Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neuroanat., 02 February 2021
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2020.565109

Cerebrotypes in Cephalopods: Brain Diversity and Its Correlation With Species Habits, Life History, and Physiological Adaptations

Giovanna Ponte1 Morag Taite2 Luciana Borrelli1 Andrea Tarallo3 A. Louise Allcock2 Graziano Fiorito1*
  • 1Department of Biology and Evolution of Marine Organisms, Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn, Naples, Italy
  • 2Department of Zoology, Ryan Institute, National University of Ireland Galway, Galway, Ireland
  • 3Department of Research Infrastructures for Marine Biological Resources (RIMAR), Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn, Naples, Italy

Here we analyze existing quantitative data available for cephalopod brains based on classical contributions by J.Z. Young and colleagues, to cite some. We relate the relative brain size of selected regions (area and/or lobe), with behavior, life history, ecology and distribution of several cephalopod species here considered. After hierarchical clustering we identify and describe ten clusters grouping 52 cephalopod species. This allows us to describe cerebrotypes, i.e., differences of brain composition in different species, as a sign of their adaptation to specific niches and/or clades in cephalopod molluscs for the first time. Similarity reflecting niche type has been found in vertebrates, and it is reasonable to assume that it could also occur in Cephalopoda. We also attempted a phylogenetic PCA using data by Lindgren et al. (2012) as input tree. However, due to the limited overlap in species considered, the final analysis was carried out on <30 species, thus reducing the impact of this approach. Nevertheless, our analysis suggests that the phylogenetic signal alone cannot be a justification for the grouping of species, although biased by the limited set of data available to us. Based on these preliminary findings, we can only hypothesize that brains evolved in cephalopods on the basis of different factors including phylogeny, possible development, and the third factor, i.e., life-style adaptations. Our results support the working hypothesis that the taxon evolved different sensorial and computational strategies to cope with the various environments (niches) occupied in the oceans. This study is novel for invertebrates, to the best of our knowledge.

Introduction

Cephalopoda is the most charismatic class of the phylum Mollusca. The richness of their behavioral repertoire inspired many aspects of human life including contemporary art (as provided by camouflage and body patterns, see Nakajima, 2018) and robotics (e.g., Cianchetti et al., 2012; Xie et al., 2020; as inspired by the study of soft, flexible, and muscular body). In recent years, increased interest for their commercial value inspired gastronomy (Mouritsen and Styrbæk, 2018; see also: Sörensen and Mouritsen, 2019 and Cephs & Chefs: https://www.cephsandchefs.com/). In addition, social media provided access to specialized information and growing interest in interdisciplinary academic fields, and images and videos where cephalopods represent a great example (Nakajima et al., 2018; McClain, 2019). Together with fishes, images of cephalopods have been “liked” more than other organisms (including sharks) on social media platforms (McClain, 2019).

Cephalopods are an ancient taxon that diverged from a monoplacophoran ancestor about 500 million years ago, during the late Cambrian (see also Allcock et al., 2015). The early Devonian saw the rise of the ammonites and nautiloids (Kröger et al., 2011), both still with chambered shells. The greatest structural innovation, the internalization of the cephalopod shell, likely occurred in the Permian or Carboniferous (Smith and Caron, 2010; Kröger et al., 2011; Tanner et al., 2017; Klug et al., 2019), exposing the mantle for the first time and providing a possible significant boost to their evolution, including cognitive abilities (Packard, 1972; Amodio et al., 2019a,b).

Cephalopods evolved several innovations, the most intriguing perhaps being their capability of exhibiting rapid and neurally-controlled changes in their body patterning (Packard and Hochberg, 1977; Packard, 1988; Messenger, 2001; Borrelli et al., 2006), and a large and complex nervous system (review in: Nixon and Young, 2003; Shigeno et al., 2018).

During their evolution the brain of cephalopods increased its complexity reaching the maximum agglomeration of the neural masses, as exemplified by comparing the outline of the “central nervous system” of Nautilus and that of Octopus vulgaris (Young, 1965, 1971). This resulted after the addition or loss of ganglia (molluscan origin and plan) that brought about the change in position and relative volume, achieving features considered to be unusual to molluscan, and invertebrate or even vertebrate, standards, but allowing significant functional analogies with vertebrates (see: Bullock, 1965; Young, 1971, 1974, 1976, 1977b, 1979; Messenger, 1979; Budelmann B., 1995; Nixon and Young, 2003; Shigeno et al., 2018; see also Supplementary Information).

These animals have been the preeminent “model” for cephalopod developmental (Naef, 1928), neurophysiological (e.g., Keynes, 1989; Pozzo-Miller et al., 1998; Brown and Piscopo, 2013) and behavioral studies (review in e.g., Huffard, 2013), including an early systematic attempt to develop a model of the brain (Young, 1964; review in Marini et al., 2017). Furthermore, cephalopods (and maybe octopuses especially) exhibit advanced cognitive faculties paralleling mammalian capabilities (Edelman and Seth, 2009; Amodio et al., 2019b). Cephalopods also provide a very interesting case study for the evolution of novelties/innovations in Metazoa (for review see for example: Shigeno et al., 2018; Zarrella et al., 2019; Albertin and Simakov, 2020). These innovations originated through an increase in genome complexity linked to polyploidy, differential arrangements of key genes, exceptional RNA editing capacities, and expansion of transposable elements, to cite some (e.g., Packard and Albergoni, 1970; De Marianis et al., 1979; Lee et al., 2003; Liscovitch-Brauer et al., 2017; Zarrella et al., 2019; Albertin and Simakov, 2020).

Cephalopods have the largest and most complex invertebrate nervous system. During evolution, the “brain” was assembled through the fusion of a number of molluscan ganglia to form lobes connected to the periphery by many nerve trunks regulating the arms, viscera and other parts of the animal's body. Although their nervous system is confined to the basic molluscan form comprising a set of (five to) six pairs of ganglia, it has a complexity akin to that of lower vertebrates (Bullock, 1965; Budelmann B. U., 1995), but with important functional analogies when compared with higher vertebrates (Shigeno et al., 2018). In addition, cephalopods are known for a brain-to-body weight ratio that exceeds that of fishes and reptiles (Packard, 1972). These features correlate with the sophisticated sensory equipment and complex behavior that cephalopods display (for review see for example: Budelmann et al., 1997; Williamson and Chrachri, 2004; Hanlon and Messenger, 2018).

The central nervous system of cephalopods is characterized by a high level of organization and is therefore considered to be a “proper” brain, which is unusual by molluscan, invertebrate, and even vertebrate standards (Young, 1967; Budelmann B. U., 1995; Hochner et al., 2006; Shigeno et al., 2018) for: (i) the highest degree of centralization compared with any other mollusc or invertebrate (insects excluded), achieved by the shortening of the connectives; (ii) the presence of very small neurons (3–5 micron of nuclear size) acting as local interneurons (Young, 1971, 1991); (iii) the reported absence of somatotopy (except for the chromatophore lobes) contrary to what appears to be the case for the insect or vertebrate brain (Plän, 1987; Zullo, 2004; Zullo et al., 2009); (iv) a blood-brain barrier, an exception for molluscs (Abbott and Pichon, 1987); (v) compound field potentials, similar to those of vertebrate brains (e.g., Bullock and Budelmann, 1991; Williamson and Chrachri, 2004; for review see Brown and Piscopo, 2013); (vi) an elevated efferent innervation of the receptors (e.g., the retina, the equilibrium receptor organs); (vii) peripheral first order afferent neurons (see: Young, 1971, 1991; Brown and Piscopo, 2013); (viii) a large variety of putative transmitters (review in Messenger, 1996).

The greatest centralization among cephalopods is found in the octopodiforms. It is achieved by the shortening of the connectives between the superior buccal and brachial lobes (Nixon and Young, 2003). In contrast, Nautilus has the simplest central nervous system characterized by three broad bands that are joined laterally, one dorsal (i.e., cerebral ganglia and commissure) and two ventral (i.e., pedal, anterior; palliovisceral, posterior) to the esophagus (Owen, 1832; Young, 1965).

During its evolution, the cephalopod brain increased in complexity and, in Coleoidea, became completely surrounded by a cartilaginous capsule. It attained maximum aggregation of the neural masses by fusing the supra– and suboesophageal regions, enclosed in a cartilaginous cranium, alongside expansion of the two large optic lobes (positioned behind the eyes) which extend laterally from the supraoesophageal mass. The change in position and relative volume of the different sections of the brain occurred as a result of the addition or loss of ganglia.

The neural mass forming the “brain” is subdivided into varying numbers of lobes in different species (from 12 in nautiluses to 24 in octopods, excluding the optic lobes).

Neuroanatomically the central nervous system varies between different cephalopod genera. Previous authors show that distinct differences exist between octopods and decapods, which correlate well with the different anatomies. The grades of complexity of the brain parallel the complexity of the sensory inputs received and the different behaviors controlled and exhibited (Young, 1977a; Maddock and Young, 1987; Budelmann B. U., 1995). Overall, the octopod brain is more centralized that the decapod brain i.e., its brachial and pedal lodes are joined, and the superior buccal lobe is united with the inferior frontal lobes. In addition, the brachial and pedal lobes of octopods, and their inferior frontal lobe system, are larger, reflecting the sophisticated use of their arms and tactile learning. Decapods, in contrast, have larger basal lobes and a larger, unfolded, vertical lobe. Their inferior frontal lobe system is simpler, and they have no suprabrachial commissure. Decapods also possess a ventral magnocellular commissure which is not found in octopods.

At the beginning of the twenty-first century, Clark et al. (2001) introduced the concept of a cerebrotype, defining it as a species-by-species measure of brain composition. This despite the fact that a number of studies, antecedent to Clark and coworkers, recognized that different groups of vertebrates possess specific patterns of brain composition that vary among clades and ecological niches.

Cerebrotypes have been shown, in one form or another, across a range of mammals and birds (e.g., Clark et al., 2001; Lundmark, 2001; Burish et al., 2004; Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005; Willemet, 2012; Lewitus et al., 2014; Hamodeh et al., 2017), amphibians and fish (e.g., Charvet et al., 2010; Sylvester et al., 2010; Yopak, 2012). The degree to which phylogeny and ecology relate to species-specific cerebrotypes varies among studies and taxa examined. Despite the demonstration of cerebrotypes in vertebrates, no such analyses have been performed in invertebrates, to the best of our knowledge.

Here we attempt to explore such a possibility.

Several studies have provided a considerable amount of quantitative data on the brains of cephalopod molluscs (Wirz, 1959; Frösch, 1971; Maddock and Young, 1987). Nixon and Young's effort (lasting 30 years) to collect and compare the “brains and lives” of cephalopods further stimulated interest in this field of research (Nixon and Young, 2003).

