Introduction

In 2019, the American Dialect Society voted for they as the word of the decade and (My) Pronouns as the word of the year. This vote recognizes two related linguistic trends. First, the singular use of they is growing in acceptability, in particular as the pronoun of choice for individuals who identify as gender nonbinary. Second, there is a movement to use pronouns to signal gender identity, as in my pronouns are they/them/theirs. This means that the speaker wishes for other people to refer to them with the singular pronoun they.

For language comprehension, this makes the word they ambiguous. Consider a story about Alex (who uses they/them pronouns) and Liz (who uses she/her pronouns): Alex went running with Liz. They fell down. Did both fall down, or just Alex? Linguistic ambiguity is not unusual, and the human mind is well adapted to interpreting ambiguous words in context. Yet few ambiguities are accompanied by explicit commentary. Singular they offers a unique opportunity to examine how a dispreferred meaning can become more available when the speaker offers explicit guidance, and how this affects an ongoing linguistic change. Our study uses three experiments to test the impact of explicit pronoun introduction and linguistic context on the interpretation of they.

The status of singular they

Contrary to public opinion, the word they has had a singular interpretation for centuries (Baron, 2020; McWhorter, 2018). In many English dialects, singular they is common for referring to people with unknown gender, such as someone or my future neighbor, and this usage has occurred in literature as far back as the middle ages (Nunberg, 2016). Singular they can be used to maintain anonymity of the referent, as in this excerpt from a newspaper article: “A Wake County resident who was at The Millennium Tour 2020 concert . . . has tested positive for coronavirus . . . . They were there between 8:20 p.m. and midnight” (Aldridge & Jasper, 2020). Camilliere et al. (2019) found that people rated they as more natural for referents that were ungendered or ambiguous (the dentist; Taylor) versus gendered (the actress; Sophia). People also rated they as more natural for referents that were socially distant (the dentist) than socially close (my friend).

By contrast, a relatively new use of they is for reference to known and singular referents (Bjorkman, 2017; Konnelly & Cowper, 2020). Most notably, individuals who identify as nonbinary often prefer to be referred to with they, although some prefer neopronouns like xe (Bradley et al., 2019; Fisher, 2018). The range of singular they also appears to be expanding and can even be used when the referent is known and identifies with a binary gender. For example, the first author observed a Facebook post that said “my spouse . . . they,” even though the spouse does not typically use they/them pronouns.

This expansion of singular they is paralleled by the movement to use pronouns as a signal of gender. This raises a question: Does pronoun introduction change the way they is understood? Most linguistic meaning is induced from the context, and not discussed explicitly. Indeed, the singular meaning of they can be obvious from the context. What role does explicit introduction play, over and above observational evidence?

Research question: What drives the interpretation of “they”?

Even though singular they is common, the plural use of they is more common. Thus, singular and plural interpretations likely compete. Current psycholinguistic accounts suggest that comprehenders weigh alternate referents for a pronoun, probabilistically activating likely referents. Activation considers both the pronoun features (e.g., gender and number) and the discourse context, which makes some referents more available than others (e.g., Arnold et al., 2000; Garnham et al., 1992; Kehler & Rohde, 2013; MacDonald & MacWhinney, 1990; McDonald & MacWhinney, 1995; Sanford & Garrod, 1989).

Two discourse properties that affect pronoun comprehension are the number of competing referents and order of mention (e.g., Arnold et al., 2000; Gernsbacher & Hargreaves, 1988; Järvikivi et al., 2005). If only one person is mentioned, that person is the likely pronoun referent (e.g., Liz in Liz went to the store. She bought milk). If two people are mentioned, the first one (the grammatical subject) is often assumed to be the referent (e.g., Liz in Liz went to the store with Ana. She bought milk). Our experiments test how they is understood in different discourse contexts within stories about three fictional characters: Alex (they), Liz (she), and Will (he).

