Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-14T08:53:28.691Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

EXPLORING SYNTACTIC PRIMING AS A MEASURE OF IMPLICIT LANGUAGE APTITUDE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2021

Shaofeng Li
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Jiancheng Qian*
Affiliation:
Zhengzhou University
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jiancheng Qian, School of International Studies, Zhengzhou University, 100 Kexue Road, Zhengzhou, Henan450001, P. R. China. E-mail: qjc2000@sina.com.

Abstract

This study investigates the validity of syntactic priming as a measure of implicit language aptitude. Syntactic priming refers to the tendency to reproduce a linguistic structure due to a previous exposure to the structure. The validity of the construct was verified by collating evidence for divergent validity—whether it is dissociable from explicit aptitude; convergent validity—whether it is correlated with other measures of implicit aptitude; and predictive validity—whether it is predictive of learning attainment. One hundred sixty-six university EFL learners completed three tests of implicit aptitude: syntactic priming, sequence learning, and LLAMA_D; three tests of explicit aptitude: LLAMA_B, _E, and _F; and three tests of L2 proficiency: untimed grammaticality judgment, metalinguistic knowledge, and elicited imitation. The results showed that syntactic priming was dissociable from explicit aptitude, but it failed to converge with the other measures of implicit aptitude, and it also failed to predict L2 proficiency. The results also showed that priming was negatively correlated with sequence learning and that sequence learning was a negative predictor of learners’ metalinguistic knowledge. On the other hand, the construct validity of explicit aptitude was strong. The results suggest the multidimensionality of implicit aptitude and the need for more research into the construct validity of syntactic priming as a cognitive ability for implicit learning.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Artieda, G., & Muñoz, C. (2016). The LLAMA tests and the underlying structure of language aptitude at two levels of foreign language proficiency. Learning and Individual Differences, 50, 4248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J., Dell, G., Chang, F., & Onishi, K. (2007). Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production. Cognition, 104, 437458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J., & Loebell, H. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35, 139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. K., Loebell, H., & Morey, R. (1992). From conceptual roles to structural relations: Bridging the syntactic cleft. Psychological Review, 99, 150171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bokander, L., & Bylund, E. (2020). Probing the internal validity of the LLAMA language aptitude tests. Language Learning, 70, 1147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z., Pickering, M., Yan, H., & Branigan, H. (2011). Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Cantonese-mandarin bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 431445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Z. G., Pickering, M. J., Wang, R., & Branigan, H. P. (2015). It is there whether you hear it or not: Syntactic representations of missing arguments. Cognition, 136, 255267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J., & Sapon, S. (2002). Manual for the MLAT. Second Language Testing, Inc.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. M. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 499533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2020). Skill acquisition theory. In VanPatten, B. Keating, G., & Wulff, S. (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition (pp. 83104). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2005). Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: A psychometric study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 141172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erlam, R. (2005). Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional effectiveness in second language acquisition. Language Teaching Research, 9, 147171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R., Li, S., & Zhu, Y. (2019). The effects of pre-task explicit instruction on the performance of a focused task. System, 80, 3847.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J., & Frankish, K. (2009). In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V., Bock, J., Wilson, M., & Cohen, N. (2008). Memory for syntax despite amnesia. Psychological Science, 19, 940946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, V. S., & Bock, J. K. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 10111029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebauer, G., & Mackintosh, N. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 3454.Google ScholarPubMed
Granena, G. (2012). Age differences and cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning in ultimate second language attainment (Unpublished PhD dissertation). The University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Granena, G. (2013). Individual differences in sequence learning ability and second language acquisition in early childhood and adulthood. Language Learning, 63, 665704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2015). Cognitive aptitudes for implicit and explicit learning and information-processing styles: An individual differences study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 577600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive aptitudes and L2 speaking proficiency. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 313336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. H. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29, 311343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granena, G., & Yilmaz, Y. (2019). Corrective feedback and the role of implicit sequence learning ability in L2 online performance. Language Learning, 69, 127156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamrick, P. (2015). Declarative and procedural memory abilities as individual differences in incidental language learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 44, 915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hiver, P., & Al-Hoorie, A. (2020). Reexamining the role of vision in second language motivation: A preregistered conceptual replication of You, Dornyei and Csizer (2016). Language Learning, 70, 48102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, J., Pickering, M., Yang, J., Wang, S., & Branigan, H. (2016). The independence of syntactic processing in Mandarin: Evidence from structural priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 8198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulstijn, J. H. (2005). Theoretical and empirical issues in the study of implicit and explicit second-language learning: Introduction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwu, F., Wei, P., & Sun, S. (2014). Aptitude-treatment interaction effects on explicit rule learning: A latent growth curve analysis. Language Teaching Research, 18, 294319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. N. (2018). Second language structural priming: A critical review and directions for future research. Second Language Research, 34, 539552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (2012). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44, 137166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Jones, J. L. (2011). Structural priming as implicit learning: Cumulative priming effects and individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 11331139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, S., DeYoung, C., Gray, J., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., & Nam, H. (2017). Measures of implicit knowledge revisited: Processing modes, time pressure, and modality. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39, 431457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Y., & McDonough, K. (2008). Learners’ production of passives during syntactic priming activities. Applied Linguistics, 29, 149154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, R. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Li, C., & Thompson, S. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Li, S. (2015). The associations between language aptitude and second language grammar acquisition: A meta-analytic review of five decades of research. Applied Linguistics, 36, 385408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S. (2016). The construct validity of language aptitude. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 801842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, S. (2017). The effects of cognitive aptitudes on the process and product of L2 interaction: A synthetic review. In Gurzynski, L. (Ed.), Expanding individual difference research in the interaction approach: Investigating learners, instructors and researchers (pp. 4170). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Li, S., Shintani, N., & Ellis, R. (2012). Doing meta-analysis in SLA: Practices, choices, and standards. Contemporary Foreign Language Studies, 384, 127.Google Scholar
Linck, J., Hughes, M. M., Campbell, S. G., Silbert, N. H., Tare, M., Jackson, S. R., Smith, B. K., Bunting, M. F., & Doughty, C. J. (2013). Hi-LAB: A new measure of aptitude for high-level language proficiency. Language Learning, 63, 530566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linck, J., Osthus, P., Koeth, J., & Bunting, M. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and produciton: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21, 861883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (2015). Second language acquisition and task-based language teaching. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mahowald, K., James, A., Futrell, R., & Gibson, E. (2016). A meta-analysis of syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 91, 527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masgoret, A., & Gardner, R. (2003). Attitudes, motivation, and second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner and associates. Language Learning, 53, 123163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K. (2006). Interaction and syntactic priming: English L2 speakers’ production of dative constructions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 179207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K. (2011). Eliciting wh-questions through collaborative syntactic priming activities during peer interaction. In Trofimovich, P. & McDonough, K. (Eds.), Applying priming methods to L2 learning, teaching and research: insights from psycholinguistics (pp. 131151). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, K., & Fulga, A. (2015). The detection and primed production of novel constructions. Language Learning, 65, 326357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M., & Ferreira, V. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Re-Mermet, A., Souza, A., Gade, M., & Bastian, C. (2019). Is executive control related to working memory capacity and fluid intelligence? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148, 13351372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, K., Suzukida, Y., & Sun, H. (2019). Aptitude, experience, and second language pronunciation proficiency development in classroom settings. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 201225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumacker, R., & Lomax, R. (2010). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modelling. Routledge.Google Scholar
Skehan, P. (2015). Foreign language aptitude and its relationship with grammar: A critical overview. Applied Linguistics, 36, 367384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93, 1351513522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2015). Comparing elicited imitation and word monitoring as measures of implicit knowledge. Language Learning, 65, 860895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suzuki, Y., & DeKeyser, R. (2017). The interface of explicit and implicit knowledge in a second language: Insights from individual differences in cognitive aptitudes. Language Learning, 67, 747790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagarelli, K., Ruiz, S., Moreno Vega, J., & Rebuschat, P. (2016). Variability in second language learning: The roles of individual differences, learning conditions, and linguistic complexity. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38, 293316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teimouri, Y., Goetze, J., & Plonsky, L. (2019). Second language anxiety and achievement: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 363387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, E., Berry, C., & Shanks, D. (2013). Age effects on explicit and implicit memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woltz, D. (2003). Implicit cognitive processes as aptitudes for learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 95104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yi, W. (2018). Statistical sensitivity, cognitive aptitudes, and processing of collocations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40, 831856. First view.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yilmaz, Y., & Granena, G. (2019). Cognitive individual differences as predictives of improvement and awareness under implicit and explicit feedback conditions. Modern Language Journal, 103, 686702 Google Scholar
Zhang, R. (2015). Measuring university-level L2 learners’ implicit and explicit linguistic knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 457486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, X. (2019). Foreign language anxiety and foreign language performance: A meta-analysis. Modern Language Journal, 103, 763781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar