Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T09:07:25.285Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Changes in Raw Material Selection and Use at 400,000 Years bp: A Novel, Symbolic Relationship between Humans and Their World. Discussing Technological, Social and Cognitive Arguments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2021

Francesca Romagnoli*
Affiliation:
Departamento de Prehistoria y Arqueología Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (UAM) Ciudad Universitaria de Cantoblanco 28049 MadridSpain Email: francesca.romagnoli@uam.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Approximately 400,000 years bp, novel technological behaviours appeared in the archaeological record, attested by evidence of the exploitation of previously unused resources and the production of new tools. I have reviewed such innovations, and I discuss them in the frame of the anthropological, palaeoneurological, genetic and behavioural changes that appeared in the Middle Pleistocene. I propose that at this chronology humans started to see the resources as ‘other-than-human’ sentient co-dwellers. The technological innovations expressed this novel cognitive complexity and the possible new things–things, human–things and environment–things relationships. Artefacts and technologies acquired multiple semiotic meanings that were strongly interconnected with the functional value. Ethnoarchaeological evidence suggested the possible symbolic acting beyond these innovations in material culture. This perspective has relevant implications in the archaeology of the ancient Palaeolithic. It suggests the need for a new view of material culture, one that goes beyond the classical list approach in the definition of modern symbolic mediated behaviour. Further, it allows one to overcome the traditional juxtaposition between ancient cultures and Homo sapiens in terms of complexity. The evidence discussed in this paper suggests that the ontological hypothesis could change our view of Middle Pleistocene hominids and the origin and definition of modern behaviour, and test the archaeological visibility of cognition in prehistory.

Type
Special Section: When Materials Speak about Ontology: A Hunter-Gatherer Perspective
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

It has been repeatedly argued that Acheulean techno-complexes are the reflection of a technological and behavioural stagnation that lasted hundreds of millennia (Klein Reference Klein1999; Whiten et al. Reference Whiten, Horner and Marshall-Pescini2003, 102). Acheulean industry is characterized by the production of large flake-based, bifacial tools, including handaxes, cleavers and picks, and had appeared in East Africa at approximately 1.75 million years ago, which is close chronologically to the appearance of Homo erectus/ergaster (Beyene et al. Reference Beyene, Katoh and WoldeGabriel2013). This cultural stillness is usually set against the great dynamicity registered in the archaeological contexts at the rise of modern humans in Africa (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Marean and Jacobs2012; McBrearty & Brooks Reference McBrearty and Brooks2000). The onset of Homo sapiens has recently been dated at perhaps 300,000 bp (Hublin et al. Reference Hublin, Ben-Ncer and Bailey2017). Richter et al. (Reference Richter, Grün and Joannes-Boyau2017) present a complex discussion about the origin of Homo sapiens and the Middle Stone Age (MSA) cultural complex. In association with technological innovation and creativity, the emergence of new capacities in Homo sapiens is usually taken for granted with the appearance of symbolic ways to process information. New technological patterns—bone technology along with spears and projectile weapons—and symbolic artefacts and behaviours, such as ornaments and the use of coloured pigments (see Somel et al. Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitovich2013, 113 and bibliography), are usually viewed as proxies for acting symbolically since the MSA. Several recent works have shown changes in Acheulean techno-complexes, including giant-core technology, the appearance of new forms of artefacts and techniques and the diversified life-cycle of handaxes (Baena Preysler et al. Reference Baena Preysler, Torres Navas and Sharon2018; Sharon Reference Sharon2009; Stout Reference Stout2011). Beyond the possible cultural variation within the Late Acheulean industry, the conventional focus on novel capacities of Homo sapiens has not taken proper account of a previous period in which several behavioural changes suggest a complex cultural dynamism out of Africa before the advent of our species.

In this paper, I review the technological changes of this period, looking at the new forms of resources exploitation. Starting from approximately 400,000 bp, it is possible to recognize the inclusion in the archaeological record of productive resources that were previously considered waste or rejected for tool production. I argue that this novel technological creativity is the expression of changes in the way hominins looked at their world. In other words, I suggest that at approximately 400,000 bp, ancient humans built new relationships with their environs through raw materials. This idea is discussed within the framework of anthropological, palaeoneurological, genetic and general behavioural changes at that time. This ancient technological dynamism has implications for the study and understanding of behavioural modernity. The scenario that can be drawn is still patchy, with non-homogeneous evidences on wide geographical areas. However, the discussion in a global view suggests, to my mind, an interesting research line that could open a fruitful discussion and future research perspective in the understanding of what defines Homo sapiens behaviours and how we can comprehend our evolutionary success.

Material culture, meanings and relationships: an integrated, multi-layer perspective on raw materials

The aim of archaeology is to understand human behaviours over a large timespan. Archaeology is a science that primarily studies objects. The approach is to look at the relationships between material culture, which has been a fundamental dimension of human life for more than three million years, and the social and environmental contexts in which these objects have been produced and used. The specific contexts weave the meanings of material culture (Hamilakis & Jones Reference Hamilakis and Jones2017). Reconstructing the social context is extremely difficult. This is especially true in ancient prehistory, when humans differed from us in terms of biology, economic strategies and technologies, and lived in a largely dissimilar environment. Furthermore, a part of their material culture has disappeared due to the decomposition of organic materials, other post-depositional disturbances and the decreasing amount of evidence the further back in time we go. Thus, the picture is always quite incomplete and puzzling, making the study of material culture a complex matter (Romagnoli et al. Reference Romagnoli, Nishiaki, Rivals and Vaquero2018a).