In English, people understand pronouns by rapidly applying gender features on the pronouns (e.g., female for she and male for he; e.g., Arnold et al., 2000; Boland et al., 1998; Ehrlich, 1980; MacDonald & MacWhinney, 1990; Osterhout & Mobley, 1995). People also pay attention to gender—or the lack of it—when interpreting they. For an ungendered referent (e.g., anyone; a runner), singular they is understood as fast as he and she (Foertsch & Gernsbacher, 1997). Comprehension of singular they/them is also faster if the referent does not have an assumed gender (the cyclist) than if it does (the mechanic; Doherty & Conklin, 2017). Similarly, in event-related potential studies, singular they or themselves can elicit a P600, which is associated with syntactic anomalies (Leventhal et al., 2020; Prasad & Morris, 2020). For example, Leventhal et al. (2020) found a P600 for Lillian . . . they compared with Lillian and Paul . . . they. These studies suggest that singular they is understood easily when the referent is nonspecific or gender neutral, but not when a male or female gender is assumed.

On the other hand, they is easier to understand with a plural than a singular interpretation (Sanford & Filik, 2007), suggesting that the plural use is primary. In particular, nonbinary they is relatively new, and facility with this form depends on personal experience. Konnelly and Cowper (2020) suggest that singular they is a change in progress, is accepted by some social groups more than others (see also Camilliere et al., 2019).

We aim to better understand this change by measuring variation in the assignment of a singular interpretation to they. We use three experiments to capture metalinguistic judgments about the referent of they in stories about Alex, who uses they/them pronouns. We examine pronoun comprehension in a natural context, where stories refer to known characters. To this end, all stories are about the same three fictional characters, introduced with pictures at the start of the task. The pictures help participants to develop a mental representation of the character. We manipulate (1) whether Alex’s pronouns are introduced explicitly or not, and (2) whether the discourse context supports Alex as the referent, manipulating either number of people (Experiment 1) or whether Alex is mentioned first (Experiments 2–3). Note that here we use the terms explicit and implicit to denote properties of the stimuli; it is possible (and indeed likely) that participants were explicitly aware of Alex’s gender in both conditions. A second question is whether additional practice with singular they can change interpretation patterns within a single experimental session, which we manipulate in Experiments 2 and 3. The current paper focuses on the manipulated variables, but analyses of how individual differences pattern with responses are reported in a companion paper (Arnold et al., 2020).

Methods for all three experiments

Participants

Native English-speaking participants were recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk and compensated $1.00 (Experiment 1) or $1.25 (Experiments 2 and 3). The HIT was only available in the United States, Australia, and the United Kingdom. There were 63 participants in Experiment 1; 79 in Experiment 2 (plus an additional 15 that were replaced because of a technical problem with the surveys), and 60 in Experiment 3; our primary analysis included 52, 54, and 44 in each experiment (see below for the criteria for exclusion from analysis).

Methods

Participants completed a Qualtrics questionnaire with three parts: (1) demographic questions, (2) instructions, and (3) main task. Materials and other supporting information can be found at https://arnoldlab.web.unc.edu/publications/supporting-materials/arnold-mayo-dong-2021/.

Demographic questions

The first two questions asked about participants’ age and English proficiency. Participants were not allowed to continue if they were under age 18, reported nonnative fluency, or learned English past age 6. Questions about gender, sex, age, race, ethnicity, and education were used to compare samples (see supporting materials). Two questions asked about participants’ familiarity with individuals who identify as nonbinary: (a) How many individuals do you know who identify as neither male nor female? (None, 1, 2–5, 6, or more), and (b) How familiar are you with people who do not identify as either male or female? (0–10 sliding scale).

For Experiment 2 only, participants also completed the author recognition task, given previous findings that print exposure modulates individual biases to assign pronouns to the previous subject (Arnold et al., 2018). We used a version of this task that was designed by Peter Gordon’s lab (personal communication), and was developed from previous versions by Acheson et al. (2008) and Stanovich and West (1989). Participants saw a list of 62 true author names intermixed with 64 false names. They were asked to check the box next to all names they recognized, and not to guess. Because we found no effect of print exposure on they interpretation, we did not include this measure in Experiment 3.