Wirz, however, was the first to compare quantitative data of the brain of 34 species of cephalopods although her pioneering study was restricted to sub-adult and adult individuals from the Mediterranean Sea (Wirz, 1959). Frösch (1971) extended Wirz's work by calculating the volumes of the brain lobes in “Schlüpfstadien” (i.e., hatchlings) of ten species of Mediterranean cephalopods. Finally, Maddock and Young (1987) assembled the largest data set available on quantitative information of the brain in cephalopods, determining the volumes of the lobes of the brain for 63 cephalopod species. Like Wirz (1959) and Frösch (1971), the values were expressed as percentages of brain volume, but in addition to the two previous studies, Maddock and Young utilized species from more varied locations, including several deep-sea forms.

In our view, cerebrotypes are identifiable in this taxon. Their evolution could be related to a number of factors. First, phylogenetic constraints could largely dictate brain composition. In this case, closely related species should have a similar brain composition or architecture. Second, developmental constraints could exert the strongest influence on cephalopod brain composition. Constraining factors could include the developmental state of hatchlings, whether or not the species undergoes metamorphosis, the habitat that the eggs are deposited on, to mention some. Third, behavior and ecology could be instrumental in determining cephalopod “cerebrotypes” such that species occupying similar niches exhibit similar brain composition. Similarity reflecting niche type has been found in vertebrates (e.g., Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2009b; Schuppli et al., 2016; Hamodeh et al., 2017; Kamhi et al., 2019) and it is reasonable to assume that it could also occur in Cephalopoda, the invertebrates with the highest degree of brain centralization.

In this study we focus on the third factor, behavior and ecology. We aim to relate the cephalopod cerebrotypes to their “adaptive” characters and niches that they occupy. Although we recognize that the other two constraints, phylogenetic, and developmental, could play an important role in the “evolution” of cerebrotypes, these aspects are not addressed in our study. Here we analyzed existing data available for cephalopod brain organization and considered the relative size of five major brain “functional” areas in relation to ecological variables in different species.

Materials and Methods

Data Set

Quantitative data of the brain of various cephalopod species were obtained from the three aforementioned studies (Wirz, 1959; Frösch, 1971; Maddock and Young, 1987; see Table 1) and compiled to build the “brain” dataset (see Supplementary Table 3). Nautilus was excluded a priori because of the differences in the nervous system with respect to that of coleoids (Young, 1965; for review see also: Budelmann B. U., 1995; Nixon and Young, 2003). Sepiola sp. and Sepietta petersi (included in Frösch, 1971) were also excluded as the data for these two species were based on juvenile specimens.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. List of species (√, N = 81) included in the three published reports including quantitative data of brains in cephalopods (Wirz, 1959; Frösch, 1971; Maddock and Young, 1987).

The final data set comprised 78 species, grouped in 33 families and six orders.

The main issue with volumetric analysis in cephalopod brains is the variation in the volume of the brain, and of the single lobes within it, with the size and age of the individual (e.g., Packard and Albergoni, 1970; Frösch, 1971; Shigeno et al., 2001a). In addition, there is a general consensus that there is not a cephalopod “reference” or “type” body size at maturity, as occurs in many vertebrate species (see also Discussion). To circumvent these problems, we utilized only the a-dimensional measurements (percentages) of the different sections of the brain from Wirz (1959) and Maddock and Young (1987).

Maddock and Young (1987) grouped single brain lobes into functional sets, allowing for comparisons between taxa with different numbers of lobes. Wirz (1959) however did not consider the lobes of the brain in terms of functional sets.

In order to combine the data included in the two papers we (i) searched for correspondence between lobes, (ii) grouped each lobe in its functional set, and (iii) summed-up the values of the lobes per functional set.

Eight functional sets and their corresponding relative brain sizes were identified (Supplementary Table 2).

The values of the different functional sets were recalculated as proportions relative to the volume of the whole brain (i.e., the sum of supra- and sub-esophageal masses and optic lobes), without altering the order of magnitude of the data within and between functional sets and providing values that prevented overemphasizing certain values, such as the volume of the optic lobes, in the standardization procedures required by the assumptions of the clustering technique (Everitt, 1993; Everitt et al., 2001).

In order to circumvent the intrinsic differences in the brain size values of the two data sets, we arbitrarily chose the data of Maddock and Young (1987) for species in common to both papers rather than calculating the average of the percentages given in the two works.

Ecological Variables

In order to relate the relative size of the different brain areas to ecological and life-history of different cephalopod species, we collated information available on the ecology, distribution, life history, behavior, morphological adaptations and reproductive strategies (see Supplementary Information for details) of the 78 taxa for which we had brain size data. In particular, we considered:

i. Method of locomotion: as indicator of the potential of a species to spread and adapt to new environments;

ii. Feeding habits: whether a species has adapted to become a generalist or a specialist;

iii. Development: as a potential indicator or the relative dispersal of hatchlings following spawning;

iv. Reproduction: i.e., mating/spawning;

v. Habitat: i.e., vertical and horizontal distribution, that potentially affects gene flow and dispersal.

In total, more than 15 categories of data counting a total of 130 variables constituted the final matrix of life-habits data. These data were utilized as life-adaptation descriptors of the species considered (See Supplementary Information and Supplementary Table 3).

Analyses of Data

The combination of relative brain size data and “ecological variables” for each of the 78 cephalopod species herein considered (Supplementary Table 3), represents our database, i.e., a multi-dimensional matrix including data on the diversity of brain lobe' size and life history attributes of 78 species representing different cephalopod families.

Data analysis followed approaches included in Zar (1999) and Everitt et al. (2001). In brief, we utilized principal component analysis (PCA) to reduce the dimensions of the anatomical data (i.e., eight functional sets), followed by Varimax rotation. The resulting factor scores (regression method, see Gorsuch, 1983) and the ecological variables (see list above and Supplementary Information) were analyzed through a hierarchical cluster analysis using Ward's method (Ward, 1963).

In analogy to similar studies carried out in vertebrates (e.g., Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005), we selected PCA to help reduce the number of variables into “components” thus exploring internal structure of the data and possibly the variance. This is considered a useful method for examining cerebrotypes (see for example discussion in Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005). As mentioned above, we also performed a cluster analysis. We selected Ward's Hierarchical method (Ward, 1963) as a general agglomerative hierarchical clustering procedure, where the criterion for choosing the pair of clusters to merge at each step is based on the optimal value of an objective function (sensu Ward, 1963). This is a method utilized in several “social” science and behavioral studies, and has the advantage of not being related to any “phylogeny” (as in our case), but linked to the variability/characteristic and structure of data.

Non parametric analysis of variance (Kruskal Wallis test) followed by post hoc pairwise comparisons (Dunn, 1964; Zar, 1999) was utilized to compare mean proportions of functional brain sets belonging to species attributed to different clusters.

All statistical analyses were carried out using SPSS (rel. 18.0 PASW—Predictive Analytics SoftWare, IBM, 2010), except for the hierarchical cluster analysis (CLUSTAN, Wishart, 1987).

Phylogenetic PCA and Further Analysis of Data

This study is not primarily aimed at finding a direct link between brain diversity in cephalopods by means of a phylogenetic analysis. However, to attempt to control for phylogenetic dependence/independence of traits here considered (brains' diversity) and possibly ruling out bias in detecting relationships and inaccurate estimates of correlations (Rezende and Diniz-Filho, 2012; see also Adams and Collyer, 2017) we ran phylogenetic principal component analysis (pPCA, Revell, 2009). This aims to explore association between brain volume proportions and species by taking into account the phylogenetic relationship between species.

We utilized as reference tree (source of “phylogenetic signal” for our data) the multigene phylogeny based on maximum likelihood analysis by Lindgren et al. (2012); this is considered one of the most complete cephalopod phylogeny published so far. The original tree comprises 188 taxa (see Figure 1 in Lindgren et al., 2012).

Despite the large number of species included by Lindgren et al. (2012) only 38 species overlapped with the list of 78 cephalopods considered in our dataset (see Table 1 and Supplementary Table 6).

The phylogenetic PCA was carried out by pruning the original tree by Lindgren et al. (2012) to obtain one including exclusively the 38 species common to both datasets (Supplementary Figure 1). The pruned tree was utilized as input phylogeny for “phyloPCA” included in the phytools package (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=phytools; September, 2020). For phyloPCA we set: method = “lambda,” mode = “corr,” rotate = “varimax.” Data for the brain functional sets were included, for corresponding species.

After pPCA, scores for the first three components were extracted, followed by a further cluster analysis (see Supplementary Information section: “Considerations taken for the phylogenetic PCA and subsequent analysis”).

Results

Cephalopod Relative Brain Size

Figure 1 presents an overview of the different proportions of brain areas (i.e., functional sets; see also Supplementary Table 2) in the 78 species of cephalopods considered. We also include a stacked bar summary of the relative proportions of brain functional sets in different species, excluding the contribution of the optic lobes (Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proportions of the eight functional sets on the whole brain (A) in the 78 species of cephalopods considered in this study (see also Supplementary Table 3 for values). The functional sets (see Supplementary Table 2 for details) are color coded. For supraoesophageal mass: INFF (yes), VERT (yes), BASAL (yes); for suboesophageal mass: BRAC (yes), PEDAL (yes), PALL (yes), CHRF (yes); for optic lobes (OPTIC, yes). (B) 100% Stack Bar graph of the relative proportions of the functional brain sets excluding OPTIC, to illustrate variability of brain areas identified in supra- and suboesophageal masses between species.

From these data PCA allowed three components to be identified, which together account for 87.3% of the total variance (Table 2). The first component (62.3% variance) is positively correlated with “inferior frontal” (INFF), “brachial” (BRAC), “pallial” (PALL) and “pedal” (PEDAL) lobe functional sets, but negatively correlated with the optic lobe. The second component accounts for roughly 13% of the variance and is correlated with “basal” (BASAL) and “chromatophore” (CHRF) lobes. The vertical lobe system (VERT) appears in the third component, which explains 12% of the total variance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Principal components analysis for the relative proportions of the eight brain-functional sets (for abbreviations and description see Supplementary Table 2) of the 78 species of cephalopods considered in this study (see also Borrelli, 2007).

