Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Ecological and Developmental Perspectives on Social Learning

Introduction to the Special Issue

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this special issue of Human Nature we explore the possible adaptive links between teaching and learning during childhood, and we aim to expand the dialogue on the ways in which the social sciences, and in particular current anthropological research, may better inform our shifting understanding of how these processes vary in different social and ecological environments. Despite the cross-disciplinary trend toward incorporating more behavioral and cognitive data outside of postindustrial state societies, much of the published cross-cultural data is presented as stand-alone population-level studies, making it challenging to extrapolate trends or incorporate both ecological and developmental perspectives. Here, contributors explore how human life history, ecological experience, cumulative culture, and ethnolinguistics impact social learning and child development in foraging and transitioning societies around the world. Using historical ethnographic data and qualitative and quantitative data from studies with contemporary populations, authors interrogate the array of factors that likely interact with cognitive development and learning. They provide contributions that explore the unique environmental, social, and cultural conditions that characterize such populations, offering key insights into processes of social learning, adaptive learning responses, and culture change. This series of articles demonstrates that children are taught culturally and environmentally salient skills in myriad ways, ranging from institutionalized instruction to brief, nuanced, and indirect instruction. Our hope is that this collection stimulates more research on the evolutionary and developmental implications associated with teaching and learning among humans.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amir, D., Jordan, M. R., McAuliffe, K., Valeggia, C. R., Sugiyama, L. S., Bribiescas, R. G., Snodgrass, J. J., & Dunham, Y. (2020). The developmental origins of risk and time preferences across diverse societies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(4), 650–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, P. M., & Harvey, P. H. (1985). Brain size, development and metabolism in birds and mammals. Journal of Zoology, 207(4), 491–509.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berndt, C., & Yunupingu, D. (1979). Land of the rainbow snake: Aboriginal children's stories and songs from western Arnhem Land. Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. (2005). Martu children’s hunting strategies in the Western Desert, Australia. In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods (1st ed., pp. 129–146). Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2002). Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature, 13(2), 239–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N., & Konner, M. J. (1976). !Kung knowledge of animal behavior. In R. B. Lee & I. DeVore (Eds.), Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors (pp. 325–348). Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones, N., Hawkes, K., & Draper, P. (1994). Differences between Hadza and !Kung children’s work: Original affluence or practical reason? In E. S. Burch & L. J. Ellanna (Eds.), Key issues in hunter-gatherer research (pp. 189–215). Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. (2002). Evolutionary demography and intrahousehold time allocation: School attendance and child labor among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. American Journal of Human Biology, 14(2), 206–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B., & Smith, B. H. (1996). Evolution of the human life cycle. American Journal of Human Biology, 8(6), 703–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2005). The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(Suppl. 2), 10918–10925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (2018). Teaching in hunter-gatherers. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 9(4), 771–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broesch, T., Carolan, P., Cebioğlu, S., von Rueden, C., Boyette, A. H., Moya, C., Hewlett, B. L., & Kline, M. A. (2021). Opportunities for interaction: Natural observations of children’s social behavior in five societies. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09393-w.

  • Bruner, J. S. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27(8), 687–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R., & Byrne, R. W. (1995). The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford University Press on Demand. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198522652.001.0001.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Caro, T. M., & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? The Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 151–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L., Feldman, M., Chen, K., & Dornbusch, S. (1982). Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science, 218(4567), 19–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., & Sperber, D. (2010). Imitation explains the propagation, not the stability of animal culture. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1681), 651–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016a). A cross-cultural comparison of children’s imitative flexibility. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1435–1444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016b). Instrumental and conventional interpretations of behavior are associated with distinct outcomes in early childhood. Child Development, 87(2), 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K. H., Fusaro, M., & Harris, P. L. (2009). Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20(3), 372–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K. H., Kim, E., Song, G., & Harris, P. L. (2013). Young children’s deference to a majority varies by culture. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13, 367–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corriveau, K. H., Min, G., & Kurkul, K. (2014). Cultural differences in children’s learning from others. In E. J. Robinson & S. Einav (Eds.), Trust and skepticism: Children’s selective learning from testimony (pp. 95–109). Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J. (2019). Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, 90(3), 759–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden, A. N., Farahani, A., Herlosky, K. N., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, I. A., Ruginski, I. T., & Cashdan, E. (2021). Harm avoidance and mobility during middle childhood and adolescence among Hadza foragers. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09390-z.