The approach that is generally used in Western archaeological tradition conceptualizes the natural environment as a passive container from which to extract what we need in a materialistic perspective. Therefore, as archaeologists, we are used to interpreting ancient technologies in terms of techno-economy: we invest effort in, for example, the identification of production sequences, the quantification of the productivity of a knapping method, the calculation of the time and energy costs of the supply distance and speculation on the efficiency of tool design. In this paper, I attempt to determine whether artefacts were, and are, solely passive, functional tools. What Harris and Robb (Reference Harris and Robb2012) applied to the anthropological interpretation of the human body through history is also true for material culture: multiple ontologies exist and include materiality, but also sensoriality and affectivity (Hamilakis & Jones Reference Hamilakis and Jones2017). An object is a means of communication and conveys something that words cannot express. It has a surplus of meaning that t is not necessarily intentional and widely socialized but can be unambiguously shared by a certain group of people.

According to Guarinello (Reference Guarinello, Funari, Zarankin and Stovel2005), it is possible to reduce the meaning of artefacts to three different fields. The functional field is the most concrete and easiest to understand. The raw material and the shape of an object are strictly related to its function and operating mode. The social field is how objects create and express identities. They are social classifiers: gender division of labour, social classes, and so on. The expressive field is the most complex to understand and is strictly related to the semiotics of the cultural material.

The focus of this article is on raw materials, and I have applied this analysis to technological changes at 400,000 bp. Technology is a basic component of an artefact. Each artefact certainly had a functional goal and was created by past humans to realize a technology-mediated task. I propose that each artefact also had surplus meanings reflecting new possible thing–thing, human–thing and environment–thing relationships. I argue that these different layers of meanings were strongly interconnected in the human populations at approximately 400,000 bp. Hominids started to see land resources as something different from merely effective means to meet their needs. They started to be engaged with materials and the world in a novel, symbolic way, thanks to novel cognitive capacities and social dynamics. These meanings were codified and socially shared within the human group. The identification of a Palaeolithic object as an expression of identity and as signalling social practices is very hard to determine. However, according to economic analysis, the process of innovation is both an individual and collective phenomenon (Saxenian Reference Saxenian1994). Furthermore, the capacity to adapt to a changing environment, as expressed by technological changes, cannot be explained simply through individual problem-solving; it implies cultural learning, social organization and social dynamics. Finally, seeing the elements in a dynamic and flexible way, relationships between humans and ‘other-than-human’ elements could be understood primarily as social in nature.

Increasing the range of exploited resources at approximately 400,000 bp: The archaeological records

Independently from the label we gave to the techno-complex and to the Homo species associated with it, at approximately 400,000 bp we can show the appearance in the archaeological record of new technological behaviours. They can be identified according to the type of raw material being exploited as a new resource and the way in which humans accessed it.

Rocks

In the case of abiotic resources, the main changes involved new strategies for acquisition. An example is the intensification of lithic recycling. Almost systematically, humans transformed the functional life history of a tool in diversified and complex ways that can be categorized according to the aim of the recycling: (i) the production of short flakes and (ii) the configuration of a new tool (giving new life to waste). In the first category, we can include the exploitation of a previous tool that is later used as a core and the transformation of an Acheulean handaxe into a core, as has been shown in the lower layers at Tabun Cave (Shimelmitz Reference Shimelmitz2015). In the second category, we can include the collection of previously abandoned blanks, usually identified in the archaeological assemblage by the presence of double patination, and the transformation of a core into a retouched flake (Barkai et al. Reference Barkai, Lemorini and Vaquero2015). Until now, specific studies on recycling strategies in the Middle Pleistocene have been carried out in Europe and the Levant, showing that this technological behaviour was widespread in these regions and persisted there in the Middle Palaeolithic with several different economic values (Romagnoli Reference Romagnoli2015; Romagnoli et al. Reference Romagnoli, Gómez de Soler, Bargalló, Chacón and Vaquero2018b with bibliography).

Another example is the novel exploitation of chert quarries, as specific primary locations in the Levant attest (Verri et al. Reference Verri, Barkai and Gopher2005). This new strategy was associated with the collection of previously known surface-level secondary sources. Some authors suggested that this new behaviour was due to the diversified production of specific tool types (Boaretto et al. Reference Boaretto, Barkai, Gopher, Berna, Kubik and Weiner2009). The exploitation of underground sources implies the frequentation of a new geographical space and all the possible novel perceptions of the landscape that could be associated with it. These activities also involve specific extraction knowledge and its inter-generational transmission because of the persistence of this new behaviour for approximately 200 years beginning at 400,000 bp. It is also relevant that in the Levant, this change is associated with a general technological shift and the appearance of the new Acheulo-Yabrudian cultural complex (AYCC), including novel tools such as the Quina scrapers with stepped, scaled retouching. These artefacts are probably most related to new forms of human mobility and planning of tasks, because they are transported items usually characterized by complex and long use, re-sharpening and a recycling life-cycle (Lemorini et al. Reference Lemorini, Bourguignon, Zupancich, Gopher and Barkai2016). It has been suggested that some aesthetic concerns have been expressed in this novel context. They are reflected in the systematic collection of several small coloured flint pebbles, less than 50 g in weight and mostly reddish, which do not present any trace of functional/productive use (Assaf Reference Assaf2018).

