Case Report
Moral credentials and the 2020 democratic presidential primary: No evidence that endorsing female candidates licenses people to favor men,

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104144Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We investigated moral licensing in the 2020 US Democratic presidential primaries.

  • We predicted that endorsing a female candidate would license Democrats to favor men.

  • Two high-powered, pre-registered studies did not support this prediction.

  • Bayesian analysis showed that the data were more consistent with a null effect.

  • Neither participant gender nor gender attitudes moderated this null effect.

Abstract

Endorsing Obama in 2008 licensed some Americans to favor Whites over Blacks––an example of moral self-licensing (Effron, Cameron, & Monin, 2009). Could endorsing a female presidential candidate in 2020–21 similarly license Americans to favor men at the expense of women? Two high-powered, pre-registered experiments found no evidence for this possibility. We manipulated whether Democrat participants had an opportunity to endorse a female Democratic candidate if she ran against a male candidate (i.e., Trump in Study 1, N = 2143; an anti-Trump Republican or independent candidate in Study 2, N = 2228). Then, participants read about a stereotypically masculine job and indicated whether they thought a man should fill it. Contrary to predictions, we found that endorsing a female Democrat did not increase participants' tendency to favor men over women for the job. We discuss implications for the robustness and generalizability of moral self-licensing.

Section snippets

Method

We pre-registered this study at aspredicted.org/blind.php?x=a3q2kr and collected data between March 3rd–7th, 2020.

Study 2

Study 1 suggests that endorsing a female presidential candidate did not license Democrat participants to indicate that a man was better suited than a woman for a stereotypically masculine job (i.e., no evidence for moral licensing). Study 2 aimed to address a potential methodological explanation for this effect: Perhaps Study 1's materials made participants interpret their choice of a woman as inadequate evidence of their non-sexism. We considered two versions of this explanation. First,

General discussion

Two high-powered, pre-registered experiments found no evidence that endorsing a female presidential candidate licensed Democrats to express a more-stereotypical hiring preference. This effect emerged regardless of participants' gender, attitudes towards women, or motivation to respond without sexism. Whereas endorsing Obama in 2008 licensed voters to favor Whites over Blacks (Effron et al., 2009), and rejecting sexist statements licensed participants to favor men over women for a hypothetical

References (64)

  • V. Tiefenbeck et al.

    For better or for worse? Empirical evidence of moral licensing in a behavioral energy conservation campaign

    Energy Policy

    (2013)
  • J. Urban et al.

    Green consumption does not make people cheat: Three attempts to replicate moral licensing effect due to pro-environmental behavior

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (2019)
  • M.G. Ahmad et al.

    Can good followers create unethical leaders? How follower citizenship leads to leader moral licensing and unethical behavior

    The Journal of Applied Psychology

    (2020)
  • I. Blanken et al.

    A meta-analytic review of moral licensing

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2015)
  • I. Blanken et al.

    Three attempts to replicate the moral licensing effect

    Social Psychology

    (2014)
  • J.C. Bradley-Geist et al.

    Moral credentialing by association: The importance of choice and relationship closeness

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2010)
  • B. Carpenter et al.

    Stan: A probabilistic programming language

    Grantee Submission

    (2017)
  • A. Chozick et al.

    Donald Trump’s gender-based attacks on Hillary Clinton have calculated risk

    (2016)
  • P. Conway et al.

    When does feeling moral actually make you a better person? Conceptual abstraction moderates whether past moral deeds motivate consistency or compensatory behavior

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2012)
  • G. Cornelissen et al.

    Rules or consequences? The role of ethical mind-sets in moral dynamics

    Psychological Science

    (2013)
  • C.S. Crandall et al.

    A justification-suppression model of the expression and experience of prejudice

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2003)
  • D.A. Effron

    Making mountains of morality from molehills of virtue: Threat causes people to overestimate their moral credentials

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2014)
  • D.A. Effron

    Beyond “being good frees us to be bad:” moral self-licensing and the fabrication of moral credentials

  • D.A. Effron et al.

    Inventing racist roads not taken: The licensing effect of immoral counterfactual behaviors

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2012)
  • F. Faul et al.

    G* power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences

    Behavior Research Methods

    (2007)
  • J. Filipovic

    Our president has always degraded women—and we’ve always let him

    (2017)
  • A. Fishbach et al.

    Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (2005)
  • O. Georgeac et al.

    Progress in women’s representation in top leadership weakens people’s disturbance with gender inequality in other domains

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2019)
  • K.J. Gergen

    Social psychology as history

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1973)
  • M. Gholamzadehmir et al.

    Moral licensing, moral cleansing and pro-environmental behaviour: The moderating role of pro-environmental attitudes

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (2019)
  • P. Glick

    Gender, sexism, and the election: Did sexism help trump more than it hurt Clinton?

    Politics, Groups, and Identities

    (2019)
  • P. Glick et al.

    The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • Cited by (4)

    • Moral inconsistency

      2023, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
    • Moral currencies: Explaining corrupt collaboration

      2022, Current Opinion in Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, it is possible to purchase eco-friendly products and avoid stealing. Similarly, a recent article shows that endorsing a female candidate did not lead to an increased tendency to favor men in another context [16]. Obviously, it is also possible to make two unethical choices when actions are independent from one another.

    • Regulatory focus and self-licensing dynamics: A motivational account of behavioural consistency and balancing

      2022, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This self-licensing effect (e.g., Merritt et al., 2010) echoes neighbouring phenomena of boomerang effect (e.g., Schultz et al., 2007), negative spillover (e.g., Truelove et al., 2014), and balancing (e.g., Fishbach & Dhar, 2005), which refer to similar alternating of positive and negative behaviours. Despite growing recent research, the circumstances under which people are more likely to demonstrate behavioural consistency versus self-licensing are not fully elucidated yet (Giurge et al., 2021; Mullen & Monin, 2016), especially in the environmental domain. In the present paper, we rely on regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 1997) to suggest one of such possible moderators.

    Data, code, experimental materials, and pre-registration documents are available via the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/ja83x/.

    This paper has been recommended for acceptance by Professor. Rachel Barkan.

    View full text