These results confirm the view that cephalopods possess largely diversified brains; a diversity particularly marked when the PCA scores were plotted against the six orders included in the data set (Figure 2; see also Table 3). Myopsid and oegopsid species included in this study are best separated by the second component (BASAL and CHRF), possibly due to the large contribution of the basal lobe system and chromatophore and fin lobes to the relative proportion of brain areas in these species. For similar reasons, octopods are widely distributed along the first component (correlated with the inferior frontal, brachial, pallial and pedal lobes, and negatively with the optic lobes), with the exception of two species (Cirroteuthis sp. and Cirrothauma murrayi) which are also widely separated by the second component, and therefore may be considered as outliers. This result may be linked to the fact that the two species had (in our data set, and to the best of our knowledge) the smallest optic lobes (38.6 and 13.5% of the brain, respectively; Supplementary Table 3; see also Maddock and Young, 1987). We can speculate that the vertical lobe system occupies a distinct component (the third) because of the large variability in relative proportions, within orders, of this structure: its size varies “more than four times among the decapods and six times in octopods” (Maddock and Young, 1987, p. 749).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Scatter plot of factor score values (after regression following PCA, Gorsuch, 1983) of the 78 cephalopod species belonging to the six Orders (color-coded) considered in this study. Different symbols group various taxa for a given order, whenever applicable (see Supplementary Information and Supplementary Table 4 for details). Cirrata and Incirrata are coded with different grades of green. Only the first two factor scores are considered here. See text for details and Table 3 for relative proportions of cephalopod brains between species allocated in different clusters.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The mean proportions of the eight brain regions (brain-functional sets) for each of the ten clusters identified after the hierarchical cluster analysis (see Figure 3).

Correlating Cephalopod Cerebrotypes Their Life-Styles and Other Adaptations

To search for any possible relationship between the components extracted from the PCA depicting brain diversity in cephalopod and cephalopod' life adaptation descriptors (the “ecological” variables; see Supplementary Information) of the various organisms we considered, we carried out a hierarchical cluster analysis as an attempt to summarize patterns of similarity/dissimilarity among species.

The clustering was carried out only on 52 species out of the original list (n = 78): 26 species were excluded for missing data (233 null values, 2.3% of the whole dataset) in 32 of the 130 total number of variables/states included as indicators of cephalopod “life-style/adaptations.” We cannot ignore that the 26 species not included in the cluster analysis might have provided a different grouping and/or the identification of additional clusters (see discussion around missing data in Supplementary Information “Number of species included in the final clustering and reasons for exclusions”).

The cluster analysis yielded a dendrogram with ten distinct clusters (labeled from 1 to 10 in Figure 3). Table 3 summarizes mean values of the proportions for each of the brain regions belonging to the groups of species identified through hierarchical clustering to illustrate differences on the resulting “cerebrotype.”

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dendrogram after Ward's hierarchical clustering showing the relationships among cephalopod species (N = 52) based upon the data set (N = 78). The clusters recognized are numbered from 1 to 10 and described in the text. See also Table 3 and Supplementary Table 5.

A short description for each cluster follows in the following paragraphs. Hereunder, the species are referred according to original names as indicated by Authors (Wirz, 1959; Maddock and Young, 1987); for current valid taxonomy refer to Table 1.

The first group of species (Cluster 1) consists of a very diverse assemblage of species (i.e., Spirula spirula, Heteroteuthis dispar, Brachioteuthis riisei, Abralia veranyi, Abraliopsis morisi), belonging to three distinct orders of cephalopods (Spirulida, Sepiolida, Teuthida). The supraoesophageal mass of these species is characterized by a very small “inferior frontal” (INFF), medium-sized VERT, and relatively large “basal” (BASAL) lobes. The functional brain sets pertaining to the suboesophageal mass are, on average, very similar within the species included in this cluster. The chromatophore lobe (the fin lobe is the major contributor to CHRF in most of these species) is almost absent.

Species in this cluster possess relatively large optic lobes representing about 65% of total brain size.

All five species are known to be oceanic (mesopelagic), achieving buoyancy by active swimming (dynamic lift) with the aid of broad fins, with the exception of Spirula which has near-neutral buoyancy with a chambered shell and short, subterminal fins. Their geographic distribution is variable, ranging from 3 to 10 Large Marine Ecosystems (LME; see Sherman and Duda, 1999 see also Supplementary Information), and, as they are reported to feed almost exclusively on pelagic crustaceans, the species should be considered diet specialists, although the diet is slightly richer in H. dispar (in H. dispar, the lower beak is characterized by a medium-narrow rostrum, curved hood, shallow or absent notch, and obtuse or right jaw angle). Finally, this group clusters the only species (from the 78 analyzed) with a “Sthenoteuthis type” reproductive strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002) and multiple spawners sensu Rocha et al. (2001). The eggs are small, single, unencapsulated and pelagic (laid at or near the surface).

Cluster 2 groups the three Sepia species: S. officinalis, S. elegans, and S. orbignyana. In this case, the supraoesophageal mass is characterized by a very small inferior lobe system, and large vertical and basal lobes. Typical of the decapod subesophageal mass, the brachial lobe is not particularly developed (PEDAL and PALL being on average similar to the other clusters). However, in these species prominent chromatophore (about 2%) and fin (3%) lobes exist, accompanied by moderately large optic lobes (about 60% of brain size). All three species are reported as having “near-neutral buoyancy” via a chambered shell, with fringed fins. They live in coastal, benthic habitats (mainly littoral and continental shelf), and are quite widely distributed (occurring in 10 LMEs on average). Cuttlefish are known to have a relatively broad diet breadth (i.e., generalists), and the lower beaks are characterized by a long and broad rostrum, curved hood, shallow notch, and curved jaw angle. All the three species have an “Illex-type” reproductive strategy (sensu Nesis, 2002) and are intermittent terminal spawners (sensu Rocha et al., 2001). The eggs are mostly intermediate in size, single, encapsulated and laid in batches on the bottom.

The third group (Cluster 3; Figure 3) includes all benthic bobtail squids in our dataset: Sepiola rondeletii, S. affinis, S. robusta, Sepietta oweniana, and Rossia macrosoma. In these species the supraesophageal mass is characterized by a relatively small INFF (the exception being S. rondeleti), large BASAL and moderately large VERT. Within the suboesophageal mass, the PEDAL and PALL are prominent in comparison to other decapods. The chromatophore and fin lobes are similar in size to those observed in Sepia species (see Cluster 2). The five species included in Cluster 3 possess moderately large optic lobes (about 60% of the entire brain). They are considered to be “dense” (see Supplementary Information and Supplementary Table 3), bottom living, finned species, inhabiting both coastal waters (littoral and continental shelf) and the slope (bathybenthic). Sepiola rondeletii, S. oweniana, and R. macrosoma occupy a moderately extended geographical range while S. affinis and S. robusta are known to be distributed within a slightly narrower range. Data available to us suggest the diet is restricted to crustaceans (with the exception of Rossia); the lower beak being characterized by a long and moderately broad rostrum, curved hood, absent notch, and obtuse jaw angle. The life-style and reproductive strategies (sensu: Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; Nesis, 2002), spawning pattern and egg morphology of the five species is the same as for cuttlefish (as for Cluster 2).

Cluster 4 comprises six coastal loliginids (Loligo vulgaris, L. forbesii, L. (Alloteuthis) media, L. (Alloteuthis) subulata1, Lolliguncula brevis, and Sepioteuthis sepioidea). To compensate for their dense body tissues, they swim actively (dynamic lift), by flapping their elongated fins to remain afloat. These squids inhabit neritic and shallow waters (epi-mesopelagic). Species included in this cluster are widely distributed across LMEs; S. sepioidea and L. (Alloteuthis) media are reported to have more restricted distributions. The supraoesophageal mass is characterized by a small inferior frontal system, with the exception of L. (Alloteuthis) media and L. brevis for which proportions are about four times those of the other species in the group. The species are reported to have large vertical lobes (as compared with other species) and basal lobes of variable relative size. Within this cluster the chromatophore lobes are smaller than the larger fin lobe (together reaching about 6% of the cerebral masses). The optic lobes are comparatively large, ranging between 57 and 78% of the total brain size. Diet is diverse among species, but is generally considered to be intermediate in breadth with L. vulgaris and L. forbesii having the most diverse diet, and L. brevis the most specialist diet. The lower beaks of these species have a long, broad rostrum, curved hood, shallow notch, and obtuse jaw angle. The species reproduce and spawn following similar strategies (see above and Supplementary Information) to the other neritic species (sepiids and sepiolids) although the eggs are laid on the bottom in collective capsules rather than singular capsules.

Cluster 5 groups six squids which occupy the same zone (epi-mesopelagic) as loliginids, but which are oceanic. This cluster includes the most voracious cephalopod species, such as the giant squid (Architeuthis dux), the muscular, flying squids (Illex illecebrosus, I. coindetii, Todarodes sagittatus, Todaropsis eblanae), and the common clubhook squid (Onychoteuthis banksii). They occupy moderately wide geographic distributions (O. banksii with a particularly broad distribution). Our data sources describe these species as feeding exclusively on pelagic organisms such as crustaceans, fish, and mollusks (including cephalopods). The lower beaks have a long, narrow rostrum (except for A. dux), curved hood, and acute (or right) jaw angle. They are fast swimmers (with broad triangular fins) and move using dynamic lift (because they are dense), with the exception of A. dux (near-neutral; chemical lift; ammonium). The supraoesophageal mass has a relatively small “inferior frontal” (INFF), moderately variable in size VERT and basal lobes; chromatophore lobes quite reduced when compared with the fin lobe (with the exception of O. banksii in which they are equivalent in size), and optic lobes representing about 60% of the total brain size. The reproductive strategy (sensu: Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; Nesis, 2002) of these species is similar to that of loliginid squids (see Cluster 4), with the exception of A. dux, which like other “ammoniacal” squids, reproduces with a “Gonatus-type” strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002).

The sixth group of species (Cluster 6) includes the chiroteuthid squids (Chiroteuthis veranyi, Grimalditeuthis bonplandi) and four Cranchiidae (Taonius pavo, Galiteuthis glacialis, Megalocranchia sp., Teuthowenia megalops); the other Cranchiidae species here considered grouped in Cluster 7. The six species are “near-neutral” and achieve buoyancy by chemical lift (ammonium)2. Their fins are short, rounded and subterminal (as in the transparent glass squids) or secondary (chiroteuthids). They are “oceanic” (epi-bathypelagic), with a quite variable geographic distribution, with the exception of T. pavo. The supraoesophageal mass has the smallest VERT and BASAL compared with both muscular (see clusters 4 and 5) and glass squids (see Cluster 7). The chromatophore lobe is reduced or absent while the fin lobe is relatively developed, when compared with species included in Cluster 7 (see also: Maddock and Young, 1987; Nixon and Young, 2003). The optic lobes are large, representing about 80% of the entire “brain.” Diet breadth (hypothetical for many species) is listed in our data set as relatively wide, restricted to pelagic prey items (i.e., cephalopods, crustaceans, fish). The lower beak is reported as short but broad rostrum, curved hood, broad notch and obtuse jaw angle. All six species are reported to reproduce with an “Gonatus-type” strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002) and are intermittent terminal spawners (sensu Rocha et al., 2001). The eggs are small (the size is hypothetical for many species) and are released as collective capsules in deep layers of the water column.