Instructions

Prior to the primary task, participants were introduced to three characters: Alex (they/them), Liz (she/her), and Will (he/him), along with pictures of each (see Fig. 1). Participants were assigned to either the explicit or implicit condition. In the explicit lists, the pronouns of each character were given, and participants were tested to ensure that they remembered the names and pronouns of each character. In the implicit lists, participants just learned and practiced the names. Each name and pronoun were tested in two questions; if participants responded incorrectly, they were asked to correct their answer. Participants were told in the consent form “The purpose of this study is to understand how healthy adults understand language in simple stories.” Pronouns were not mentioned at all in the instructions except during introduction of the characters in the explicit lists.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Introductions of the characters for the explicit lists. Each picture and introductory sentence appeared alone on a separate screen. The implicit lists were identical, except they did not mention pronouns

Main task materials and design

The primary task was to read two-sentence stories and answer two questions (see Fig. 2 for example). For critical stimuli, the question of interest was designed to reveal whether participants assigned a singular or plural interpretation to the pronoun. For example, following “Liz walked home. She forgot to bring the keys,” the critical question asked “Who forgot the keys?” (see Table 1). Participants answered by clicking on either a picture of Liz alone, or a picture of Liz with Alex. During the practice questions, they were instructed to pick the picture that signaled a singular or plural actor: “The next example illustrates the use of pictures as answers. One picture shows Will and Alex together. This means they both did the action together. The other picture shows just Will. This picture means that just Will did the action alone. Pick the best answer.” Each question also had a content question, such as “What did Liz do?” (walked home/went to work). These questions were easy to answer if participants were reading the stories, and contributed to our inclusion criteria (see below).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Example critical test story and questions

Table 1 Example stimuli and critical questions

The experiment began with two practice stories, followed by training stories (12 in Experiments 1 and 2; 16 in Experiment 3), and then eight critical stimuli intermixed with 15 fillers. The training, critical, and filler stories were not labeled for the participants; instead, all stories appeared in a single sequence, one at a time on the screen. All stories were followed by two questions, one “who” question that was answered by selecting either a singular or plural option, and one content question. For three fillers the second sentence did not mention a person, so the “who” question was replaced by a content question.

The training stories served to illustrate the pronouns used by each character, so even in the implicit condition participants had the opportunity to learn them. In Experiments 1 and 2, there were four stories about each of the three characters. There was only one character in each story, providing strong evidence that the pronoun should refer to that character (see Table 1). The appropriate pronouns were consistently used for each character (Liz–she; Alex–they; Will–he).

Experiment 3 tested whether the number of training stories about Alex would improve performance on the critical items, so it included an additional four training stories about Alex (total eight Alex training stories). The first four training stories were identical to those used in Experiments 1 and 2; the second four followed a similar format.

The eight critical test stimuli used stories about Alex (see Fig. 2). These stories appeared in one of two critical discourse conditions. In Experiment 1, stories were either about just Alex (1-character condition), or Alex and another character (2-character condition). The two-person stories always introduced Alex in the subject position and the other person as an adjunct, in the form “Alex [did something] with Liz/Will.” The one-person stories were identical, except they did not include the with-phrase that mentioned the second character. In Experiments 2 and 3, all critical stories had two characters, manipulating whether Alex appeared first or second. There were two lists for each experiment, where List 1 and List 2 each had four items in each condition, counterbalanced across lists. Each of the two lists also appeared with both explicit and implicit instructions, resulting in a total of four lists (List 1-explicit; List 1-implicit; List 2-explicit, List 2-implicit).

Critical items were intermixed with 15 filler stories, with three each in five structures: (1) 1 character/no pronoun (e.g., Liz took a vacation. Sweden was lovely); (2) 1 character unambiguous pronoun (e.g., Will cleaned his apartment. He got his clothes dirty); (3) Unambiguous pronoun refers to subject (e.g., Liz danced with Will. She tripped many times); (4) Unambiguous pronoun refers to nonsubject (e.g., Will went shopping with Liz. She bought a suit); and (5) Plural they sentences (e.g., Will ran a marathon with Liz. They took more than five hours to finish). The plural-they fillers were all about Will and Liz, so the most plausible interpretation of they was as a plural pronoun. These items served to remind participants that they also has a plural interpretation. The fillers were pseudorandomly interspersed with the critical items such that there were no clusters of fillers of a particular type, and each type of filler was evenly distributed across the experiment.

Procedure

Participants accepted the Hit in Amazon Mechanical Turk and began the survey. After providing consent and answering the demographic questions, they read instructions about the survey and answered practice questions. Incorrect answers to the practice questions were corrected, and participants were asked to reread the story and answer again. They were then reminded that too many incorrect answers during the survey would result in the termination of the study, and they would not be paid. Then they began the main task.

Inclusion criteria

Participants were automatically excluded from the survey if they made 12 mistakes on the 43 test questions (26%) for Experiments 1 and 2, or 15 out of 47 (32%) for Experiment 3. These test questions excluded the critical “Who” questions about Alex. We also excluded the last nine items from our calculation to prevent participants from being kicked out when they were almost done with the study.