Wood

Organic material is usually not preserved and its recovery is quite exceptional. Evidence of wood artefacts has been reported in few Middle Palaeolithic European sites, attesting to wood technology in Neanderthal groups (Aranguren et al. Reference Aranguren, Revedin and Amico2018; Carbonell & Castro-Curel Reference Carbonell and Castro-Curel1992; Rios-Garaizar et al. Reference Rios-Garaizar, López-Bultó and Iriarte2018). To date, the most ancient and complete wood artefacts have been found at Schöningen in northern Germany. The site was previously dated at approximately 400,000 years ago, according to models of sedimentations and biostratigraphical correlations. Recently, thermoluminescence dating suggested a maximum age of the level of wood technology at Marine Isotope Stage (MIS) 9, at approximately 330,000–300,000 years ago (Richter & Krbetschek Reference Richter and Krbetschek2015). Highly skilled horse hunters repeatedly occupied the open-air site (Julien et al. Reference Julien, Rivals, Serangeli, Bocherens and Conard2015). The site is well known for the exceptional degree of conservation of the archaeological records, including several botanical macro-remains of aquatic and lakeshore plants, shrubs and trees. Ethnobotanical studies have shown that most of the species yielded at the site are used for subsistence and as raw material by hunter-gatherers living in similar environments; several of the species also have medicinal effects (Bigga et al. Reference Bigga, Schoch and Urban2015).

Wood technology at Schöningen was attested by nine almost complete spears, one lance, one double-pointed rod and one worked wooden stick. These artefacts were archaeologically associated with the remains of at least 35 butchered horses. Humans selected thin trunks of spruce (in one case, pine) and removed the bark, scraping and smoothing the surface of the tools. The traces of these activities are still clearly visible. One spear shows clear evidence of several resharpening activities, suggesting repeated use. The tip was always sharpened away from the central axis of the trunk (Fig. 1D; Schoch et al. Reference Schoch, Bigga, Böhner, Richter and Terberger2015). Technological analysis and experimental archaeology suggested that the spears, with weights and dimensions similar to modern championship javelins, were well balanced and effective in the hunting of large animals from a distance of several metres. Furthermore, their technological characteristics prevented the spears from breaking, making them long-lasting tools (Smith Reference Smith2003). There is other evidence suggesting the control of wood technology in hunting activities by hominins in this period. At Clacton-on-Sea, in southern England, a tip of a yew spear was dated at approximately 400,000 bp (Oakley et al. Reference Oakley, Andrews, Keeley and Desmond1977).

Figure 1. (A) Castel di Guido site (Italy): a handaxe made of an elephant bone. (Modified from Zutovski & Barkai Reference Zutovski and Barkai2016); (B) Schöningen site (Germany): a horse bone used as an anvil; (C): Schöningen site (Germany): a horse metacarpal with knapping damage (B & C modified from Kolfschoten et al. Reference Kolfschoten, Parfitt, Serangeli and Bello2015); (D) Schöningen site (Germany): detail of a spearhead. (Modified from Schoch et al. Reference Schoch, Bigga, Böhner, Richter and Terberger2015); (E) Trinil site (Java): shell tool. Scale bar 1 cm. (Modified by Joordens et al. Reference Joordens, d'Errico and Wesselingh2015.)

Bones

Since 2.5 million years bp, ancient humans fractured animal bones to obtain the highly caloric marrow. Bone fragments were then thrown away as waste. Between 400,000 and 300,000 bp, animal bones were first used as raw material in tool production. Two types of bone artefacts are known in this chronology (Fig. 1A–C): functional tools and bone artefacts that were used to modify stone tools (retouchers). Bone retouchers became usual in Late Pleistocene Neanderthal sites (see bibliography in Hutson et al. Reference Hutson, García-Moreno, Noack, Turner, Villaluenga and Gaudzinski-Windheuser2018) and have recently been identified in the same chronology in northern China (Doyon et al. Reference Doyon, Li, Li and d'Errico2018). Within the most ancient evidence known today, there is a distal epiphysis of a red deer humerus at Boxgrove from MIS 13 associated with several retouchers made of red deer antler (Roberts & Parfitt Reference Roberts and Parfitt1999). Between MIS 12 and 11, these objects were used in France at Caune de l'Arago, La Micoque and Terra Amata. In MIS 9, in several sites in northern and western Europe, including Schöningen (see bibliography in Daujeard et al. Reference Daujeard, Valensi, Fiore, Hutson, García-Moreno, Noack, Turner, Villaluenga and Gaudzinski-Windheuser2018, 94) and in the Levant at Qesem Cave, they possibly represent local, convergent development (Blasco et al. Reference Blasco, Rosell, Cuartero, Fernández Peris, Gopher and Barkai2013a). Between MIS 12 and 9, bone retouchers were variously associated with bifacial, AYCC and pre-Mousterian technology.

Bone tools became basic elements in the culture of Homo sapiens in Europe. They firstly appeared around 400,000 bp in the Levant at Revadim Quarry, in three sites in central-western Italy (where a later fourth site was dated as MIS 7), at Vértesszőlős in Hungary and at Bilzingsleben in Germany (see bibliography in Gaudzinski et al. Reference Gaudzinski, Turner and Anzidei2005; Zutovski & Barkai Reference Zutovski and Barkai2016). They were large bifacial handaxes made of proboscidean bones and produced with the same procedures applied to shape the similar Acheulean lithic tools (Villa & d’Errico Reference Villa and d'Errico2001). At Bilzingsleben, other tools have been found, including an elephant bone used as an anvil and five splinters on which a regular series of parallel and convergent cut marks were engraved. Authors have suggested that the regularity in the patterns of lines has to be interpreted as an abstract meaning, a way of communicating, rather than the accidental production of cut marks (Mania & Mania Reference Mania, Mania, Gamble and Porr2005). A horse bone anvil has also been identified at Schöningen (Kolfschoten et al. Reference Kolfschoten, Parfitt, Serangeli and Bello2015). Two other bone tools, similar to lithic retouched tools, have been found in the TD10 layer at the Gran Dolina site in Atapuerca dated to MIS 10/9 (Rosell et al. Reference Rosell, Blasco, Fernández Peris, Carbonell, Barkai and Gopher2015). The strong predominance of proboscidean bones for Acheulean organic handaxes is not surprising. In these techno-complexes, humans were strongly dependent on these animals for subsistence. Finally, at Qesem Cave, some indirect evidence of early bone-working activities has been identified within AYCC assemblages. They are residue and use-wear traces on two chert scrapers and a mark on a fallow deer tibia not linked to butchering tasks (Zupancich et al. Reference Zupancich, Nunziante-Cesaro and Blasco2016).