  • Crognier, E., Villena, M., & Vargas, E. (2002). Helping patterns and reproductive success in Aymara communities. American Journal of Human Biology, 14(3), 372–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2006). Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In Y. Munakata & M. H. Johnson (Eds.), Processes of change in brain and cognitive development (pp. 249–274). Oxford University Press.

  • Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2019). Spatial cognition, navigation, and mobility among children in a forager-horticulturalist population, the Tsimané of Bolivia. Cognitive Development, 52, 100800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H. E., Stieglitz, J., Kaplan, H., & Gurven, M. (2020). Poor school quality augments differences in children’s abstract reasoning over time: A natural experiment. Under review. https://psyarxiv.com/d3sgq/.

  • Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2020). You don’t have to know where your kids are, just where they aren’t: Exploring free-range parenting in the Bolivian Amazon. In B. K. Ashdown & A. N. Faherty (Eds.), Parents and caregivers across cultures (pp. 59–74). Springer International.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, H.E., Stack, J., and Cashdan, E. (2021). Cultural change reduces gender differences in mobility and spatial ability among seminomadic pastoralist-forager children in northern Namibia. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09388-7.

  • Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (1980). Differential socialization of girls and boys in light of cross-cultural research. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1980(8), 45–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, B. J., Bianchi, J., Griskevicius, V., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2017). Beyond risk and protective factors: An adaptation-based approach to resilience. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(4), 561–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fagen, R. (1974). Selective and evolutionary aspects of animal play. The American Naturalist, 108(964), 850–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, E. G., Laland, K. N., Kendal, R. L., & Kendal, J. R. (2013). Developmental niche construction (with commentaries). Developmental Science, 16(2), 296–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froehle, A. W., Wells, G. K., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Lew‐Levy, S., & Crittenden, A. N. (2019). Physical activity and time budgets of Hadza forager children: Implications for self-provisioning and the ontogeny of the sexual division of labor. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(1). https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23209.

  • Garfield, Z. H., Garfield, M. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2016). A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 19–34). Japan: Springer.

  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion, 41(3), 389–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A. (2020). Childhood as a solution to explore–exploit tensions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1803), 20190502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., O’Grady, S., Lucas, C. G., Griffiths, T. L., Wente, A., Bridgers, S., Aboody, R., Fung, H., & Dahl, R. E. (2017). Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 7892–7899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, R. A. (1969). Yiwara: Foragers of the Australian desert. Charles Scribners.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, M. D. (2018). Broadening horizons: Sample diversity and socioecological theory are essential to the future of psychological science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 11420–11427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hames, R., & Draper, P. (2004). Women’s work, child care, and helpers-at-the-nest in a hunter-gatherer society. Human Nature, 15(4), 319–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, P. L., & Corriveau, K. H. (2011). Young children’s selective trust in informants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1179–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22(8), 727–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 244–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(4), 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 123–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., & Broesch, J. (2011). On the nature of cultural transmission networks: Evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1139–1148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2016). Innovation, processes of social learning, and modes of cultural transmission among the Chabu adolescent forager-farmers of Ethiopia. In H. Terashima & B. S. Hewlett (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers (pp. 203–215). Japan: Springer.

  • Hewlett, B. L. (2021). Social learning and innovation in adolescence: A comparative study of Aka and Chabu hunter-gatherers of Central and Eastern Africa. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09391-y.