Our key question was whether explicitly introducing one’s pronouns has an impact on how people interpret singular they. Yet this question would be meaningless if it our comparison group were people who did not even realize that Alex uses they/them pronouns. Thus, the strongest test of our hypothesis is to compare the explicit lists with people from the implicit lists who responded successfully to the training questions, so we know that the subjects in both groups were fully aware of Alex’s use of they/them pronouns and willing to accept a singular interpretation of they. Participants may have resisted the singular interpretation for multiple reasons: perhaps they speak a dialect of English that does not permit singular they; perhaps they are ideologically opposed to this usage; or perhaps they were inattentive to the story context. Whatever the reason, we used the training set to divide participants into two groups. We labeled “resisters” as participants who made at least one “plural” response. The training stories mentioned only one person (e.g., “Alex saw a worm on the sidewalk. They tried to pick it up”), so the discourse context strongly supported a singular interpretation. Some individuals consistently responded “plural” to these items, demonstrating a strong aversion to the singular interpretation. Other participants provided a mix of singular and plural responses. Regardless of the motivation, we limited the “nonresister” group to those who responded singular on 100% of the training stories, demonstrating successful recognition that Alex uses they/them pronouns.

Following other studies in our lab (e.g., Williams & Arnold, 2019), we aimed for an initial sample size of about 30 participants per between-subjects condition, prior to resister exclusions. We excluded data from analysis for participants who reported a language disorder, or for those with an overall accuracy lower than 75% on the full set of filler questions, which included the content questions for all items, plus all questions for fillers, including the last nine that were excluded from the automatic Qualtrics exclusion criteria. For Experiment 2, we also followed our lab’s standard policy of excluding participants if more than 33% of their responses on the author recognition task were foils, because it signaled that they did not follow the directions to not guess (see Table 2).

Table 2 Number of participants excluded in each study

Analysis

Our dependent measure in all experiments was the interpretation of the eight critical test questions for the stories about Alex, where participants chose either a singular or plural interpretation. Our key predictors were our two manipulated variables: (1) Did participants receive explicit introduction of pronouns or not? and (2) Does the discourse context promote singular interpretation or not? The discourse context variable reflected the singular versus two-person context in Experiment 1, and the Alex-first versus Alex-second context in Experiments 2 and 3.

Given our binary dependent measure, we analyzed responses in a mixed-effects logistic regression, using SAS proc glimmix, with a binary distribution and a logit link. Models included maximal random effects (Barr et al., 2013), including random intercepts for participants and items, and random slopes as appropriate. All predictors were grand-mean centered. Raw data are available here: https://osf.io/fr9az/?view_only.

Predictions

Our first question is whether explicit introduction of pronouns facilitates the singular interpretation of they. We predict that it does. Our second question is whether the discourse context changes the availability of the singular interpretation. Following other work on pronoun comprehension, we expect that the singular interpretation should be stronger when there is only one character in the context, and when the nonbinary character was mentioned first in the story. Our final question was whether an increased number of training stories in Experiment 3 would result in a higher rate of singular interpretations for the critical stories, compared with Experiment 2.

Results

Training Items

Table 3 shows training performance for the Alex stories in all three experiments. By comparison, in the training stories about Liz and Will, interpretation of he/she pronouns was singular almost all the time (100% in Experiment 1, 99.8% in Experiment 2, and 99.6% in Experiment 3). Across all three experiments, the total number of nonresisters was 84 for explicit lists, and 66 for implicit lists, while the total number of resisters was nine for explicit lists and 24 for implicit lists. The chi-square statistic for this 2 × 2 comparison is 8.9 (p = .003), suggesting that explicit introduction of pronouns leads to higher singular interpretation in the training set.

Table 3 Rate of selecting the singular interpretation for the training Alex sentences

Critical item results

Given that participants had already answered “singular” on all training questions, we asked how often they had a singular interpretation of the critical question about Alex. In all cases the question asked “Who did [the action]?”, where the two possible answers were either Alex, or Alex together with someone else. Results (see Table 4 and Fig. 3) showed that for all the two-person contexts, singular interpretations were higher following explicit than following implicit pronoun introduction, with the difference between means averaging 25% for all the Alex first and Alex second conditions (range: difference of 15%–31%). In the one-person context, people responded singular almost all the time.