An antecedent of Middle Pleistocene bone technology could be seen in the use of fragments of bones and a horn by South African Paranthropus to dig into the soil for the collection of termites for consumption (d’Errico & Backwell Reference d'Errico and Backwell2009). However, in this case, hominins ‘used’ bones but did not modify them before use. Middle Pleistocene bone retouchers are conceptualized differently because they imply the use of a bone fragment to modify another raw material to shape artefacts. Thus, it is a novel technology-related behavioural pattern. Researchers have continued to demonstrate bone technology in the Late Middle Palaeolithic as well. Neanderthals frequently used animal and, to a lesser extent, human skull fragments and long bone retouchers, especially within Quina techno-complexes (Hutson et al. Reference Hutson, García-Moreno, Noack, Turner, Villaluenga and Gaudzinski-Windheuser2018; Rougier et al. Reference Rougier, Crevecoeur and Beauval2016). Sporadically, they also used animal and human bones to produce specialized tools and to express symbolic communication (Burke & d’Errico Reference Burke and d'Errico2008; Majkić et al. Reference Majkić, Evans, Stepanchuk, Tsvelykh and d'Errico2017; Soressi et al. Reference Soressi, McPherron and Lenoir2013).

Shells

The origin of the exploitation of coastal resources stepped into the spotlight latterly because of its implication in human evolution. European Neanderthals exploited molluscs and marine mammals for consumption (see bibliography in Álvarez-Fernández Reference Álvarez-Fernández2015). At Cueva de los Aviones and Cueva Antón in Spain and Fumane Cave in Italy, shells have been found associated with mineral pigments, suggesting a role of marine shells in Neanderthal symbolically mediated behaviour (Peresani et al. Reference Peresani, Vanhaeren, Quaggiotto, Queffelec and d'Errico2013; Zilhão et al. Reference Zilhão, Angelucci and Badal-García2010). Neanderthals’ use of shell technology also shows that it was well integrated into the techno-economic behaviour of the human groups (Romagnoli Reference Romagnoli, Borgia and Cristiani2018; Romagnoli et al. Reference Romagnoli, Baena and Sarti2016; Reference Romagnoli, Baena, Pardo Naranjo and Sarti2017). It included the systematic selection of Callista chione valves, their configuration as tools using specific techniques and deep knowledge of the constraints imposed by this organic resource.

At present, the most ancient evidence of the use of marine shells as raw material has been identified in a Homo erectus context in Southeast Asia at Trinil and is dated by luminescence at a minimum age of 0.43 ± 0.05 million years. Within several consumed Pseudodon specimens, one showed anthropogenic modifications of the ventral margin associated with smoothing and polishing due to use (Fig. 1E). Furthermore, one valve showed an engraving on the central part of the external surface. A zigzag line was associated with a set of parallel lines and was probably made on the brown periostracum of a fresh valve (Joordens et al. Reference Joordens, d'Errico and Wesselingh2015).

Other changes in the same chronological period and their implications for the appearance of novel communication codes in human evolution

These new technologies and varieties of raw materials appeared in the Middle Pleistocene alongside other major behavioural and anthropological changes.

The control and regular use of fire in Europe were first attested at this time (Roebroeks & Villa Reference Roebroeks and Villa2011). Fire control, attested less frequently since at least about 800,000 bp (Alperson-Afil Reference Alperson-Afil2008; see Berna et al. Reference Berna, Goldberg, Horwitz, Brink, Holt, Bamford and Chazan2012 for the oldest evidence), has affected new social and semiotic relationships among humans, and wood resources could have acquired additional meanings reflecting them.

New stone tool technologies and toolkits appeared. Evidence is found in the shift to AYCC in the Levant (Barkai & Gopher Reference Barkai and Gopher2011) with Quina scrapers and laminar technology and in Africa and Eurasia with the appearance of predetermined Levallois core strategies. Levallois production methods were used in some areas in tandem with previous bifacial technology, most probably due to evolutionary processes based on a common technological ancestry (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Wilkinson and Blockley2014; Moncel et al. Reference Moncel, Moigne, Youcef and Combier2011). MSA assemblages appeared in eastern and southern Africa in this period of global cultural dynamism.

New economic strategies were demonstrated at this chronology in the accumulated first evidence of a decrease in pachyderm eating, the active communal hunting of large ungulates (Rodríguez-Hidalgo et al. Reference Rodríguez-Hidalgo, Saladié, Ollé, Arsuaga, Bermúdez de Castro and Carbonell2017), standardized carcass processing (Blasco et al. Reference Blasco, Rosell and Domínguez-Rodrigo2013b) and a diversification in diet. The diversification included an ever-increasing consumption of small game (e.g., tortoises and birds) and marine resources (see bibliography in Blasco et al. Reference Blasco, Rosell, Smith, Maul, Sañudo, Barkai and Gopher2016), as well as plants for nutrition and medication (Hardy Reference Hardy2018). These new subsistence strategies imply novel, flexible and complex relationships between humans, plants, animals and specific ecological niches with which humans would have had few previous interactions. Active communal hunting could also have driven novel relationships with weapon technology. Furthermore, these economic strategies imply reorganization and new social human relationships, including the social division of labour, food sharing and the transmission of knowledge of plants and animals. These strong and complex social relationships could explain the social assistance that would have supported the survival of pathological individuals (Trinkaus Reference Trinkaus2018). Changes in the socio-economy could have driven novel meanings within the additional value of resources and technologies related to a new perception of the landscape and the novel relationships humans built with it.