Cluster 7 groups the six remaining Cranchiidae species (Cranchia scabra, Leachia pacifica, Helicocranchia papillata, Bathothauma lyromma, Sandalops melancholicus, Egea inermis). They occupy the same area of the marine realm as Cluster 6, and have similar modes of locomotion, reproductive and spawning patterns, egg morphology, and site of deposition. The supraoesophageal mass is characterized by the smallest INFF, but larger VERT and basal lobes compared with other cranchiids. The chromatophore lobe is reduced or absent, the fin lobe is relatively small (except in C. scabra); the optic lobes are fairly large, accounting for about 75% of the total brain size. The diet is hypothetical although similar lower beak morphology (variable rostrum, curved hood, variable notch, and obtuse jaw angle) to species included in Cluster 6 would suggest similar diet breadth (see also above).

Armhook squid, Gonatus fabricii, the gelatinous octopuses (Japetella sp., Bolitaena sp.) and the common blanket octopus Tremoctopus violaceus are included in Cluster 8. These species are finless except for G. fabricii, free-swimming (near-neutral by chemical lift via lipids or chlorine) and live-in open waters, mainly occupying surface or intermediate layers (epi-mesopelagic), although bolitaenids are also reported to live in deeper waters (bathypelagic). All species are reported as widely distributed around the world's oceans (7-17 LMEs; but G. fabricii is currently known as restricted to the North Atlantic). In our data the diet breadth, similar to glass squids, is reported as relatively broad, with a focus on pelagic organisms, such as pteropods (molluscs), amphipods, copepods and euphausiids (crustaceans), chaetognaths and fish. Lower beak morphology is distinctive in these species: a short rostrum, curved or flat hood, notch absent. The supraoesophageal mass is characterized in these species by a small INFF (with the exception of Japetella sp.), small basal lobes and a medium-sized vertical lobe system. The chromatophore and fin lobes are reduced or absent. The optic lobes are large reaching about 70% of the total size of the brain. Japetella sp., Bolitaena sp., and T. violaceus reproduce with an “Octopus-type” strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002) and are simultaneous terminal spawners (sensu Rocha et al., 2001), while G. fabricii reproduces with a “Gonatus-type” strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002) and is an intermittent terminal spawner (sensu Rocha et al., 2001). Recent studies reported that G. fabricii exhibit a geographically localized reproduction, relatively uncommon for deep-water squids (Golikov et al., 2019).

Cluster 9 groups seven species of benthic octopuses: Octopus vulgaris, O. bimaculatus, O. defilippi3, O. macropus3, “Octopus” salutii, Enteroctopus dofleini, and Pteroctopus tetracirrhus. All of them are “dense,” finless, bottom living species inhabiting coastal waters (littoral and continental shelf, with only O. macropus, O. salutii and P. tetracirrhus extending to bathybenthic layers). Octopus vulgaris, O. bimaculatus, O. defilippi, O. macropus3 have a wide geographical distribution; O. bimaculatus, O. salutii, P. tetracirrhus, and E. dofleini occupy a more restricted range (but see also Jereb et al., 2016).

The supraoesophageal mass is characterized by a large “inferior frontal” (INFF) and vertical lobe systems (VERT; although this is quite variable among species) and a well-developed chromatophore lobe (fin lobe absent). The optic lobes are moderately large as they represent about 50% of the entire brain. Octopus vulgaris is reported to be a generalist species, while the other taxa in this cluster are described in our dataset with a more restricted diet4. The lower beak is described with a short-broad rostrum, narrow curved hood, broad notch, and obtuse jaw angle. All seven species reproduce following an “Octopus-type” strategy (sensu Nigmatullin and Laptikhovsky, 1994; see also Nesis, 2002) and are simultaneous terminal spawners (sensu Rocha et al., 2001). The eggs, small-intermediate in size, are laid in clusters on the substrate (e.g., females of O. vulgaris lay their egg strings in their den).

Cluster 10 groups the remaining octopods: Eledone moschata, E. cirrhosa, Bathypolypus sponsalis, Benthoctopus piscatorum5. These species (like in Cluster 9) are also benthic (dense body tissues) and finless. However, they are known to occupy deeper layers than the species included in Cluster 9; particularly an exclusively bathybenthic distribution is known for B. sponsalis and B. piscatorum. In contrast to Cluster 9, these species occupy a relatively restricted geographical distribution (E. moschata and B. sponsalis being even more restricted).

The supraoesophageal mass is characterized by large INFF, VERT and basal lobes. The chromatophore lobe is moderately large; the fin lobe is absent. The optic lobes are the smallest in our dataset: 26-33% of the total brain size; 45% for E. cirrhosa. The diet is reported as specialized6, with the exception of E. cirrhosa. The lower beak is described as short, with broad rostrum, variable curved hood, broad notch, and acute or recessed jaw angle. The reproductive and spawning strategies of the two Eledone species are comparable to those of shallow water octopods (see cluster 9). Contrarily, the deep-sea octopuses (B. sponsalis and B. piscatorum) are reported to be continuous spawners in our source of data. There is no consensus on the spawning pattern of deep-sea octopuses due to lack of data. The eggs of all four species are large (when compared with other octopods), and are in clusters which are laid on substrate.

At first glance, the dendrogram (Figure 3) reveals the strong division of coleoid cephalopods into Decapodiformes (clusters 1–7) and Octopodiformes (clusters 8–10), which corresponds to a high dissimilarity index (Squared Euclidean Distance coefficient = 2579.3). The sole exception to this general pattern is represented by the oegopsid squid, Gonatus fabricii, which is clustered with pelagic octopuses. As already described (see Cluster 8), this association may be explained by the fact that Armhook squids share with free-swimming octopuses the common behavior of females brooding eggs on their arms, instead of releasing them in the water column (as do other oceanic squids) or laying them on the ground (as do neritic squids). However, a more attentive analysis of the figure suggests that the separation of coleoids in two distinct lineages (i.e., decapods vs. octopods) is less clear-cut than expected. The relative affinity or relatedness among species—within and between clusters—seems largely to depend on the life adaptations they share in common (e.g., buoyancy mechanisms, habitats occupied, reproductive strategies) that, in turn, has brought about a similar differentiation in the lobes of the brain, as for our hypothesis.

For example, Bobtail squids (cluster 3) and inshore squids (cluster 4) are grouped together because they share several features in common. They both live in coastal waters (although sepiolids may reach deeper layers of the water column), have “dense” bodies and fins (see Supplementary Table 3). Both sepiolids and loliginids are intermittent terminal spawners (Illex-type strategy) so that the eggs are laid, in separate clutches, over a relatively long-time frame (Rocha et al., 2001). The eggs (small-intermediate in size) are laid on the substrate in batches (sepiolids) or collective capsules (loliginids). Moreover, both taxa are reported in our data-set with brains characterized by a very small “inferior frontal,” medium-sized vertical lobe system and considerable basal lobes. The optic lobes are also well developed in these species, representing roughly 60 and 70% of the total brain size in sepiolids and loliginids, respectively. The fin lobe is rather more conspicuous in loliginids which can be explained by their strictly pelagic life style, in contrast to sepiolids live mostly in contact with the bottom.

Differences Between Cephalopod Cerebrotypes as Identified by Hierarchical Cluster Analysis

To illustrate whether the ten clusters identified after Ward's hierarchical method correspond to a characteristic “cerebrotype,” we calculated mean values of the proportions for each of the brain functional sets of species belonging to every cluster identified (Table 3). Differences between the proportions for each of the eight brain regions (brain-functional sets) shown for species belonging to the clusters identified, were significant according to Kruskal–Wallis one-way analysis of variance (Supplementary Table 5). Post hoc pairwise comparisons further confirmed a composite variation of cephalopod brain areas attributed to different clusters (see Supplementary Table 5.2). In particular we found significant differences for INFF (clusters 9 and 10 vs. others), VERT in more than 12 pairwise comparisons (26% of the total), BASAL (cluster 6 vs. 1-3, 5, 10; 2 vs. 7, 8; 3 vs. 6-9, to mention some; see Supplementary Table 5.2) when considering the supraoesophageal mass. Differences between areas also emerged when suboesophageal mass was considered: e.g., PEDAL - cluster 1 vs. 9, 10; cluster 3 vs. 6, 7; cluster 4 vs. 9, 10; cluster 5 vs. 6, 7; cluster 6 vs. 2, 5, 9, 10, cluster 7 vs. 3, 9, 10 (see Supplementary Table 5 for details).

Phylogenetic PCA

The phylogenetic Principal Component Analysis (Revell, 2009) was carried out on a reduced number of species (n = 38; 48.7% of the total) because of missing correspondence between organisms selected by Lindgren et al. (2012) and our dataset.

The resulting first two components accounted for 77% of variance (Eigenvalues, %Variance: PC1 = 5.20, 65.0; PC2 = 0.97, 12%); a third component (eigenvalue = 0.82) was also considered accounting for a total of 88% of cumulative variance (for extracted scores see Supplementary Table 7). Because of pruning and lack of overlap between our dataset and the species considered by Lindgren et al. (2012) a further reduction in the list of species was required for the following cluster analysis (n = 24 out 52; 46% of the species included in Figure 3)7.

The resulting dendrogram (Supplementary Figure 2) shows limited similarities with the clustering of Figure 3.

In brief, the grouping corresponding to clusters 3 and 4 was almost retained, despite some loss in species (i.e., Sepiola affinis, S. robusta, Rossia macrosoma, Loligo vulgaris, Lolliguncula brevis; Supplementary Figure 2), and is nested with Abralia veranyi and Spirula spirula, originally belonging to cluster 1, but within the same branching. Species originally included in cluster 5 appear in a different branch mixing with some other members of cluster 1 (see Supplementary Figure 2). Furthermore, octopuses and cuttlefishes mixed together (with the exception of O. vulgaris), and with different branching when compared with the maximum-likelihood topology as shown by Lindgren et al. (2012).

Discussion

Our study indicates that the cephalopod brain is largely differentiated among species (Figure 1 and Table 3; see also: Maddock and Young, 1987; Nixon and Young, 2003) and evolved specific cerebrotypes in disparate taxa (Table 3 and Supplementary Table 5), similar to what has been reported in vertebrates (e.g., Burish et al., 2004; Iwaniuk et al., 2004; Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005; Yopak, 2012; Kotrschal et al., 2017).

Many comparative studies on brain evolution in vertebrates utilized multivariate statistics (e.g., van Dongen, 1998; Burish et al., 2004; Iwaniuk et al., 2004; Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005; Lisney and Collin, 2006; Gonzalez-Voyer et al., 2009a; Yopak, 2012; Steinhausen et al., 2016; Kotrschal et al., 2017; Mai and Liao, 2019). The approach is useful to investigate brain evolution for two main reasons: (i) there are contingencies among brain regions that result in correlated evolution among some areas; (ii) a multitude of selection pressures and constraints determine the composition and evolution of the brain.

