Skip to main content
Log in

Spatial and attentional aftereffects of virtual reality and relations to cybersickness

  • S.I. : VR and Cognitive Science
  • Published:
Virtual Reality Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Cybersickness describes the nausea and discomfort that frequently emerges upon exposure to a virtual reality (VR) environment. The extent to which cybersickness leads to temporary constraints in cognitive functioning after VR exposure is a critical aspect of evaluating the risk to human safety where VR tasks are used for workforce training. Here, we examined whether VR exposure results in deteriorated cognitive spatial ability and attention, and if this possible deterioration is related to cybersickness. A standardized cognitive test battery consisting of Corsi blocks task (CBT), Manikin spatial task (MST), and color trails test (CTT-A and -B) was administered before and after participants were exposed to virtual reality (VR group), or engaged in interactive board games (control group). The performance of participants in CBT remained unchanged from pre-test to post-test in both groups, while performance in MST improved in the control and remained stable in VR group. Response times in CTT-A remained stable in the VR group but reduced significantly in the control group. Regarding CTT-B, participants from both groups became significantly faster in post-test. We did not observe any significant sex differences, or effects of past VR experience, across measures of cognitive performance or cybersickness. Crucially, no significant correlations were found between cognitive performance changes and cybersickness scores in any cases. The results provide encouragement for the use of VR in professional settings, suggesting that VR and cybersickness may minimally limit subsequent cognitive processing. However, it will be crucial to further examine the aftereffects in other cognitive functions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

The data that support the findings of this study are freely available on the Open Science Framework: Varmaghani, S., Abbasi, Z., Weech, S., Rasti, J. (2020). Spatial and attentional aftereffects of virtual reality and relations to cybersickness. Retrieved from https://osf.io/eqdva/

Notes

  1. Models with normally distributed responses that incorporate random effects are called linear mixed-models (LMMs); they are a special case of Generalized LMMs (GLMM). Interestingly, GLMMs allow not only continuous, but also binary and count-dependent variables to be analyzed (McCulloch et al. 2008). Modern mixed models that use a model building approach rather than a hypothesis testing approach, are more flexible than classical analysis of variance (ANOVAs), allowing e.g., non-normal responses, unbalanced experimental designs, and more complex grouping structures. LMMs providing results with acceptable type I error rates (Baayen et al. 2008) and have advantages compared to the classic ANOVA, as they require fewer assumptions to be met.

References

  • Andersen RA (1997) Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 352(1360):1421–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, Bates DM (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J Mem Lang 59(4):390–412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball K, Pearson DG, Smith DT (2013) Oculomotor involvement in spatial working memory is task-specific. Cognition 129(2):439–446

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barra J, Bray A, Sahni V, Golding JF, Gresty MA (2006) Increasing cognitive load with increasing balance challenge: recipe for catastrophe. Exp Brain Res 174:734–745

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67(1):1–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson AJ, Gedye JL (1963) Logical processes in the resolution of orientational conflict. RAF Institute of Aviation Medicine Report 259, Farnborough, UK.

  • Biocca F, Levy MR (2013) Communication in the age of virtual reality. Routledge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bos JE (2015) Less sickness with more motion and/or mental distraction. J Vestib Res 25(1):23–33

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botella C, Serrano B, Baños R, Garcia-Palacios A (2015) Virtual reality exposure-based therapy for the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: a review of its efficacy, the adequacy of the treatment protocol, and its acceptability. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 11:2533–2545

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE et al (1998) A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21(4):761–773

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cruz-Neira C, Fernández M, Portalés C (2018) Virtual Reality and Games. Multimodal Technol Interact 2(1):8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Weerd P (2003) Attention, neural basis of. In: Nadel L (ed) Encyclopedia of cognitive science, vol 1. Nature Publishing Group, London, pp 238–246

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahlman J, Sjors A, Lindstrom J, Ledin T, Falkmer T (2009) Performance and autonomic responses during motion sickness. Hum Factors 51:56–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis S, Nesbitt K, Nalivaiko E (2014) A Systematic Review of Cybersickness. In: Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Interactive Entertainment (pp 1–9). ACM, Newcastle, NSW

  • Dennison M, D’Zmura M (2017) Cybersickness without the wobble: experimental results speak against postural instability theory. Appl Ergon 58:215–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennison M, Wisti AZ, D’Zmura M (2016) The use of physiological signals to predict cybersickness. Displays 44:42–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dingler T, Schmidt A, Machulla T (2017) Building cognition-aware systems: a mobile toolkit for extracting time-of-day fluctuations of cognitive performance. Proc ACM Interact Mobile Wear Ubiquitous Technol 1(3):47

    Google Scholar 

  • Düking P, Holmberg HC, Sperlich B (2018) The potential usefulness of virtual reality systems for athletes: a short SWOT analysis. Front Physiol 9:128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Englund CE, Reeves DL, Shingledecker CA, Thorne DR, Wilson KP, Hegge FW (1987) Unified Tri-Service Cognitive Performance Assessment Battery (UTC-PAB) I: Design and Specification of the Battery (No. NHRC-87–10). Naval Health Research Center San Diego, CA.