Ethnoarchaeology gives several pieces of evidence regarding how technology is fully integrated into the ontological perception of landscape. Australian Aborigines construct complex relationships with their territory according to the belief that rocks can listen and oversee human activities (Povinelli Reference Povinelli1995). The North American Ojibwe look at animals, plants, inanimate objects and natural forces as ‘persons’ with whom humans maintain social relationships (Hallowell Reference Hallowell and Diamond1960). South Indian Nayaka hunter-gatherers looked at plants, animals and rocks in different ways—as ‘other-than-human’ sentient co-dwellers or objects—not on the basis of the inherent essence of these ‘entities’ or their location in the landscape, but by when, why and which people dealt with them (Naveh & Bird-David Reference Naveh and Bird-David2014). This dynamic and living perception of entities in the landscape declined when Nayaka became producers and started to perceive plants, animals and stones as means to acquire something else. According to Naveh and Bird-David (Reference Naveh and Bird-David2014), this transition to a more pragmatic, static and unique conceptualization was due to socio-economic changes and the delayed use of these resources by anonymous consumers rather than to changes in human social relationships. All of this suggests that it might be unduly restrictive to interpret ancient changes in material culture only in terms of economic rationality—production sequences, effectivity, transportability, etc.—according to a modern, Western, consumer conception of tools and land resources.

From a palaeoanthropological viewpoint, the Middle Pleistocene is characterized by a great diversity of human species, and all of them are in different ways associated with some of these novel behaviours. Thus, we see a complex mosaic that make it impossible to link a human species linearly to a specific technology and behavioural set. Looking at anatomical and genetic traits that can support the capacity for creating novel and more complex semiotic meanings in material culture, current data suggest that all these hominins shared such traits. At the latest, they appeared at the emergence of the speciation events occurring around 600,000 bp, long before the advent of modern humans.

Within a progressive process of encephalization, hominins species who lived in the Middle Pleistocene shared some genetic mutations that are relevant when speaking about behaviours. The GADD45G and DUF1220/NBPF genes (probably leading to brain size expansion) and the HAR1 mutation (related to cortical expansion) were already present within Neanderthal features (O'Bleness et al. Reference O'Bleness, Searles, Varki, Gagneux and Sikela2012; Somel et al. Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitovich2013) that first appeared in the European fossil record at approximately 400,000 bp (see bibliography in Hublin Reference Hublin2009). The FOXP2 amino acid mutation was already present at this point in Neanderthals, Denisovans and Heidelbergensis, suggesting that changes in brain connectivity linked with the motor controls of fingers and language were already present not only in these hominin species (Somel et al. Reference Somel, Liu and Khaitovich2013, 113 and bibliography) but most probably also in their common ancestor. Palaeontological and archaeological evidence also supports complex linguistic capacity and codes in Neanderthals (Dediu & Levinson Reference Dediu and Levinson2018). Semiotically complex ways of communicating novel perceptions of the landscape could fit within these new communication codes and could be the result of the complex process of anatomical and cognitive reorganization attested in hominins since approximately 600,000 bp.

I propose that the changes in the archaeological record presented in this paper are the expression of a novel, complex, dynamic conceptualization of the world, similar to what is attested in ethnoarchaeological contexts where resources were viewed as ‘other-than-human’ sentient co-dwellers. These innovations in technology and the selection of production resources are the expression of a novel, symbolic way in which humans related with materials and the land.

This hypothesis is based on several arguments. The changes in material culture presented in this paper appeared in a short time interval, associated with different human species, and across different and unconnected regions, each characterized by specific cultural traits. Thus, they were most probably due to a cognitive shift, rather than being novel technological or economic strategies. Furthermore, in these innovations there are some semiotic aspects that I find particularly significant. The first is that some of the resources that are introduced in the production process from this moment were previously seen as waste, as in the case of animal bones. This change implies a novel conceptualization of the raw material where it goes from being disregarded to being the focus of repeated and systematized human interactions and a novel element of human culture. Moreover, these resources are not isolated but are part of a whole. The use of bone as raw material could be related to a novel, holistic conceptualization of the animals, which were fundamental entities for human survival. Ethnoarchaeological literature offers several forms of evidence for the possible complex way of meaning of human–animal interactions in hunter communities. On one hand, the animal must be dominated, while on the other, dependence on the animal for livelihood engages humans in a series of reciprocal relations with it, including a specific treatment of each part of the animal carcass (see Nadasdy Reference Nadasdy2007 and bibliography). The vision of raw materials as part of a whole, with consequent novel semiotics and possible novel cognitive human–whole interactions, could be also applied to underground resources (as part of the soil on which we stand and move), of wood (as part of the forest) and shells (as part of the sea and being themselves animals). More generally, these resources are all part of the landscape and their novel exploitation could be related to a novel conceptualization of land. Recycling could be a further component of the novel semiotics of technology. The recycling of tools enlarges the life-cycle of an artefact. This could be an economic advantage, reducing the energy and time costs of searching for the raw material and modifying it up to get the final, functional product (Romagnoli Reference Romagnoli2015). At the same time we cannot exclude that the recycled object is gaining importance as container of external memories, as happens with our contemporary objects handed down for generations. Finally, some of these resources could have had a value in relation to their aesthetic appearance. Shells, for example, are characterized by an external shiny and coloured surface; and we cannot exclude that lithic resources were also selected according to their colour and the combination in the recycled tool of the patinated, older surfaces and the original colour of the matrix, revealed by the latest retouching.