Our data reflect the view of Maddock and Young (1987) that there are significant quantitative differences between the brains of different cephalopod species, and that - despite individual variations due to growth or other factors (e.g., seasonal), these differences should reflect the habitat the cephalopod occupies (as also suggested in Nixon and Young, 2003). Most of the clusters identify cerebrotypes that map on to ecological and/or behavioral similarities among cephalopod species. By using a hierarchical cluster analysis approach, we recognize 10 groups of species that reveal differences and analogies among the 52 cephalopod species included in our final data set. The topology of the relationship among species we observed (see dendrogram in Figure 3) strongly supports J.Z. Young's view (Young, 1977a) and our working hypothesis that analysis combining relative brain size and life strategies may provide the basis for assumptions on the pressures and adaptations that drove cephalopods to evolve.

Evolutionary speculations are beyond our data and approach. However, to control for phylogenetic dependence/independence of traits here considered—i.e., cephalopod brain diversity—and possibly ruling out bias in detecting relationships we ran phylogenetic principal component analysis (Revell, 2009) thus to explore association between brain proportions of different cephalopods taking into account the phylogenetic relationship between species. Our pPCA benefits from the data of Lindgren et al. (2012). Unfortunately, the currently available phylogenetic data and limited correspondence with detailed “brain data” did not allow us to achieve enough resolution in the phylogeny to be utilized as additional information in this work. The data we present have to be considered a preliminary outcome and the basis of future work.

Octopods are characterized by large brachial and inferior frontal lobes and smaller optic lobes as opposed to decapods (see Figures 1, 2; see also Maddock and Young, 1987). It appears evident that differences in relative proportions of the inferior frontal lobe system and brachial lobes of octopuses, as compared with decapods, are largely linked with the large use of arms associated with the benthic habitat (Young, 1977a; see also Hanlon and Messenger, 2018).

Figure 1 highlights a large variability in the proportions of the lobes within decapods (see also Table 3 for differences between cerebrotypes). This was already noticed by Maddock and Young (1987) and is not surprising considering the numerous families included within the taxon. However, the most striking differences among cephalopod species emerged when considering the vertical lobe system (e.g., about four times differences between species included in cluster 6 vs. cluster 2; Table 3). The five lobules of the vertical lobe in O. vulgaris—for example—allow a packing-effect and a volume reduction of the structure by increasing the surface area and the corresponding number of cells counting the lobe (Young, 1963). The small cells (amacrine) also minimize the length of connections, increase connectivity and computational abilities (Young, 1991, 1995; see also Shigeno et al., 2018), and reduce neuropilar space, as occurs in higher vertebrates (e.g., Hofman, 1985; Sherman and Duda, 1999; Hof et al., 2005; Toro and Burnod, 2005; Molnar et al., 2006; Geschwind and Rakic, 2013; Van Essen et al., 2018; Amiez et al., 2019). In Loligo (and Sepia) we find the opposite: there is no folding of the surface of the vertical lobe, an estimated reduced number of cells, a correspondingly huge neuropil (Young, 1979). In the words of J.Z. Young: “The octopod condition seems to favor a large number of small cells, the decapod a large number of large cells. The large cells are numerous even within the neuropil. These differences are very striking and call for further knowledge of fine structure and experiments on function” (Young, 1979, p. 352; see also for example Shomrat et al., 2011).

Figure 3 presents the outcome of the hierarchical cluster analysis and highlights several common features, for example, between cuttlefish (cluster 2) and shallow-water octopuses (cluster 9), which probably reflect similar adaptations to the environment and/or common mechanisms evolved to counteract predation. Both are reported to have a wide distribution and to have colonized mainly coastal waters of both temperate and tropical regions, although cuttlefish are completely absent from the Americas (but fossil records provide evidence of their existence in those areas). As bottom living organisms, both have had to adapt to various types of substrate (e.g., rubble, rocky reefs, open sand plains) and prey (generalists). The “ecological” demands, in terms of relative habitat complexity and predation pressure, have brought to the evolution of cephalopods in both taxa, development of rich behavioral repertoires, complex cognitive capabilities (for review see for example: Marini et al., 2017; Mather and Dickel, 2017; Hanlon and Messenger, 2018) and reproductive strategies (i.e., intermittent and simultaneous terminal spawning; review in Rocha et al., 2001) capable of dealing with unstable environments. Again as an example, it is not surprising that O. vulgaris has the most conspicuous chromatophore lobe (5%) and that S. officinalis is characterized by the largest VERT complex (9.7% of total brain size; see Supplementary Information and Figure 1) among the cephalopods included in this study. Following Maddock and Young (1987) “cirrates are sharply distinguished from other octopods by their relatively large brachial lobes and small vertical lobes. […]” and “among the octopods other than cirrates, the benthic species are distinct from the pelagic, largely on a basis of greater brachial and inferior frontal systems. […] epipelagic and bathypelagic [octopods are] broadly separate [… with] the inferior frontal systems […] reduced and the optic lobes […] large. It would not be legitimate to separate the epipelagic from the bathypelagic octopods on the brains alone […] but there are other features that clearly separate the groups such as the arms and web, […] the whole body form and habitat” (Maddock and Young, 1987, p. 765).

The application of a phylogenetic PCA provided an additional interesting approach. We had access to a large dataset (Lindgren et al., 2012), but the convergence in terms of species included was very limited (38 out 188 species corresponding to 20% of species; only 52% of the species of the dataset of this study were retained for pPCA), limiting the potential of this approach.

Our analyses suggest that the phylogenetic signal alone is not a justification for the grouping of species we found (see Figure 3 and Supplementary Figure 2). Due to the limited set of data available to us, we can only hypothesize that brains evolved in cephalopods on the basis of different factors including phylogeny, development and the third factor (life-style adaptations).

Future research will be required extending the dataset by including all different categories of variables here considered and a strong set of phylogenetic signals as recently applied to decapodiforms by Anderson and Lindgren (2021).

In her original study, Borrelli (2007) attempted to correlate cephalopod' relative brain size with species richness (e.g., Lynch, 1990; Owens et al., 1999; Nicolakakis et al., 2003; Sol et al., 2005; Sayol et al., 2019). Species richness, and possibly subspecies (Sol et al., 2005) appears to be affected by behavioral flexibility, so that taxa appearing more flexible to environmental changes (i.e., opportunistic species) are also those that are represented by a higher number of species as opposed to those characterized by specialist species, and which are less speciose. In a preliminary analysis, Borrelli calculated the total number of species per family in the entire class Cephalopoda noting that species counts were differently distributed among the class with some families more speciose. Borrelli was able to obtain mantle length and brain size of 32 species (a single species per family/subfamily was chosen as representative of the taxon) belonging to 28 families (data deduced from Maddock and Young, 1987). Standardized residuals of brain size8 were regressed against the log transformed values of the number of species per family/subfamily providing a significant relationship between the two (Linear regression: β = 0.31 ± 0.10, F1,31 = 8.97, P = 0.005, R2 = 0.23; see Figure 2.1 in Borrelli, 2007) that—according to the original study—supports the idea of the behavioral drive hypothesis in cephalopods, sensu Wilson (1985): the most speciose cephalopod families were also those having larger brains, as opposed to families with less species. Unfortunately, such an approach was not possible in this work.

During the analysis of the data set and in agreement to what was reported by Borrelli (2007), we faced a problem in attempting to correlate the relative size (volume) of brain areas with body size. A similar issue was also encountered by Maddock and Young (1987) when comparing their data with “previous measurements”; in the words of the Authors “for the optic lobes (which are relatively easy to measure) our figures are about equal to hers [Wirz, 1959, NdA] in eight genera, rather more in four genera and less in nine genera. The only serious discrepancies are for Eledone and Bathypolypus where she records values equivalent to 154 and 94 compared with our 35 and 44. We have checked our figures and, finding no reason to doubt their accuracy, conclude that the differences may be due to differences in the sizes of animals. We intend to undertake a study brain/body sizes in Eledone and other cephalopods, which should help to clear up this point” (Maddock and Young, 1987, p. 762-763). The Authors also extend this remark by comparing with data from Frösch (1971) pointing out that the “vertical lobes were considerably smaller in all the newly-hatched forms than in the adults, but the superior frontals and subverticals were larger […]. The subfrontals, like the vertical lobes, were much smaller in the younger animals. Presubably, they develop with learning experience” (Maddock and Young, 1987, p. 764).

The main reason is that “brain scaling” has to be accurately assessed in cephalopod species given the marked variation in the volume of the brain, and of the single lobes within it, with the size and age of the individual (e.g., Packard and Albergoni, 1970; Frösch, 1971; Dickel et al., 1997, 2006; Shigeno et al., 2001a). As a consequence, it is not possible to identify a “reference” or “type” body size at maturity for cephalopods, as it is in many vertebrate species including fish (e.g., Huber et al., 1997), birds (e.g., Portmann, 1947) and mammals (e.g., Stephan and Pirlot, 1970; Marino, 1998). Therefore, we utilized only a-dimensional measurements (i.e., percentages) of the different sections of the brain from our data sources.

It could be extremely interesting and informative in the future to focus attention on a qualitative assessment of the gross brain morphology in cephalopods. This may help in assessing the degree of inter-specific variability in gross brain structures and in finding potential similarities among morphotypes, other than those that result from comparing the typical decapod vs. octopod brain (i.e., Loligo or Octopus, respectively).

In spite of the wealth of data available from the literature on the organization of the nervous system of cephalopods (e.g., Young, 1971, 1974, 1976, 1977b, 1979; Messenger, 1979; Shigeno et al., 2001a), complete atlases and accurate 3D rendering of the brain morphology are not available (but see Chung et al., 2020). Therefore, a quantitative assessment of gross brain morphology based on the degree of “encephalization” (sensu lato), as recently carried out for example in fish (Lisney and Collin, 2006) is still not possible in cephalopods.

An experimental and data-analysis strategy similar to what has been carried out in vertebrates (e.g., Barton, 1996; Clark et al., 2001; Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005; Kalisinska, 2005; Ratcliffe et al., 2006; Macrì et al., 2019) may reveal important and significant scientific outcomes for cephalopod biology.

Closing Remarks

Despite the intrinsic limitations of our dataset, the results provide support for a close relationship between “cerebrotypes” and life styles in cephalopods. This, again, supports our working hypothesis that this taxon evolved different sensory (and computational) strategies to cope with the demands of life in the ocean (Amodio et al., 2019a,b). These resemble similar adaptations achieved by fish (e.g., Lisney and Collin, 2006) and other vertebrates. By sharing the same environments and ecological niches, octopuses, squids and their allies were forced to compete with fish (their primary predators), which drove cephalopods to colonize and radiate across the world's oceans (Packard, 1972; O'Dor and Webber, 1986; see also, e.g., Aronson, 1991). Our results strongly support Young's view (1977a) of the evolution of the cephalopod brain.