  • Fischer MH (2000) Probing spatial working memory with the corsi blocks task. Brain Cogn 45:143–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Getso MMA, Bakon KA (2017) Virtual reality in education: the future of learning. Int J Inf Syst Eng 5(2):30–39

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould JH, Glencross DJ (1990) Do children with a specific reading disability have a general serial ordering deficit? Neuropsychologia 28:271–278

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gresty MA, Golding JF, Le H, Nightingale K (2008) Cognitive impairment by spatial disorientation. Aviat Space Environ Med 79(2):105–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang-Pollock CL, Carr TH, Nigg JT (2002) Development of selective attention: perceptual load influences early versus late attentional selection in children and adults. Dev Psychol 38(3):363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal KS (1993) Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int J Aviat Psychol 3(3):203–220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kerr B, Condon SM, McDonald LA (1985) Cognitive spatial processing and the regulation of posture. J Exp Psychol 11(5):617–622

    Google Scholar 

  • Keshavarz B, Hecht H (2014) Pleasant music as a countermeasure against visually induced motion sickness. Appl Ergon 45(3):521–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keshavarz B, Riecke BE, Hettinger LJ, Campos JL (2015) Vection and visually induced motion sickness: how are they related? Front Psychol 6:472

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim H, Park J, Choi Y, Choe M (2018) Virtual reality sickness questionnaire (VRSQ): motion sickness measurement index in a virtual reality environment. Appl Ergon 69:66–73

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaViola JJ (2000) A discussion of cybersickness in virtual environments. ACM SIGCHI Bull 32(1):47–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine M, Stern R (2002) Spatial task performance, sex differences, and motion sickness susceptibility. Percept Mot Skills 95(2):425–431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW (2004) Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Linn MC, Petersen AC (1985) Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Dev 56(6):1479–1498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lo S, Andrews S (2015) To transform or not to transform: using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Front Psychol 6:1171

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarley JS, Mounts JR, Kramer AF (2004) Age-related differences in localized attentional interference. Psychol Aging 19(1):203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCauley M, and Sharkey T (1992) Cybersickness: Perception of self-motion in virtual environments. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ 1(3):311–318.

  • McCulloch P, Searle SR, Neuhaus JM (2008) Generalized, linear, and mixed models, 2nd edn. Wiley, New york

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Mitrushina M, Boone KB, Razani J, D’Elia LF (2005) Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstaedt JM, Wacker J, Stelling D (2019) VR aftereffect and the relation of cybersickness and cognitive performance. Virtual Reality 23:143–154

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monahan T, McArdle G, Bertolotto M (2008) Virtual reality for collaborative e-learning. Comput Educ 50(4):1339–1353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore T, Fallah M (2004) Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 91(1):152–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nalivaiko E, Davis S, Blackmore K, Vakulin A, Nesbitt K (2015) Cybersickness provoked by head-mounted display affects cutaneous vascular tone, heart rate and reaction time. Physiol Behav 151:583–590

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesbitt K, Davis S, Blackmore K, Nalivaiko E (2017) Correlating reaction time and nausea measures with traditional measures of cybersickness. Displays 48:1–8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nobre AC, Gitelman DR, Dias EC, Mesulam MM (2000) Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: overlapping neural systems. Neuroimage 11(3):210–216

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nooriafshar M, Williams R, Maraseni TN (2004) The use of virtual reality in education. In: Proceedings of the 7th American Society of Business and Behavioral Sciences International Conference (ASBBS 2004). American Society of Business and Behavioral Sciences (ASBBS). ISBN: 0-646-43717-8

  • Oman CM, Cullen KE (2014) Brainstem processing of vestibular sensory exafference: implications for motion sickness etiology. Exp Brain Res 232(8):2483–2492

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmisano S, Allison RS, Kim J (2020) Cybersickness in head-mounted displays is caused by differences in the user’s virtual and physical head pose. Front Virt Reality 1:587698

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan X, Hamilton AdC (2018) Why and how to use virtual reality to study human social interaction: the challenges of exploring a new research landscape. Br J Psychol 109:395–417

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2018) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.Rproject.org.