All these arguments suggest that the technological innovations presented in this paper could have been the expression of novel meanings of raw materials and tools, and novel modalities in which humans interpreted the land and engaged with it. This behavioural complexity needed a larger working memory capacity, a cognitive system that allows abilities in learning and reasoning (Baddeley Reference Baddeley2012) and higher language processing with increased ability to create and comprehend signs and meanings. An increase in these neuronal functions is expected to be correlated with a larger cerebellar volume (Kochiyama et al. Reference Kochiyama, Ogihara and Tanabe2018, 4). Furthermore, the novel conception of land I propose in this paper would have needed a higher visual cognition and, in particular, the ‘semantic’ processing of visual information (Jeannerod & Jacob Reference Jeannerod and Jacob2005). This capacity implies the perception of spatial relations between objects, that is to say the identification of an isolated object seen in a complex, multiple disposition, and the codification of that object according to the conceptual information and knowledge of it that are stored in our long-term memory. An increase in visual cognition, including the enhancement of multi-step manipulation of spatially presented information, could have allowed humans to get around more safely in the land, know better the different geographical areas and, thus, exploit a larger range of resources. Furthermore, it could have modified the conception humans had of a whole as composed by different parts. This understanding of their realm could also have led to a different conception of the individual as part of a community, as expressed in the care of the aged and infirm people (Trinkaus Reference Trinkaus2018) and, at the end of the Middle Pleistocene, to intentional human burials (e.g. Pomeroy et al. Reference Pomeroy, Bennett and Hunt2020). Finally, the improvement in processing spatial information could have enhanced the 2D and 3D manipulation capacities that allowed for the creation of novel technologies and tools. Visual cognition is stored in the parietal lobule of the human brain (Jeannerod & Jacob Reference Jeannerod and Jacob2005).

Some studies suggested that cerebellar volume and the parietal lobule were larger in Homo sapiens than in Neanderthals (Bruner & Iriki Reference Bruner and Iriki2016; Kochiyama et al. Reference Kochiyama, Ogihara and Tanabe2018; Neubauer et al. Reference Neubauer, Hublin and Gunz2018), suggesting higher cognitive and social capacities in our species. However, there are some limits in the ontological interpretation of the use of new raw materials based on morpho-anatomical and palaeoneurological studies. The first is that we still do not know the exact relationship between dimension, morphology and functional capacities in the human brain, genetics, and cognitive performance. The second is that the argument used in these studies to sustain the lower level of cognitive complexity in pre-sapiens is usually the lack of complexity in the archaeological record. However, the pre-sapiens cultural inferiority is usually defined by a European Upper Palaeolithic perspective and is based on a traditional list approach, which is anachronistic and untrustworthy according to the novel multidisciplinary data. Thus, this is a flawed, circular argument that risks becoming self-referenced and limiting our understanding of pre-sapiens cognitive and social capacities and behaviours. Furthermore, my hypothesis is based on the comparison between the Middle Pleistocene populations and their predecessor and we know little about the cognitive capacities of pre-600,000 bp hominids (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Polly and Bruner2020). Finally, these studies mentioned that the origin of the modern brain morphology did not necessary coincide with the origin of our lineage (Bruner & Iriki Reference Bruner and Iriki2016, 104) and that there were no significant differences in the relative size of the parietal regions between Neanderthal and Early Homo sapiens (Kochiyama et al. Reference Kochiyama, Ogihara and Tanabe2018, 5). This evidence suggests that Middle Pleistocene populations shared similar palaeoneurological and cognitive traits and that there are still many unknown data about cerebrocranial organization and its palaeoneurological implications in extinct humans.

Hypotheses in cognitive archaeology are speculative and need to be discussed with a multidisciplinary approach. The notion of the brain–artefact interface (Malafouris Reference Malafouris2010) suggests that brain, material culture and environment are integrated in a complex manner. According to this idea, technologically mediated cultural behaviours are a part of the human cognitive architecture, and interaction with cultural objects and the material world may remodel brain anatomy and function. Based on functional neuroimaging and evolutionary neurosciences, even early stone tools should be capable of extending human cognition, being not merely a passive aid for humans to realize a specific task (see Malafouris Reference Malafouris2010, 266–7 and bibliography). As Bruner and Iriki pointed out (Reference Bruner and Iriki2016, 107), we still ignore how the relationships between brain, material culture and environment could affect ‘intelligence’ and ‘creativity’. I propose to change the traditional archaeological hypothesis about the conceptualization of the material world at 400,000 bp and to test the possible cognitive interpretations we can generate combining palaeoneurology, genetics, psychology, archaeology, ethnoarchaeology and cognitive archaeology.

Assuming my hypothesis is true, there is a delay compared to the appearance of the anatomical and genetic traits supporting this cognitive capacity in the fossil record. To be identifiable in archaeological records, human behaviours must be repeated over time. This implies a standardized conduct shared by the human group. Looking at the innovative exploitation of resources at 400,000 bp, their additional values and means of relationships humans maintained with these objects and raw materials should have been unambiguously shared. For this to happen, specific demographic, social and cultural conditions are needed to foster the collective codification of the means of communication that these new objects expressed. They were most probably driven by the socio-economic changes that appeared at this time in the diet, collective hunting and hearth-related organization of the living space. A similar argument can be used concerning the occurrence of graphic symbols and personal ornaments. There is a delay between the appearance of Homo sapiens and the recurrent presence of these evidences in the archaeological records, dated at approximately 100,000 bp and independently associated with modern humans and Neanderthals (e.g. Henshilwood et al. Reference Henshilwood, d'Errico and Van Niekerk2011; Hoffmann et al. Reference Hoffmann, Standish and García-Diez2018; Radovčić et al. Reference Radovčić, Sršen, Radovčić and Frayer2015) and it was most probably related to novel social and demographic dynamics.