  • Hewlett, B., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88(4), 922–934.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching in hunter-gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science, 3(1), 150403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1168–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Imamura, K., & Akiyama, H. (2016). How hunter-gatherers have learned to hunt: Transmission of hunting methods and techniques among the central Kalahari San (Natural History of Communication among the Central Kalahari San). African Study Monographs (Suppl.), 52, 61–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S., & Robson, A. J. (2002). The emergence of humans: The coevolution of intelligence and longevity with intergenerational transfers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(15), 10221–10226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. L. (1983). Hunter-gatherer mobility strategies. Journal of Anthropological Research, 39(3), 277–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, O., Ventura, P., Fernandes, T., Marques, L. G., Licata, L., & Semin, G. R. (2010). Effects of schooling and literacy on linguistic abstraction: The role of holistic vs. analytic processing styles. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(7), 1095–1102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinfeld, J. (1971). Visual memory in village Eskimo and urban Caucasian children. Arctic, 24(2), 132–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, M. A., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2013). Teaching and the life history of cultural transmission in Fijian villages. Human Nature, 24(4), 351–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood: Relationships, emotion, mind. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, K. L. (2021). Childhood teaching and learning among savanna Pumé hunter-gatherers: Mismatch between foraging and postindustrial societies. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09392-x.

  • Kuzawa, C. W., & Bragg, J. M. (2012). Plasticity in human life history strategy: Implications for contemporary human variation and the evolution of genus Homo. Current Anthropology, 53(S6), S369–S382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning and Behavior, 32(1), 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2010). Learning “from nobody”: The limited role of teaching in folk models of children’s development. Childhood in the Past, 3(1), 79–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2014). The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel, changelings. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lancy, D. F. (2016). Playing with knives: The socialization of self-initiated learners. Child Development, 87, 654–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, R. B. (1966). !Kung Bushman subsistence: An input-output analysis. Queen's Printer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills? Human Nature, 28(4), 367–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Reckin, R., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2018). How do hunter-gatherer children learn social and gender norms? A meta-ethnographic review. Cross-Cultural Research, 2, 213–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Kissler, S. M., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Hewlett, B. S. (2020). Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka hunter-gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(1), 12–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lew-Levy, S., Ringen, E. J., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., Broesch, T., & Kline, M. A. (2021). The life history of learning subsistence skills among Hadza and BaYaka foragers from Tanzania and the Republic of Congo. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09386-9.

  • Maynard, A. E., & Tovote, K. E. (2010). Learning from other children. In David F. Lancy, John Bock, & Suzanne Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood (pp. 181–205). Rowman and Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., & Strimling, P. (2008). When natural selection favors imitation of parents. Current Anthropology, 49(2), 307–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, R. A., & Wertz, A. E. (2021). Early plant learning in Fiji. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09389-6.

  • Meehan, C. L., & Crittenden, A. N. (Eds.). (2016). Childhood: Origins, evolution, and implications. University of New Mexico Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Chang, L., Murray, K., & Lu, H. J. (2015). Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1798), 20142209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. J. (1990). Thoughtful foragers: A study of prehistoric decision making. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moller, L. C., Hymel, S., & Rubin, K. H. (1992). Sex typing in play and popularity in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 26–26(7–8), 331–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., & Angelillo, C. (2003). Cultural variation in young children’s access to work or involvement in specialised child-focused activities. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(3), 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, R. K. (1986). Hunters of the northern forest: Designs for survival among the Alaskan Kutchin. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: Essential elements in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 170–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odden, H., & Rochat, P. (2004). Observational learning and enculturation. Educational and Child Psychology, 21(2), 39–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., and Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press.

