463. doi:10.1207/S15374424JCCP3004_02

76. Orpinas P, Murray N, Kelder S. Parental influences on students’ aggressive behaviors and weapon carrying. Health Educ Behav. 1999;26(6):774–787. doi:10.1177/109019819902600603

77. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS. 1980;10:85.

78. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(1):113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

79. Mestre V, Frías MD, Samper P. La medida de la empatía: análisis del interpersonal reactivity index [The measure of empathy: analysis of the interpersonal reactivity index]. Psicothema. 2004;16:255–260. Italian.

80. Healey ML, Grossman M. Cognitive and affective perspective-taking: evidence for shared and dissociable anatomical substrates. Front Neurol. 2018;9:491. doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.00491

81. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

82. Kaufman L, Rousseeuw PJ. Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Interscience; 1990.

Creative Commons License © 2021 The Author(s). This work is published and licensed by Dove Medical Press Limited. The full terms of this license are available at https://www.dovepress.com/terms.php and incorporate the Creative Commons Attribution - Non Commercial (unported, v3.0) License. By accessing the work you hereby accept the Terms. Non-commercial uses of the work are permitted without any further permission from Dove Medical Press Limited, provided the work is properly attributed. For permission for commercial use of this work, please see paragraphs 4.2 and 5 of our Terms.