The behavioural dynamism highlighted at 400,000 bp was restricted neither to a specific human species and hominin lineage nor to an ecological niche or geographical area, and predates the emergence of modern humans in Africa. The appearance of complex conduct is a matter of both higher cognitive capacities and social/cultural context. Symbolic behaviour is usually assumed for ornaments, graphic signs and complex technologies. Although between 600,000 and 100,000 bp several human species shared the cognitive capacities to produce a cultural modernity as defined by the presence of symbolic mediated behaviour, it makes its appearance in the archaeological records only much later than the emergence of modern humans and most probably independently within sapiens and Neanderthal communities. I propose that we need to rethink the definition of symbolic behaviour beyond our contemporary, Western conception. Ethnoarchaeology suggests that symbolic mediated behaviours could be less evident than the use of colour and personal ornaments. They can also be expressed by a shared perception of the landscape that was represented symbolically in communication and in the relationships humans built with objects that became the expression of social and ontological factors (Arthur Reference Arthur2018; Weedman Reference Weedman2006). Thus, a different view of the study of past material culture, going beyond economic and technological perspectives to include additional meanings, may enlarge the narrative of ancient archaeology and open new paths in the understanding of Homo sapiens cultural and cognitive records.

The idea discussed in this paper must now be tested through an analysis of the degree of technological complexity with a multidisciplinary approach and an in-depth study of the intensity of the novel behaviours on a scale from episodic to systematic, to verify whether these aspects have led to real adaptations with evolutionary implications. Furthermore, we still disregard the flexibility and versatility of most of the technological and economic strategies within their cultural contexts, and we still lack adequate in-depth knowledge of pre-sapiens cultural evolution. My goal is to open the reader's mind to new possibility in the way in which we look at and interpret the Pleistocene archaeological records. The evidence I discuss in this paper suggests that the ontological hypothesis I have proposed here could be relevant to change our view of Middle Pleistocene hominids and the origin and definition of modern behaviours, and test the archaeological visibility of cognition in ancient prehistory. It deserves to be seriously tested. Social and symbolic complexity in Homo sapiens seem not to have been abrupt novelties, and we have to investigate the possible exaptation of some traits that we have perhaps uncritically taken for granted in ourselves and regarded from a limited, distorted and flawed perspective.

Acknowledgements

This study has been supported by the project ‘Re-definiendo forma, uso y expression cultural en el Paleolítico medio’, ref.: SI1/PJI/2019-00488, funded by Comunidad Autónoma de Madrid and Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. The author is very grateful to the editor and anonymous reviewers for their useful comments that allowed for the improvement in clarity and straightforwardness of a previous version of the paper. The ideas and the arguments, and therefore, any possible mistake and weakness, are my responsibility. The author is thankful to Ella Assaf for discussing and sharing ideas, and doubts, about hunter-gatherer ontologies.