  • Ohmagari, K., & Berkes, F. (1997). Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the Western James Bay Cree women of subarctic Canada. Human Ecology, 25(2), 197–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paradise, R., & Rogoff, B. (2009). Side by side: Learning by observing and pitching in. Ethos, 37(1), 102–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pope, S. M., Fagot, J., Meguerditchian, A., Washburn, D. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2019). Enhanced cognitive flexibility in the seminomadic Himba. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 50(1), 47–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinlan, M. B., Quinlan, R. J., Council, S. K., & Roulette, J. W. (2016). Children’s acquisition of ethnobotanical knowledge in a Caribbean horticultural village. Journal of Ethnobiology, 36(2), 433–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-Garcia, V., Molina, J. L., Broesch, J., Calvet, L., Huanca, T., Saus, J., Tanner, S., Leonard, W. R., & McDade, T. W. (2008). Do the aged and knowledgeable men enjoy more prestige? A test of predictions from the prestige-bias model of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 275–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García, V., Broesch, J., Calvet-Mir, L., Fuentes-Peláez, N., McDade, T. W., Parsa, S., Tanner, S., Huanca, T., Leonard, W. R., & Martínez-Rodríguez, M. R. (2009). Cultural transmission of ethnobotanical knowledge and skills: An empirical analysis from an Amerindian society. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 274–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyes-García, V., Pyhälä, A., Díaz-Reviriego, I., Duda, R., Fernández-Llamazares, Á., Gallois, S., Guèze, M., & Napitupulu, L. (2016). Schooling, local knowledge and working memory: A study among three contemporary hunter-gatherer societies. PLoS ONE, 11(1), 0145265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Moore, L., Najafi, B., Dexter, A., Correa-Chávez, M., & Solís, J. (2007). Children’s development of cultural repertoires through participation in everyday routines and practices. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 490–515). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2021). Co-occurrence of ostensive communication and generalizable knowledge in forager storytelling: Cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager societies. Human Nature, 32(1), https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09385-w.

  • Scalise Sugiyama, M., & Sugiyama, L. S. (2011). “Once the child is lost, he dies”: Monster stories vis-a-vis the problem of errant children. In E. Slingerland & M. Collard (Eds.), Creating consilience: Integrating the sciences and the humanities (pp. 351–371). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scalise Sugiyama, M., Mendoza, M., & Quiroz, I. (2020a). Ethnobotanical knowledge encoded in Weenhayek oral tradition. Journal of Ethnobiology, 40(1), 39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scalise Sugiyama, M., Mendoza, M., & Sugiyama, L. (2020b). War games: Intergroup coalitional play fighting as a means of comparative coalition formidability assessment. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences. Advance online publication. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Febs0000251.

  • Schniter, E., Macfarlan, S. J., Garcia, J. J., Ruiz-Campos, G., Beltran, D. G., Bowen, B. B., and Lerback, J. C. (2021). Age-appropriate wisdom? Ethnobiological knowledge ontogeny in pastoralist Mexican Choyeros. Human Nature, 32(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09387-8.

  • Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1973). Cognitive consequences of formal and informal education: New accommodations are needed between school-based learning and learning experiences of everyday life. Science, 182(4112), 553–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2011). From hominins to humans: How sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1566), 809–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S., Ziv, M., & Stein, A. (2002). Teaching as a natural cognition and its relations to preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 17(3–4), 1473–1487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9(4), 545–569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D. (1978). Play and aggression: A study of rhesus monkeys. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tehrani, J. J., & Riede, F. (2008). Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology, 40(3), 316–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tehrani, J. J., & Collard, M. (2009). On the relationship between interindividual cultural transmission and population-level cultural diversity: A case study of weaving in Iranian tribal populations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 286–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405–2415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction, and the arts. SubStance, 30(1), 6–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, B., & Young, A. G. (2017). Growing up Mikea. In B. S. Hewlett & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter-gatherer childhoods (1st ed., pp. 147–171). Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vashro, L., Padilla, L., & Cashdan, E. (2016). Sex differences in mobility and spatial cognition: A test of the fertility and parental care hypothesis in northwestern Namibia. Human Nature, 27(1), 16–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psycho-cultural analysis. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helen Elizabeth Davis.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Davis, H.E., Crittenden, A.N. & Scalise Sugiyama, M. Ecological and Developmental Perspectives on Social Learning. Hum Nat 32, 1–15 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09394-9

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-021-09394-9

Keywords

Navigation