The evolution of cephalopod cerebrotypes could be related to a number of factors. As mentioned in the introduction, phylogenetic (e.g., closely related species should have a similar brain composition) and developmental (e.g., paralarvae or miniature adults at hatching; see for example: Frösch, 1971; Young and Harman, 1988; Sweeney et al., 1992; Shigeno et al., 2001b) constraints could largely dictate brain composition. In addition, behavior and ecology—as a third factor—appeared to influence cephalopod “cerebrotypes”: species occupying similar niches appear to possess similar brain organization/composition.

Here we attempted to relate the cephalopod cerebrotypes to their “adaptive features” and niches that species occupy. We selected only a number of possible variables to consider, based on the data available for the largest number of species included in this study and our aim: a first attempt to provide a revisited glance to the outcome of observational approach originally driven by Young (1977a). We are fully aware that the other two constraints (phylogenetic and developmental) play an important role in the “evolution” of cerebrotypes in this taxon.

A comparison between the dendrograms (Figure 3 and Supplementary Figure 2) and a phylogenetic tree of the species (see Figure 1 in Lindgren et al., 2012), clearly show that the overall clustering pattern is not congruent with phylogeny to some extent, and suggest that many of the clusters reflect similarities in brain and their relation with behavior and ecology. We found relationships between clustering pattern and behavior and ecology analagous to those found in fish (e.g., Huber et al., 1997), birds (e.g., Iwaniuk and Hurd, 2005) and mammals (e.g., de Winter and Oxnard, 2001).

A future effort should focus on testing the interplay between the above-mentioned factors, with a focus on evolution and phylogeny, thus to test whether the aforementioned constraints are independent or interlinked in the overall evolution of cephalopod brains.

Cephalopod molluscs represent a promising group among invertebrates for studies concerning the organizing principles that underlie the architecture and ontogeny of complex brains. In a similar fashion to multivariate analyses of brain composition in other taxa, our study indicates that the cephalopod brain evolved specific cerebrotypes that have evolved in disparate taxa.

Data Availability Statement

All the data presented in this study are included in the paper and in the Supplementary Material. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the animal study because the study is based on historical data obtained from the literature.

Author Contributions

LB built the original dataset. GP and MT drafted the manuscript. AT helped with the phylogenetic PCA analysis. All authors contributed to this study and finalized the manuscript.

Funding

This work has been supported by the Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Part of this work was included in the Ph.D. Thesis of LB (Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn, Italy and Open University, UK). We thank Dr. Annie Lindgren for providing access to the dataset included in Lindgren et al. (2012). We are also grateful to Dr. M. V. Modica for suggestions and Ms. Marisa Barjami for assistance. MT is supported by an Irish Research Council Government of Ireland Postgraduate Scholarship and the Dr. Tony Ryan Research Fund. The study has been supported by the Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn (Napoli, Italy) to GP and GF.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnana.2020.565109/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Current valid species: Alloteuthis media, Alloteuthis subulata.

2. ^Grimalditeuthis also has flotation devices on its tail; see: http://tolweb.org/Grimalditeuthis_bonplandi/19463.

3. ^Current valid species: Macrotritopus defilippi, Callistoctopus macropus.

4. ^But see: Quetglas et al. (2009); Scheel and Anderson (2012); Villegas et al. (2014).

5. ^The species is currently “unaccepted” with accepted name Bathypolypus bairdii (see also Table 1).

6. ^The diet of these species is defined as ‘specialized’, but not restricted, in our original data source. Recent studies expand knowledge available of habits of deep-sea species (e.g., Quetglas et al., 2001; Valls et al., 2017).

7. ^Species retained (in alphabetical order): Abralia veranyi, Architeuthis dux, Chiroteuthis veranyi veranyi, Cranchia scabra, Eledone cirrhosa, Enteroctopus dofleini, Gonatus fabricii, Grimalditeuthis bonplandi, Illex coindetii, Japetella sp, Loligo vulgaris, Lolliguncula brevis, Octopus vulgaris, Onychoteuthis banksii, Rossia macrosoma, Sepia elegans, Sepia officinalis, Sepiola affinis, Sepiola robusta, Spirula spirula, Taonius pavo, Teuthowenia megalops, Todaropsis eblanae, Tremoctopus violaceus violaceus.

8. ^Factoring out body size. Mantle length and total brain volume were log transformed to normalize variances and consequently regressed (Borrelli, 2007).

References

Abbott, N. J., and Pichon, Y. (1987). The glial blood-brain barrier of crustacea and cephalopods: a review. J. Physiol. Paris 82, 304–313.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Adams, D. C., and Collyer, M. L. (2017). Multivariate phylogenetic comparative methods: evaluations, comparisons, and recommendations. Syst. Biol. 67, 14–31. doi: 10.1093/sysbio/syx055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albertin, C. B., and Simakov, O. (2020). Cephalopod biology: at the intersection between genomic and organismal novelties. Ann. Rev. Anim. Biosci. 8, 71–90. doi: 10.1146/annurev-animal-021419-083609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allcock, A. L., Lindgren, A., and Strugnell, J. (2015). The contribution of molecular data to our understanding of cephalopod evolution and systematics: a review. J. Nat. Hist. 49, 1373–1421. doi: 10.1080/00222933.2013.825342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allcock, A. L., Strugnell, J. M., Ruggiero, H., and Collins, M. A. (2006). Redescription of the deep-sea octopod Benthoctopus normani (Massy 1907) and a description of a new species from the Northeast Atlantic. Mar. Biol. Res. 2, 372–387. doi: 10.1080/17451000600973315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amiez, C., Sallet, J., Hopkins, W. D., Meguerditchian, A., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Ben Hamed, S., et al. (2019). Sulcal organization in the medial frontal cortex provides insights into primate brain evolution. Nat. Commun. 10:3437. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-11347-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, P., Boeckle, M., Schnell, A. K., Ostojic, L., Fiorito, G., and Clayton, N. S. (2019a). Shell loss in cephalopods: trigger for, or by-product of, the evolution of intelligence? A Reply to Mollo et al. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 34, 690–692. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amodio, P., Boeckle, M., Schnell, A. K., Ostojíc, L., Fiorito, G., and Clayton, N. S. (2019b). Grow smart and die young: why did cephalopods evolve intelligence? Trends Ecol. Evol. 34, 45–56. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2018.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, F. E., and Lindgren, A. R. (2021). Phylogenomic analyses recover a clade of large-bodied decapodiform cephalopods. Mol. Phylo. Evol. 156:107038. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2020.107038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aronson, R. B. (1991). Ecology, paleobiology and evolutionary constraint in the octopus. Bull. Mar. Sci. 49, 245–255.

Google Scholar

Barton, R. A. (1996). Neocortex size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 263, 173–177.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Borrelli, L. (2007). Testing the Contribution of Relative Brain Size and Learning Capabilities on the Evolution of Octopus vulgaris and Other Cephalopods. Ph.D. Thesis, Stazione Zoologica Anton Dohrn, Italy & Open University, Napoli, United Kingdom.

Google Scholar

Borrelli, L., Gherardi, F., and Fiorito, G. (2006). A Catalogue of Body Patterning in Cephalopoda. Stazione Zoologica A. Dohrn, Firenze University Press, Napoli, United Kingdom.

Google Scholar

Brown, E. R., and Piscopo, S. (2013). Synaptic plasticity in cephalopods; more than just learning and memory? Invert. Neurosci. 13, 35–44. doi: 10.1007/s10158-013-0150-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budelmann, B. (1995). “The cephalopod nervous system: what evolution has made of the molluscan design,” in The Nervous Systems of Invertebrates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Approach, eds O. Breidbach and W. Kutsc (Basel: Springer), 115–138.

Google Scholar

Budelmann, B. U. (1995). “The cephalopod nervous system: what evolution has made of the molluscan design,” in The Nervous Systems of Invertebrates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Approach, eds. O. Breidbach and W. Kutsch. (Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag), 115–138.

Google Scholar

Budelmann, B. U., Schipp, R., and Boletzky, S. V. (1997). “Cephalopoda,” in Microscopic Anatomy of Invertebrates, eds. F. W. Harrison and A. J. Kohn. (New York, NY: Wiley-Liss, Inc.), 119–414.

Google Scholar

Bullock, T. H. (1965). “Mollusca: Cephalopoda,” in Structure and Function in the Nervous Systems of Invertebrates, eds. T. H. Bullock and G. A. Horridge. (San Francisco, CA and London: W.H. Freeman and Company), 1433–1515.

Bullock, T. H., and Budelmann, B. U. (1991). Sensory evoked potentials in unanesthetized unrestrained cuttlefish: a new preparation for brain physiology in cephalopods. J. Comp. Physiol. A 168, 141–150.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Burish, M. J., Kueh, H. Y., and Wang, S. S. H. (2004). Brain architecture and social complexity in modern and ancient birds. Brain Behav. Evol. 63, 107–124. doi: 10.1159/000075674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charvet, C. J., Sandoval, A. L., and Striedter, G. F. (2010). Phylogenetic origins of early alterations in brain region proportions. Brain Behav. Evol. 75, 104–110. doi: 10.1159/000300573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, W.-S., Kurniawan, N. D., and Marshall, N. J. (2020). Toward an MRI-based mesoscale connectome of the squid brain. iScience 23, 100816. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2019.100816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cianchetti, M., Follador, M., Mazzolai, B., Dario, P., and Laschi, C. (2012). “Design and development of a soft robotic octopus arm exploiting embodied intelligence,” in: Robotics and Automation (ICRA), 2012 IEEE International Conference on IEEE (Saint Paul, MN), 5271–5276. doi: 10.1109/ICRA.2012.6224696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. A., Mitra, P. P., and Wang, S. S.-H. (2001). Scalable architecture in mammalian brains. Nature 411, 189–193. doi: 10.1038/35075564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marianis, B., Olmo, E., and Giuditta, A. (1979). Excess DNA in the nuclei of the subseophagel region of octopus brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 186, 293–300. doi: 10.1002/cne.901860211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Winter, W., and Oxnard, C. E. (2001). Evolutionary radiations and convergences in the structural organization of mammalian brains. Nature 409, 710–714. doi: 10.1038/35055547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickel, L., Chichery, M. P., and Chichery, R. (1997). Postembryonic maturation of the vertical lobe complex and early development of predatory behavior in the cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 67, 150–160. doi: 10.1006/nlme.1996.3754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickel, L., Darmaillacq, A. S., Poirier, R., Agin, V., Bellanger, C., and Chichery, R. (2006). Behavioural and neural maturation in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Vie et Milieu 56, 89–95.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dunn, O. J. (1964). Multiple comparisons using rank sums. Technometrics 6, 241–252. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1964.10490181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edelman, D. B., and Seth, A. K. (2009). Animal consciousness: a synthetic approach. Trends Neurosci. 32, 476–484. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B., Landau, S., and Leese, M. (2001). Cluster Analysis. London: Edward Arnold Publishers Ltd.