  • Raz A, Buhle J (2006) Typologies of attentional networks. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:367–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason JT (1978) Motion sickness adaptation: a neural mismatch model. J R Soc Med 71(11):819–829

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reason J, Brand J (1975) Motion sickness. Academic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebenitsch L, Owen C (2016) Review on cybersickness in applications and visual displays. Virtual Reality 20:101–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riccio GE, Stoffregen TA (1991) An ecological theory of motion sickness and postural instability. Ecol Psychol 3(3):195–240

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Vives MV, Slater M (2005) From presence to consciousness through virtual reality. Nat Rev Neurosci 6(4):332–339

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandi C (2013) Stress and cognition. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 4(3):245–261

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saredakis D, Szpak A, Birckhead B, Keage HA, Loetscher T (2019) Factors associated with virtual reality sickness in head-mounted displays: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psyarxiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/7u4hn

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sevinc V, Berkman MI (2019) Psychometric evaluation of Simulator Sickness Questionnaire and its variants as a measure of cybersickness in consumer virtual environments. Appl Ergon 82:102958

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafer DM, Carbonara CP, Korpi MF (2019) Factors affecting enjoyment of virtual reality games: a comparison involving consumer-grade virtual reality technology. Games Health J Res Develop Clin Appl 8(1):15–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smart LJ Jr, Stoffregen TA, Bardy BG (2002) Visually induced motion sickness predicted by postural instability. Hum Factors 44(3):451–465

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sohlberg MM, Mateer CA (1989) Introduction to cognitive rehabilitation: theory and practice. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoffregen TA, Smart LJ Jr (1998) Postural instability precedes motion sickness. Brain Res Bull 47(5):437–448

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith SP, Burd EL (2019) Response activation and inhibition after exposure to virtual reality. Array 3(4):100010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanney K, Kennedy R, Drexler J (1997) Cybersickness is not simulator sickness. Proc Human Factors Ergon Soc Annu Meet 41(2):1138–1142

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stróżak P, Francuz P, Lewkowicz R et al (2018) Selective attention and working memory under spatial disorientation in a flight simulator. Int J Aerosp Psychol 138:102398

    Google Scholar 

  • Szpak A, Michalski SC, Saredakis D, Chen CS, Loetscher T (2019) Beyond feeling sick: the visual and cognitive aftereffects of virtual reality. IEEE Access 7:130883–130892

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tombaugh TN (2004) Trail making test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 19(2):203–214

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ventura S, Banos R, Botella C (2018) Virtual and augmented reality: new frontiers for clinical psychology. In: Mohamudally N (ed) State of the Art Virtual Reality and Augmented Reality Knowhow. IntechOpen, London, pp 99–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang G, Suh A (2019) User adaptation to cybersickness in virtual reality: a qualitative study. In: Proceedings of the 27th European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS), Stockholm and Uppsala, Sweden.

  • Weech S, Kenny S, Barnett-Cowan M (2019) Presence and cybersickness in virtual reality are negatively related: a review. Front Psychol 10:158

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weech S, Moon J, Troje NF (2018) Influence of bone-conducted vibration on simulator sickness in virtual reality. PloS one 13(3):e0194137

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weech S, Varghese JP, Barnett-Cowan M (2018) Estimating the sensorimotor components of cybersickness. J Neurophysiol 120(5):2201–2217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yanaka H, Saito D, Uchiyama Y, Sadato N (2010) Neural substrates of phasic alertness: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Res 68:51–58

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yardley L, Gardner M, Bronstein A, Davies R, Buckwell D, Luxon L (2001) Interference between postural control and mental task performance in patients with vestibular disorder and healthy controls. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 71:48–52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yen Pik Sang FD, Golding JF, Gresty MA (2003) Suppression of sickness by controlled breathing during mildly nauseogenic motion. Aviat Space Environ Med 74(9):998–1002

    Google Scholar 

  • Young SD, Adelstein BD, Ellis SR (2006) Demand characteristics of a questionnaire used to assess motion sickness in a virtual environment. In: IEEE Virtual Reality Conference (VR 2006), Alexandria, VA, USA

  • Zhou C, Bryan CL, Wang E, Artan NS, Dong Z (2019) Cognitive distraction to improve cybersickness in virtual reality environment. In: The 16th International Conference on Mobile Ad-hoc and Sensor Systems (IEEE MASS 2019): The Sixth National Workshop for REU Research in Networking and Systems (2019 6th REU Research in N&S). Retrieved from http://par.nsf.gov/biblio/10128315

Download references

Funding

This research was supported by grants to Javad Rasti from University of Isfahan Center of Entertainment Industries (https://uicvgame.ui.ac.ir/). The industry sponsor had no influence in the design or execution of the current research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by Sina Varmaghani and Zahra Abbasi. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Sina Varmaghani and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Javad Rasti.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (PDF 723 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Varmaghani, S., Abbasi, Z., Weech, S. et al. Spatial and attentional aftereffects of virtual reality and relations to cybersickness. Virtual Reality 26, 659–668 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-021-00535-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-021-00535-0

Keywords

Navigation