Skip to main content
Log in

Harm Avoidance and Mobility During Middle Childhood and Adolescence among Hadza Foragers

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 01 March 2021

This article has been updated

Abstract

Cross-cultural sex differences in mobility and harm avoidance have been widely reported, often emphasizing fitness benefits of long-distance travel for males and high costs for females. Data emerging from adults in small-scale societies, however, are challenging the assumption that female mobility is restricted during reproduction. Such findings warrant further exploration of the ontogeny of mobility. Here, using a combination of machine-learning, mixed-effects linear regression, and GIS mapping, we analyze range size, daily distance traveled, and harm avoidance among Hadza foragers during middle childhood and adolescence. Distance traveled increased with age and, while male adolescents had the longest daily ranges, average daily distance traveled by each sex was similar. We found few age- or sex-related patterns in harm-avoidant responses and a high degree of individual variation. When queried on the same issues, children and their parents were often in alignment as to expectations pertaining to harm avoidance, and siblings tended to behave in similar ways. To the extent that sex differences in mobility did emerge, they were associated with ecological differences in physical threats associated with sex-specific foraging behaviors. Further, we found no strong association between harm avoidance and mobility. Young Hadza foragers of both sexes are highly mobile, regardless of how harm avoidant they are. Taken together, our findings indicate that the causal arrows between harm avoidance and mobility must be evaluated in ecologically specific frameworks where cultural expectations of juvenile mobility can be contextualized.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

De-identified data and code are available on Open Science Framework.

Change history

References

  • Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., & Waters, E. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apicella, C. L., Crittenden, A. N., & Tobolsky, V. A. (2017). Hunter-gatherer males are more risk-seeking than females, even in late childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 592–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1975). The contributions of the family to the development of competence in children. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 1(14), 12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1996). The discipline controversy revisited. Family Relations, 45(4), 405–414.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berbesque, J. C., Marlowe, F. W., & Crittenden, A. N. (2011). Sex differences in Hadza eating frequency by food type. American Journal of Human Biology, 23(3), 339–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B. M., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, A., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016). Eat first, share later: Hadza hunter-gatherer men consume more while foraging than in central places. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 281–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., & Bird, B. R. (2002). Children on the reef. Human Nature, 13(2), 269–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N. (2016). Demography and evolutionary ecology of Hadza hunter-gatherers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolker, B. M. (2015). Linear and generalized linear mixed models. In G. A. Fox, S. Negrete-Yankelevich, V. J. Sosa (Eds.), Ecological statistics: Contemporary theory and application (pp. 309–333). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Bolker, B. M., Brooks, M. E., Clark, C. J., Geange, S. W., Poulsen, J. R., Stevens, M. H. H., & White, J. S. S. (2009). Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 24(3), 127–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, vol. 1: Attachment. Random House: Random House.

  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N. (2016). To share or not to share? Social processes of learning to share food among Hadza hunter-gatherer children. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 61–70). Tokyo: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Sorrentino, J., Moonie, S. A., Peterson, M., Mabulla, A., & Ungar, P. S. (2017). Oral health in transition: The Hadza foragers of Tanzania. PLoS One, 12(3), e0172197.

  • Crittenden, A. N., & Zes, D. A. (2015). Food sharing among Hadza hunter-gatherer children. PLoS One, 10(7), e0131996.

  • Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2019). Spatial cognition, navigation, and mobility among children in a forager-horticulturalist population, the Tsimané of Bolivia. Cognitive Development, 52, 100800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2020). You don’t have to know where your kids are, just where they aren’t: Exploring free-range parenting in the Bolivian Amazon. In B. K. Ashdown, A. N. Faherty (Eds.), Parents and caregivers across cultures (pp. 59–74). Berlin: Springer.

  • Devlin, A. S., & Bernstein, J. (1995). Interactive wayfinding: Use of cues by men and women. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(1), 23–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Draper, P. (1976). Social and economic constraints on child life among the !Kung. Anthropology Faculty Publications 13. In R. B. Lee, I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Accessed at https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/anthropologyfacpub/13/.