Google Scholar

Everitt, B. S. (1993). Cluster Analysis. London: Edward Arnold Publishers Ltd.

Google Scholar

Frösch, D. (1971). Quantitative untersuchungen am zentralnervensystem der schlüpfstadien von zehn mediterranen cephalopodenarten. Rev. Suisse Zool 78, 1069–1122.

Geschwind Daniel, H., and Rakic, P. (2013). Cortical evolution: judge the brain by its cover. Neuron 80, 633–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golikov, A. V., Blicher, M. E., Jørgensen, L. L., Walkusz, W., Zakharov, D. V., Zimina, O. L., et al. (2019). Reproductive biology and ecology of the boreoatlantic armhook squid Gonatus fabricii (Cephalopoda: Gonatidae). J. Mollusc. Stud. 85, 341–353. doi: 10.1093/mollus/eyz023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Winberg, S., and Kolm, N. (2009a). Brain structure evolution in a basal vertebrate clade: evidence from phylogenetic comparative analysis of cichlid fishes. BMC Evol. Biol. 9:238. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-9-238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Voyer, A., Winberg, S., and Kolm, N. (2009b). Social fishes and single mothers: brain evolution in African cichlids. Proc. R. Soci. 276, 161–167. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor Analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hamodeh, S., Bozkurt, A., Mao, H., and Sultan, F. (2017). Uncovering specific changes in network wiring underlying the primate cerebrotype. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 3255–3266. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1402-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, R. T., and Messenger, J. B. (2018). Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Hochner, B., Shomrat, T., and Fiorito, G. (2006). The octopus: a model for a comparative analysis of the evolution of learning and memory mechanisms. Biol. Bull. 210, 308–317. doi: 10.2307/4134567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hof, P. R., Chanis, R., and Marino, L. (2005). Cortical complexity in cetacean brains. Anat. Rec. 287A, 1142–1152. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hofman, M. A. (1985). Size and shape of the cerebral cortex in mammals. I. The cortical surface. Brain Behav. Evol. 27, 28–40. doi: 10.1159/000116529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, R., van Staaden, M. J., Kaufman, L. S., and Liem, K. F. (1997). Microhabitat use, trophic patterns, and the evolution of brain structure in African cichlids. Brain Behav. Evol. 50, 167–182. doi: 10.1159/000113330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huffard, C. L. (2013). Cephalopod neurobiology: an introduction for biologists working in other model systems. Invert. Neurosci. 13, 11–18. doi: 10.1007/s10158-013-0147-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N., Dean, K. M., and Nelson, J. E. (2004). A mosaic pattern characterizes the evolution of the avian brain. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. 271, S148–S151. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaniuk, A. N., and Hurd, P. L. (2005). The evolution of cerebrotypes in birds. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 215–230. doi: 10.1159/000084313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jereb, P., Roper, C., Norman, M., and Finn, J. (2016). “Cephalopods of the World,” An Annotated and Illustrated Catalogue of Species Known to Date. Octopods and Vampire Squids. Roma: FAO, Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.

Google Scholar

Kalisinska, E. (2005). Anseriform brain and its parts versus taxonomic and ecological categories. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 244–261. doi: 10.1159/000084315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamhi, J. F., Ilie,ş, I., and Traniello, J. F. A. (2019). Social complexity and brain evolution: comparative analysis of modularity and integration in ant brain organization. Brain Behav. Evol. 93, 4–18. doi: 10.1159/000497267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keynes, R. D. (1989). The role of giant axons in studies of the nerve impulse. BioEssays 10, 90–93. doi: 10.1002/bies.950100213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klug, C., Landman, N. H., Fuchs, D., Mapes, R. H., Pohle, A., Guériau, P., et al. (2019). Anatomy and evolution of the first Coleoidea in the Carboniferous. Commun. Biol. 2:280. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0523-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotrschal, A., Zeng, H.-L., van der Bijl, W., Öhman-Mägi, C., Kotrschal, K., Pelckmans, K., et al. (2017). Evolution of brain region volumes during artificial selection for relative brain size. Evolution 71, 2942–2951. doi: 10.1111/evo.13373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kröger, B., Vinther, J., and Fuchs, D. (2011). Cephalopod origin and evolution: a congruent picture emerging from fossils, development and molecules. BioEssays 33, 602–613. doi: 10.1002/bies.201100001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, P. N., Callaerts, P., de Couet, H. G., and Martindale, M. Q. (2003). Cephalopod Hox genes and the origin of morphological novelties. Nature 424, 1061–1065. doi: 10.1038/nature01872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewitus, E., Kelava, I., Kalinka, A. T., Tomancak, P., and Huttner, W. B. (2014). An adaptive threshold in mammalian neocortical evolution. PLoS Biol. 12:e1002000. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindgren, A. R., Pankey, M. S., Hochberg, F. G., and Oakley, T. H. (2012). A multi-gene phylogeny of Cephalopoda supports convergent morphological evolution in association with multiple habitat shifts in the marine environment. BMC Evol. Biol. 12:129. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-12-129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liscovitch-Brauer, N., Alon, S., Porath, H. T., Elstein, B., Unger, R., Ziv, T., et al. (2017). Trade-off between transcriptome plasticity and genome evolution in cephalopods. Cell 169, 191–202.e111. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisney, T., and Collin, S. (2006). Brain morphology in large pelagic fishes: a comparison between sharks and teleosts. J. Fish Biol. 68, 532–554. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-1112.2006.00940.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundmark, C. (2001). Evolution of mammalian brains. BioScience 51, 992–992. doi: 10.1641/0006-3568(2001)051[0992:Eomb]2.0.Co;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, M. (1990). The rate of morphological evolution in mammals from the standpoint of the neural expectation. Am. Nat 136, 727–741.

Google Scholar

Macrì, S., Savriama, Y., Khan, I., and Di-Poï, N. (2019). Comparative analysis of squamate brains unveils multi-level variation in cerebellar architecture associated with locomotor specialization. Nat. Commun. 10:5560. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13405-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddock, L., and Young, J. Z. (1987). Quantitative differences among the brains of cephalopods. J. Zool. 212, 739–767.

Google Scholar

Mai, C. L., and Liao, W. B. (2019). Brain size evolution in anurans: a review. J. Evol. Biol. 69:265. doi: 10.1163/15707563-00001074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marini, G., De Sio, F., Ponte, G., and Fiorito, G. (2017). “Behavioral analysis of learning and memory in cephalopods,” in Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference (Second Edition), ed. J. H. Byrne. (Amsterdam: Academic Press, Elsevier), 441–462.

Google Scholar

Marino, L. (1998). A comparison of encephalization between odontocete cetaceans and anthropoid primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 51, 230–238. doi: 10.1159/000006540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, J. A., and Dickel, L. (2017). Cephalopod complex cognition. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 16, 131–137. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClain, C. R. (2019). Likes, comments, and shares of marine organism imagery on Facebook. Peer J. 7:e6795. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messenger, J. (1979). The nervous system of Loligo: IV. The peduncle and olfactory lobes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 285, 275–309.

Google Scholar

Messenger, J. B. (1996). Neurotransmitters of cephalopods. Invertebr. Neurosci. 2, 95–114. doi: 10.1007/BF02214113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messenger, J. B. (2001). Cephalopod chromatophores: neurobiology and natural history. Biol. Rev. 76, 473–528. doi: 10.1017/S1464793101005772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molnar, Z., Metin, C., Stoykova, A., Tarabykin, V., Price, D. J., Francis, F., et al. (2006). Comparative aspects of cerebral cortical development. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 921–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04611.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouritsen, O. G., and Styrbæk, K. (2018). Cephalopod gastronomy—a promise for the future. Front. Commun. 3:38. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naef, A. (1928). Die Cephalopoden (Embryologie). Fauna und Flora des Golfes von Neapel. Roma; Berlin: Bardi; R. Friedländer & Sohn.

Google Scholar

Nakajima, R. (2018). “Can I Talk to a Squid? The origin of visual communication through the behavioral ecology of cephalopod,” in Human Interface and the Management of Information. Interaction, Visualization, and Analytics, eds. S. Yamamoto and H. Mori. (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 594–606.

Google Scholar

Nakajima, R., Shigeno, S., Zullo, L., De Sio, F., and Schmidt, M. R. (2018). Cephalopods between science, art, and engineering: a contemporary synthesis. Front. Commun. 3:20. doi: 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesis, K. N. (2002). Life style strategies of recent cephalopods: a review. Bull. Mar. Sci. 71, 561–579.

Google Scholar

Nicolakakis, N., Sol, D., and Lefebvre, L. (2003). Behavioural flexibility predicts species richness in birds, but not extinction risk. Anim. Behav. 65, 445–452. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigmatullin, C. M., and Laptikhovsky, V. V. (1994). Reproductive strategies in the squids of the family Ommastrephidae (preliminary report). Ruthenica 4, 79–82.

Google Scholar

Nixon, M., and Young, J. Z. (2003). The Brains and Lives of Cephalopods. New York, NY: Oxford University.

Google Scholar

O'Dor, R. K., and Webber, D. M. (1986). The constraints on cephalopods: why squid aren't fish. Can. J. Zool. 64, 1591–1605.

Google Scholar

Owen, R. (1832). Memoir on the Pearly Nautilus (Nautilus Pompilius, Linn.). London: Richard Taylor.

Google Scholar

Owens, I. P. F., Bennett, P. M., and Harvey, P. H. (1999). Species richness among birds: body size, life history, sexual selection or ecology? Proceed. R. Soc. Lond. Biol. Sci. 266, 933–939.

Google Scholar

Packard, A. (1972). Cephalopods and fish: the limits of convergence. Biol. Rev. 47, 241–307.

Google Scholar

Packard, A. (1988). “The skin of cephalopods (coleoids): general and special adaptations,” in The Mollusca, eds E. R. Trueman and M. R. Clarke (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 37–67.

Google Scholar

Packard, A., and Albergoni, V. (1970). Relative growth, nucleic acid content and cell numbers of the brain in Octopus vulgaris (Lamarck). J. Exp. Biol. 52, 539–552.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Packard, A., and Hochberg, F. G. (1977). Skin patterning in Octopus and other genera. Symposia of the Zoological Society of London 38, 191–231.

Plän, T. (1987). Funktionelle Neuroanatomie sensorisch/motorischer loben im gehirn von Octopus vulgaris. Doktorgrades der Naturwissehschaften (Dr. Rer. Nat.), Universität Regensburg.

Portmann, A. (1947). Études sur la cérébralisation chez les oiseaux. II. Les indices intracérébraux. Alauda 15, 1–15.