Table 4 Average selection of the singular interpretation in the critical stories about Alex (standard error is reported in parentheses)
Fig. 3
figure 3

Average percentage of singular interpretations for nonbinary “they” as a function of the pronoun-introduction condition (explicit vs. implicit) and discourse context (1-person; 2-person Alex first, 2-person Alex second); error bars represent the standard error of subject means

We also observed a strong effect of discourse context. In Experiment 1, the singular interpretation was adopted almost all the time when Alex was the only participant in the discourse, but less than half the time when there were two people in the context. In Experiment 1, Alex was always mentioned first in the two-person context. Experiments 2 and 3 further explored the two-person context by manipulating whether Alex was mentioned first or second. The singular interpretation was more frequent for the Alex-first contexts than the Alex-second contexts in both experiments, with a difference of 9% or 10% between conditions.

We tested the reliability of these patterns with a mixed-effects logistic regression, using SAS proc glimmix. Inferential statistics are presented in Table 5. In Experiment 1, the effect of discourse context was significant (p < .001), although the effect of explicit versus implicit pronoun introduction did not quite reach significance (p = .091). This experiment also revealed that all variation occurred in the two-person context, while singular interpretations were at ceiling in the one-person context. In Experiments 2 and 3, we observed significant effects of both discourse context and explicitness. There were no interactions.

Table 5 Inferential statistics

Our second question was whether singular interpretation is higher when people have had more practice. In Experiment 3, we provided eight training stories about Alex, instead of the four training stories used in Experiments 1 and 2. However, Experiment 3 had no more singular responses than Experiment 2, and in fact the average rate of singular responses was numerically lower for Experiment 3 (21%) than Experiment 2 (25%). When we entered both experiments into a single analysis, we again observed main effects of the discourse context and explicitness, but no effect of Experiment 2 versus 3, nor any interactions (see supporting materials for statistical detail).

We also asked whether individuals vary in how often they adopt the singular interpretation, based on (1) how many nonbinary individuals they know, and (2) how familiar they are with nonbinary individuals. However, there were no effects of Question 1 (how many nonbinary people they know). While greater familiarity predicted more singular responses for Experiment 1, this effect was not replicated in the other experiments. For Experiment 2, we also tested individual differences in print exposure, using the author recognition task, and found no effect. For a full report of the individual difference analyses, see Arnold et al. (2020).

General discussion

Our critical finding is that explicitly introducing Alex’s pronouns promotes the singular interpretation of they. This finding emerged in our main analysis, which was restricted to the “nonresister” participants who clearly understood that Alex uses they/them pronouns. While it is not surprising that people can learn from our training examples that Alex uses they/them pronouns, it is notable that the explicit introduction of pronouns had a significant effect over and above this exposure effect. We also saw an effect of explicit introduction on training performance: We had more “resisters” in the implicit than explicit conditions. Unsurprisingly, discourse context mattered too: Singular interpretation was more frequent when Alex was the only character or the first character in the context. However, reading an additional four training stimuli in Experiment 3 had no effect.

Our experimental task mimics the newly popular social convention of stating one’s pronouns explicitly, and in doing so demonstrates that this practice has a direct effect on how pronouns are understood. To understand why this practice matters, we consider the cognitive processes known to be involved in the representation and use of gender during pronoun interpretation.

Gender critically has representations at multiple levels, both conceptual and linguistic. Pronouns are words that are marked linguistically for gender, including feminine (she), masculine (he), and ungendered (it, they). They are also marked for number, where she/he/it are singular, but they can be used with either singular or plural meanings. These features help determine how pronouns map onto referents in the world, which can be categorized in terms of gender at a conceptual level. While some languages have broader systems of lexical gender (e.g., Spanish or German), here we focus on the use of pronouns in English, where gender is primarily used to match the conceptual gender of animate referents.