  • Ecuyer-Dab, I., & Robert, M. (2004). Have sex differences in spatial ability evolved from male competition for mating and female concern for survival? Cognition, 91(3), 221–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., Glätzle-Rützler, D., & Sutter, M. (2013). The development of egalitarianism, altruism, spite and parochialism in childhood and adolescence. European Economic Review, 64, 369–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Froehle, A. W., Wells, G. K., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, A. Z., Lew-Levy, S., & Crittenden, A. N. (2019). Physical activity and time budgets of Hadza forager children: Implications for self-provisioning and the ontogeny of the sexual division of labor. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(1), e23209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (2009). Methods and measures: The network of relationships inventory: Behavioral systems version. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 470–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagnon, K. T., Cashdan, E. A., Stefanucci, J. K., & Creem-Regehr, S. H. (2016). Sex differences in exploration behavior and the relationship to harm avoidance. Human Nature, 27(1), 82–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaulin, S. J. (1992). Evolution of sex difference in spatial ability. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 35(S15), 125–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1995). Sexual selection and sex differences in spatial cognition. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(4), 289–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, A., & Hill, J. (2006). Data analysis using regression and multilevel/hierarchical models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, A. (2018). Farmers, tourists, and cattle threaten to wipe out some of the world’s last hunter-gatherers. Science. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau2032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenacre, M. (2006). From simple to multiple correspondence analysis. In M. Greenacre & J. Blasius (Eds.), Multiple correspondence analysis and related methods (pp. 41–76). New York: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenacre, M. (2013). Contribution biplots. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 22(1), 107–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenacre, M. (2017). Correspondence analysis in practice. New York: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R., & Friedman, J. (2009). The elements of statistical learning: Data mining, inference, and prediction. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, F., & Jones, N. B. (1995). Hadza children’s foraging: juvenile dependency, social arrangements, and mobility among hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology, 36(4), 688–700.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herlosky, K. N., Benyshek, D. C., Mabulla, I. A., Pollom, T. R., & Crittenden, A. N. (2020). Postpartum maternal mood among Hadza foragers of Tanzania: A mixed methods approach. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 44, 305–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.). (2005). Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives. Abingdon: Routledge.

  • Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter–gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1168–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2013). An introduction to statistical learning. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jodoin, S. (2017). Forest preservation in a changing climate: REDD + and indigenous and community rights in Indonesia and Tanzania. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Khattak, A. J., & De Palma, A. (1997). The impact of adverse weather conditions on the propensity to change travel decisions: a survey of Brussels commuters. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 31(3), 181–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M. (2008). Building predictive models in R using the caret package. Journal of Statistical Software, 28(5), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, M., & Johnson, K. (2013). Applied predictive modeling. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2016). Playing with knives: The socialization of self-initiated learners. Child Development, 87(3), 654–665.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F., & Grove, M. A. (2011). Getting noticed. Human Nature, 22(3), 281–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawton, C. A., & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: A cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles, 47(9–10), 389–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Crittenden, A. N., Boyette, A. H., Mabulla, I. A., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2019). Inter-and intra-cultural variation in learning-through-participation among Hadza and BaYaka forager children and adolescents from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 29(4), 309–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Kissler, S. M., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Hewlett, B. S. (2020). Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka hunter-gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(1), 12–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Human Nature, 28(4), 367–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, D. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (1999). Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Current Anthropology, 40(4), 501–505.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W. (2003). A critical period for provisioning by Hadza men: Implications for pair bonding. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(3), 217–229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marlowe, F. W., Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B., Crittenden, A., Porter, C., & Mabulla, A. (2014). Honey, Hadza, hunter-gatherers, and human evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 71, 119–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, M. H. (1987). Gender, home range and environmental cognition. Transactions of the Institute of British Geographers, 12(1), 43–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehan, C. L., & Crittenden, A. N. (Eds.). (2016). Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications. Santa Fe: School for Advanced Research and University of New Mexico Press.

  • Miner, E. J., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., & Gaulin, S. J. (2014). Sex difference in travel is concentrated in adolescence and tracks reproductive interests. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1796), 1476.

  • Moshi, H. S., Makundi, A., & Reguly, B. M. (2019). Land degradation and its impacts on cultural tourism among the indigenous peoples in Tanzania: A case of the Hadzabe. Paper presented at the International Conference on the Future of Tourism (ICFT). Open University of Tanzania. http://repository.out.ac.tz/2487/1/Henry%20Moshi.pdf.

  • Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1971). Effect of environmental experience on spatial ability in an East African society. The Journal of Social Psychology, 83(1), 15–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nenadic, O., & Greenacre, M. (2007). Correspondence analysis in R, with two-and three-dimensional graphics: The ca package. Journal of Statistical Software, 20(3), 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollom, T. R., Herlosky, K. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Crittenden, A. N. (2020a). Changes in juvenile foraging behavior among the Hadza of Tanzania during early transition to a mixed-subsistence economy. Human Nature, 31, 123–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollom, T. R., Cross, C. L., Herlosky, K. N., Ford, E., & Crittenden, A. N. (2020b). Effects of a mixed-subsistence diet on the growth of Hadza children. American Journal of Human Biology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Austria: Vienna. https://www.R-project.org/.

  • Reiches, M. W. (2019). Adolescence as a biocultural life history transition. Annual Review of Anthropology, 48, 151–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlegel, A., & Barry, H. I. I. I. (1991). Adolescence: An anthropological inquiry. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. F., & Hampson, E. (1997). Evolution and the hormonal control of sexually-dimorphic spatial abilities in humans. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(2), 50–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, B., & Young, A. G. (2005). Growing up Mikea. In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental, and cultural perspectives (pp. 147–171). Piscataway: Transaction.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vashro, L., & Cashdan, E. (2015). Spatial cognition, mobility, and reproductive success in northwestern Namibia. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(2), 123–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vashro, L., Padilla, L., & Cashdan, E. (2016). Sex differences in mobility and spatial cognition. Human Nature, 27(1), 16–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, B., & Edwards, C. P. (1973). A cross-cultural analysis of sex differences in the behavior of children aged three through 11. The Journal of Social Psychology, 91(2), 171–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. M., Harris, J., Raichlen, D., Pontzer, H., Sayre, K., Sancillo, A., Berbesque, C., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, A., McElreath, R., Cashdan, E., & Jones, J. H. (2021). Gendered and landscape use in a Hadza hunter-gatherers. Nature Human Behaviour. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41562-020-01002-7.

  • Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., & Elphick, C. S. (2010). A protocol for data exploration to avoid common statistical problems. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 1(1), 3–14.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the Hadza families who participated in this study. We would also like to thank Helen Elizabeth Davis for helpful feedback on early drafts of this manuscript and Phoebe McNeally from the DIGIT Lab at the University of Utah for research support and analytical assistance. This research was funded by grant #162858 from the National Science Foundation (to E. Cashdan and A. Crittenden).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ANC and AF contributed equally to this paper. EC and ANC acquired research funding, designed the study, and prepared data collection materials. Data were collected by KNH, TRP, and IAM. Analyses were performed by AF, ANC, KNH, and ITG. The first draft of the manuscript was written by ANC and AF; all authors commented on subsequent versions of the manuscript; all authors read and approved of the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alyssa N. Crittenden.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were done so in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional Review Board in the Office of Research Integrity at the University of Nevada, Las Vegas (protocol # 1048457-1), the Tanzanian Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH), and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original online version of this article was revised: An author’s initials were transposed in the authors’ note.

Supplementary Information

ESM 1

(PDF 316 KB)

Appendix

Appendix

Table 5 Interview questions asked of children and juveniles not included in Table 1

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Crittenden, A.N., Farahani, A., Herlosky, K.N. et al. Harm Avoidance and Mobility During Middle Childhood and Adolescence among Hadza Foragers. Hum Nat 32, 150–176 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09390-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09390-z

Keywords

Navigation