Google Scholar

Pozzo-Miller, L. D., Moreira, J. E., and Llinás, R. R. (1998). The first-order giant neurons of the giant fiber system in the squid: electrophysiological and ultrastructural observations. J. Neurocytol. 27, 419–429.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Quetglas, A., González, M., Carbonell, A., and Sánchez, P. (2001). Biology of the deep-sea octopus Bathypolypus sponsalis (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae) from the western Mediterranean Sea. Marine Biol. 138, 785–792. doi: 10.1007/s002270000495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quetglas, A., Ordines, F., González, M., and Franco, I. (2009). Life history of the bathyal octopus Pteroctopus tetracirrhus (Mollusca, Cephalopoda) in the Mediterranean Sea. Deep Sea Res. 56, 1379–1390. doi: 10.1016/j.dsr.2009.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliffe, J. M., Brock Fenton, M., and Shettleworth, S. J. (2006). Behavioral flexibility positively correlated with relative brain volume in predatory bats. Brain Behav. Evol. 67, 165–176. doi: 10.1159/000090980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revell, L. J. (2009). Size-correction and principal components for interspecific comparative studies. Evolution 63, 3258–3268. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00804.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezende, E. L., and Diniz-Filho, J. A. F. (2012). Phylogenetic analyses: comparing species to infer adaptations and physiological mechanisms. Compreh. Physiol. 2, 639–674. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c100079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rocha, F., Guerra, A., and Gonzalez, A. F. (2001). A review of reproductive strategies in cephalopods. Biol. Rev. 76, 291–304. doi: 10.1017/S1464793101005681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayol, F., Lapiedra, O., Ducatez, S., and Sol, D. (2019). Larger brains spur species diversification in birds. Evolution 73, 2085–2093. doi: 10.1111/evo.13811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheel, D., and Anderson, R. (2012). Variability in the Diet Specialization of Enteroctopus dofleini (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae) in the Eastern Pacific examined from midden contents. Am. Malacol. Bull. 30, 267–279. doi: 10.4003/006.030.0206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuppli, C., Graber, S. M., Isler, K., and van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Life history, cognition and the evolution of complex foraging niches. J. Hum. Evol. 92, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2015.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sherman, K., and Duda, A. M. (1999). An ecosystem approach to global assessment and management of coastal waters. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 190, 271–287.

Google Scholar

Shigeno, S., Andrews, P. L. R., Ponte, G., and Fiorito, G. (2018). Cephalopod Brains: An overview of current knowledge to facilitate comparison with vertebrates. Front. Physiol. 9:952. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.00952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shigeno, S., Kidokoro, H., Tsuchiya, K., Segawa, S., and Yamamoto, M. (2001a). Development of the brain in the oegopsid squid, Todarodes pacificus: an atlas from hatchling to juvenile. Zool. Sci. 18, 1081–1096. doi: 10.2108/zsj.18.1081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shigeno, S., Tsuchiya, K., and Segawa, S. (2001b). Embryonic and paralarval development of the central nervous system of the loliginid squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana. J. Comp. Neurol. 437, 449–475. doi: 10.1002/cne.1295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shomrat, T., Graindorge, N., Bellanger, C., Fiorito, G., Loewenstein, Y., and Hochner, B. (2011). Alternative sites of synaptic plasticity in two homologous “Fan-out Fan-in” learning and memory networks. Curr. Biol. 21, 1773–1782. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. R., and Caron, J.-B. (2010). Primitive soft-bodied cephalopods from the Cambrian. Nature 465, 469–472. doi: 10.1038/nature09068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sol, D., Gray Stirling, D., and Lefebvre, L. (2005). Behavioral drive or behavioral inhibition in evolution: subspecific diversification in holarctic passerines. Evolution 59, 2669–2677. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2005.tb00978.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sörensen, P. M., and Mouritsen, O. G. (2019). Science education and public understanding of science via food, cooking, and flavour. Int. J. Gastron. Food Sci. 15, 36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.ijgfs.2018.11.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinhausen, C., Zehl, L., Haas-Rioth, M., Morcinek, K., Walkowiak, W., and Huggenberger, S. (2016). Multivariate meta-analysis of brain-mass correlations in eutherian mammals. Front. Neuroanat. 10:91. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, H., and Pirlot, P. (1970). Volumetric comparisons of brain structures in bats. Zeitschrift für Zool. Systematik Evolutionsforschung 8, 200–236.

Google Scholar

Sweeney, M. J., Roper, C. F., Mangold, K. M., Clark, M., and Boletzky, S. v. (1992). “Larval” and juvenile cephalopods: a manual for their identification. Smithson. Contrib. Zool. 513, 1–282. doi: 10.5479/si.00810282.513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sylvester, J. B., Rich, C. A., Loh, Y.-H. E., van Staaden, M. J., Fraser, G. J., and Streelman, J. T. (2010). Brain diversity evolves via differences in patterning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 9718–9723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000395107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanner, A. R., Fuchs, D., Winkelmann, I. E., Gilbert, M. T. P., Pankey, M. S., Ribeiro, Â. M., et al. (2017). Molecular clocks indicate turnover and diversification of modern coleoid cephalopods during the Mesozoic Marine Revolution. Proc. R. Soc. 284:20162818. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.2818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toro, R., and Burnod, Y. (2005). A morphogenetic model for the development of cortical convolutions. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1900–1913. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valls, M., Rueda, L., and Quetglas, A. (2017). Feeding strategies and resource partitioning among elasmobranchs and cephalopods in Mediterranean deep-sea ecosystems. Deep Sea Res. Part I 128, 28–41. doi: 10.1016/j.dsr.2017.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dongen, P. A. M. (1998). “Brain Size in Vertebrates,” in The Central Nervous System of Vertebrates. (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg), 2099–2134. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-18262-4_23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Essen, D. C., Donahue, C. J., and Glasser, M. F. (2018). Development and evolution of cerebral and cerebellar cortex. Brain Behav. Evol. 91 158–169. doi: 10.1159/000489943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villegas, E. J. A., Ceballos-Vázquez, B. P., Markaida, U., Abitia-Cárdenas, A., Medina-López, M. A., and Arellano-Martínez, M. (2014). Diet of Octopus bimaculatus Verril, 1883 (Cephalopoda: Octopodidae) in Bahía De Los Ángeles, Gulf of California. J. Shellfish Res. 33, 305–314. doi: 10.2983/035.033.0129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, J. H. j. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize and objective function. J. Am. Stat. Ass. 58, 244.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Willemet, R. (2012). Understanding the evolution of mammalian brain structures; the need for a (new) cerebrotype approach. Brain Sciences 2, 203–224. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2020203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williamson, R., and Chrachri, A. (2004). Cephalopod neural networks. NeuroSignals 13, 87–98. doi: 10.1159/000076160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A. C. (1985). The molecular basis of evolution. Sci. Am. 253, 148–157.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wirz, K. (1959). Étude biométrique du système nerveux des Céphalopodes. Bull. Biol. Fr. Belg 93, 78–117.

Google Scholar

Wishart, D. (1987). Clustan, Cluster Analysis Software. Edinburgh: Clustan Ltd.

WoRMS Editorial Board, M. A. (2020). World Register of Marine Species [Online]. Available online at: http://marinespecies.org (accessed March 29, 2020).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Xie, Z., Domel, A. G., An, N., Green, C., Gong, Z., Wang, T., et al. (2020). Octopus arm-inspired tapered soft actuators with suckers for improved grasping. Soft Robo. 7, 639–648. doi: 10.1089/soro.2019.0082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yopak, K. E. (2012). Neuroecology of cartilaginous fishes: the functional implications of brain scaling. J. Fish Biol. 80, 1968–2023. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8649.2012.03254.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1963). The number and sizes of nerve cells in Octopus. Proc. Zool. Soc. Lond. 140, 229–254.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1964). A Model of the Brain. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Young, J. Z. (1965). The central nervous system of Nautilus. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 249, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1967). “Some comparisons between the nervous systems of cephalopods and mammals,” in Invertebrate Nervous Systems, their Significance for Mammalian Neurophysiology, ed. C. A. G. Wiersma. (Chicago: University of Chicago Press), 353–362.

Young, J. Z. (1971). The Anatomy of the Nervous System of Octopus vulgaris. London: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1974). The central nervous system of Loligo I. The optic lobe. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 267, 263–302.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1976). The nervous system of Loligo. II. Suboesophageal centres. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. 274, 101–167.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1977a). Brain, behaviour and evolution of cephalopods. Symp. Zool. Soc. Lond. 38, 377–434.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1977b). The nervous system of Loligo III. Higher motor centres: the basal supraoesophageal lobes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 276, 351–398.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1979). The nervous system of Loligo: V. The vertical lobe complex. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. 285, 311–354.

Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1991). Computation in the learning system of cephalopods. Biol. Bull. 180, 200–208.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Young, J. Z. (1995). “Multiple matrices in the memory system of Octopus,” in Cephalopod Neurobiology, eds. J.N. Abbott, R. Williamson and L. Maddock. (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 431–443.

Google Scholar

Young, R. E., and Harman, R. F. (1988). “Larva”, “paralarva” and “subadult” in cephalopod terminology. Malacologia 29, 201–207.

Google Scholar

Zar, J. H. (1999). Biostatistical Analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Zarrella, I., Herten, K., Maes, G. E., Tai, S., Yang, M., Seuntjens, E., et al. (2019). The survey and reference assisted assembly of the Octopus vulgaris genome. Sci. Data 6:13. doi: 10.1038/s41597-019-0017-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zullo, L. (2004). Functional Organisation of the Sensory-Motor Areas in the SNC of Octopus vulgaris. Ph.D. Università degli Studi di Napoli Federico II. Facoltà di Scienze Matematiche, Fisiche e Naturali.

Zullo, L., Sumbre, G., Agnisola, C., Flash, T., and Hochner, B. (2009). Nonsomatotopic organization of the higher motor centers in octopus. Curr. Biol. 19, 1632–1636. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuroecology, cephalopods, brain diversity, adaptation, evolution

Citation: Ponte G, Taite M, Borrelli L, Tarallo A, Allcock AL and Fiorito G (2021) Cerebrotypes in Cephalopods: Brain Diversity and Its Correlation With Species Habits, Life History, and Physiological Adaptations. Front. Neuroanat. 14:565109. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2020.565109

Received: 23 May 2020; Accepted: 30 December 2020;
Published: 02 February 2021.

Edited by:

Jean-Pierre Bellier, Shiga University of Medical Science, Japan

Reviewed by:

Christian Marcelo Ibáñez, Andres Bello University, Chile
Jennifer Ann Mather, University of Lethbridge, Canada
Simon G. Sprecher, Université de Fribourg, Switzerland

Copyright © 2021 Ponte, Taite, Borrelli, Tarallo, Allcock and Fiorito. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Graziano Fiorito, graziano.fiorito@szn.it

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Download