We couch our interpretation within Ackerman’s (2019) framework, which proposes three levels of representation of gender (see Fig. 4). Linguistic gender features are represented on the “feature” tier. Conceptual (nonlinguistic) gender falls on two different tiers. The “exemplar” tier is a multidimensional representation of features of gender expression, based on culturally relevant gender features (e.g., body shape, clothing). These exemplars form the input to the gender categories on the “category” tier. For most people, exemplars are clumped into bimodal representations of male and female, while the space outside these categories accounts for cases of unknown gender, ambiguous gender, or nonbinary gender.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Ackerman’s (2019) schema of three gender tiers: the exemplar tier, the category tier, and the feature tier. (Reprinted from Ackerman, 2019, p. 22)

Research suggests that they is perceived as ungendered (Foertsch & Gernsbacher, 1997; Camilliere et al., 2019; Leventhal et al., 2020; Prasad & Morris, 2020). Konnelly and Cowper (2020) suggest that the most innovative users of singular they find it acceptable even with a gendered referent (e.g., mother), but we have observed that these cases tend to occur when the identity of the referent is backgrounded or suppressed. This suggests that for many language users, they is only used when the conceptual representation of gender is either absent or very weak. Thus, on the Akerman model, the feature tier represents they without masculine or feminine features, and at the category tier it picks out referents that fall between the feminine and masculine categories.

Under the above model, the comprehension of they requires the identification of a specific referent. The set of available referents is constrained by the gender and number features of the pronoun, and could include singular/ungendered or plural referents. This presents a classic case of lexical ambiguity. We theorize that encountering an instance of they leads to a frequency-weighted competition between singular and plural meanings, similar to other words with multiple senses or meanings (e.g., Duffy et al., 1988). It simultaneously leads to activation of potential referents, weighted by the contextual support for each. In our example Alex went to the store. They . . . , the plural meaning is relatively more activated due to frequency, but Alex is the most available discourse referent, supporting the singular interpretation.

On this model, there are three possible mechanisms by which pronoun introduction affects interpretation. First, it may affect linguistic representations: hearing “Alex uses they/them pronouns” might remind the listener of the singular use of they and thereby strengthen the singular lexical representation, increasing its level of baseline activation. Second, it may affect conceptual representations by directing the participant’s attention to the fact that some people identify outside the gender binary, effectively broadening the ungendered gap in the category tier. Third, it may situate Alex clearly on the exemplar tier. Either of the second two possibilities would strengthen the representation of Alex outside of binary gender categories, supporting the match with they. A question for future work is whether introducing Alex’s pronouns facilitates the interpretation of they for a new nonbinary individual; if so, it would support a more general shift in the use of they (the first two possibilities here).

Explicit instructions about pronouns may also modulate the semantic features associated with they. Usage statistics would suggest that singular they bears a socially distant semantic feature (Camilliere et al., 2019), while the nonbinary use requires a socially close interpretation. These variants also may compete during interpretation. Explicit pronoun instructions establish expectations that they can refer to a specific individual, and not just when the speaker wishes to deemphasize the identity and gender of the referent.

Importantly, our experiment shows that when people read that Alex uses they/them pronouns, it does more than simply help them categorize Alex as nonbinary. We limited our analysis to people who responded perfectly to the Alex training questions, which means that all participants were equally aware of Alex’s nonbinary status. Yet even within this sample, people who had received the explicit instructions were more likely to assign a singular interpretation. This showed up particularly in the two-person context, where there was a plausible competing plural interpretation.

Our experiment highlights variation as a function of discourse features, but singular they is also more common for some groups than others (Camilliere et al., 2019; Konnelly & Cowper, 2020; Nichols et al., 2019). Individuals likely vary in both the baseline weight of singular they, and their experience of gendered exemplars and the corresponding conceptual categories relating to gender. Individuals may also differ in the degree to which singular they is semantically marked for social distance. In principle, individual facility with singular they should change as a function of repeated exposure to discourse contexts where the singular interpretation is supported, as in our explicit condition and our supporting discourse contexts. However, we did not observe any difference between Experiments 2 and 3, so merely adding four training sentences is not enough. We also found little effect of our measures of familiarity with nonbinary individuals (see Arnold et al., 2020), but this is likely due to limitations in our coarse-grained questions, and lack of variability in our sample. At this point in time, nonbinary singular they is relatively new to many people, so the biggest influence on interpretation is a direct discussion of its meaning.

In sum, singular they has been around for centuries, but its frequency and range of uses is currently expanding. This expansion parallels a movement to adopt a more fluid concept of gender, which has entered public awareness through the trend of using pronouns as a symbol of gender identity. It is likely that these changes are influenced by multiple forces, one of which is talking about pronouns. In short, people who hear that Alex uses they/them pronouns are more likely to assume that they refers to Alex.