References

Adler, D.S., Wilkinson, K.N., Blockley, S., et al. , 2014. Early Levallois technology and the Lower to Middle Paleolithic transition in the Southern Caucasus. Science 345, 1609–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alperson-Afil, N., 2008. Continual fire-making by hominins at Gesher Benot Ya'aqov, Israel. Quaternary Science Reviews 27, 1733–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Álvarez-Fernández, A., 2015. L'exploitation des ressources marines au Paléolithique moyen et supérieur initial en Europe: synthèse des données disponibles [Marine resource exploitation during the Middle and Early Upper Paleolithic in Europe: overview of the available evidence ]. Palethnologie 7. https://doi.org/10.4000/palethnologie.805Google Scholar
Aranguren, B., Revedin, A., Amico, N., et al. , 2018. Early Neanderthal wooden artifacts from Italy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, 2054–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arthur, K.W., 2018. The Lives of Stone Tools: Crafting the status, skill and identity of flintknappers. Tucson (AZ): University of Arizona Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assaf, E., 2018. Paleolithic aesthetics: collecting colorful flint pebbles at Middle Pleistocene Qesem Cave, Israel. Journal of Lithic Studies 5(1). https://doi.org/10.2218/jls.2616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., 2012. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 63, 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baena Preysler, J., Torres Navas, C. & Sharon, G., 2018. Life history of a large biface. Quaternary Science Reviews 190, 123–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkai, R. & Gopher, A., 2011. Innovative human behavior between Acheulian and Mousterian: a view from Qesem Cave, Israel. Etudes et Recherches Archéologiques de l'Université de Liège 999, 112.Google Scholar
Barkai, R., Lemorini, C. & Vaquero, M. (eds), 2015. The origins of recycling: a Paleolithic perspective. Quaternary International 361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berna, F., Goldberg, P., Horwitz, L.K., Brink, J., Holt, S., Bamford, M. & Chazan, M., 2012. Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape province, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, 1215–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beyene, Y., Katoh, S., WoldeGabriel, G., et al. , 2013. The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110, 1584–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bigga, G., Schoch, W.H. & Urban, B., 2015. Paleoenvironment and possibilities of plant exploitation in the Middle Pleistocene of Schöningen (Germany). Insights from botanical macro-remains and pollen. Journal of Human Evolution 89, 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Cuartero, F., Fernández Peris, J., Gopher, A. & Barkai, R., 2013a. Using bones to shape stones: MIS 9 bone retouchers at both edges of the Mediterranean Sea. PloS ONE 8, e76780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., et al. , 2013b. Learning by the heart: cultural patterns in the faunal processing sequence during the Middle Pleistocene. PLoS ONE 8, e55863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Smith, K.T., Maul, L.C., Sañudo, P., Barkai, R. & Gopher, A., 2016. Tortoises as dietary supplement: a view from the Middle Pleistocene site of Qesem Cave, Israel. Quaternary Science Reviews 133, 165–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boaretto, E., Barkai, R., Gopher, A., Berna, F., Kubik, P.W. & Weiner, S., 2009. Specialized flint procurement strategies for hand axes, scrapers and blades in the Late Lower Palaeolithic: a 10Be study at Qesem Cave, Israel. Human Evolution 24, 112.Google Scholar
Brown, K.S., Marean, C.W., Jacobs, Z., et al. , 2012. An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa. Nature 491, 590–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, E. & Iriki, A., 2016. Extending mind, visuospatial integration, and the evolution of the parietal lobes in the human genus. Quaternary International 405, 98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, A. & d'Errico, F., 2008. A Middle Palaeolithic bone tool from Crimea (Ukraine). Antiquity 82, 843–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carbonell, E. & Castro-Curel, Z., 1992. Palaeolithic wooden artefacts from the Abric Romaní (Capellades, Barcelona, Spain). Journal of Archaeological Science 19, 707–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daujeard, C., Valensi, P., Fiore, I., et al. , 2018. A reappraisal of Lower to Middle Palaeolithic bone retouchers from southeastern France (MIS 11 to 3), in The Origins of Bone Technologies, eds Hutson, J.M., García-Moreno, A., Noack, E.S., Turner, E., Villaluenga, A. & Gaudzinski-Windheuser, S.. (RGZM-Tagungen 35). Mainz: Verlag des Römisch-Germanischen Zentralmuseums, 93132.Google Scholar
Dediu, D. & Levinson, S.C., 2018. Neanderthal language revisited: not only us. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 21, 4955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d'Errico, F. & Backwell, L., 2009. Assessing the function of early hominin bone tools. Journal of Archaeological Science 36, 1764–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doyon, L., Li, Z., Li, H. & d'Errico, F., 2018. Discovery of circa 115,000-year-old bone retouchers at Lingjing, Henan, China. PLos ONE 13, e0194318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaudzinski, S., Turner, E., Anzidei, A.P., et al. , 2005. The use of Proboscidean remains in every-day Palaeolithic life. Quaternary International 126–128, 179–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guarinello, N.L., 2005. Archaeology and the meanings of material culture, in Global Archaeological Theory, eds Funari, P.P., Zarankin, A. & Stovel, E.. New York (NY): Kluwer Academic/Plenum, 19–28.Google Scholar
Hallowell, I., 1960. Ojibwa ontology, behavior, and worldview, in Culture in History: Essays in honor of Paul Radin, ed. Diamond, S.. New York (NY): Columbia University Press, 1952.Google Scholar
Hamilakis, Y. & Jones, A.M., 2017. Archaeology and assemblage. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 27, 7784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardy, K., 2018. Plant use in the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic: food, medicine and raw materials. Quaternary Science Reviews 191, 393405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, O.J.T. & Robb, J., 2012. Multiple ontologies and the problem of the body in history. American Anthropologist 114, 668–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, C., d'Errico, F., Van Niekerk, K.L., et al. , 2011. A 100,000-year-old ochre-processing workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science 334, 219–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, D.L., Standish, C.D., García-Diez, M., et al. , 2018. U-Th dating of carbonate crusts reveals Neandertal origin of Iberian cave art. Science 359, 912–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hublin, J.J., 2009. The origin of Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 16022–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hublin, J.J., Ben-Ncer, A., Bailey, S.E., et al. , 2017. New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo sapiens. Nature 546, 289–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutson, J.M., García-Moreno, A., Noack, E.S., Turner, E., Villaluenga, A. & Gaudzinski-Windheuser, S. (eds), 2018. The Origins of Bone Technologies. (RGZM-Tagungen 35). Mainz: Verlag des Römisch-Germanischen Zentralmuseums.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. & Jacob, P., 2005. Visual cognition: a new look at the two-visual systems model. Neuropsychologia 43, 301–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joordens, J.C.A., d'Errico, F., Wesselingh, F.P., et al. , 2015. Homo erectus at Trinil on Java used shells for tool production and engraving. Nature 518, 228–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Julien, M.A., Rivals, F., Serangeli, J., Bocherens, H. & Conard, N.J., 2015. A new approach for deciphering between single and multiple accumulation events using intra-tooth isotopic variations: application to the Middle Pleistocene bone bed of Schöningen 13 II-4. Journal of Human Evolution 89, 114–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R.G., 1999. The Human Career: Human biological and cultural origins. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kochiyama, T., Ogihara, N., Tanabe, H.C., et al. , 2018. Reconstructing the Neanderthal brain using computational anatomy. Scientific Reports 8, 6296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolfschoten, T.V., Parfitt, S.A., Serangeli, J. & Bello, S.M., 2015. Lower Paleolithic bone tools from the ‘Spear Horizon’ at Schöningen (Germany). Journal of Human Evolution 89, 226–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemorini, C., Bourguignon, L., Zupancich, A., Gopher, A. & Barkai, R., 2016. A scraper's life history: morpho-techno-functional and use-wear analysis of Quina and demi-Quina scrapers from Qesem Cave, Israel. Quaternary International 398, 8693.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majkić, A., Evans, S., Stepanchuk, V., Tsvelykh, A. & d'Errico, F., 2017. A decorated raven bone from the Zaskalnaya VI (Kolosovskaya) Neanderthal site, Crimea. PLoS ONE 12, e0173435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malafouris, L., 2010. The brain-artefact interface (BAI): a challenge for archaeology and cultural neurosciences. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5, 264–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mania, D. & Mania, U., 2005. The natural and socio-cultural environment of Homo erectus at Bilzingsleben, Germany, in The Hominin Individual in Context: Archaeological investigations of Lower and Middle Palaeolithic landscapes, locales and artefacts, eds Gamble, C. & Porr, M.. London: Routledge, 98114.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S. & Brooks, A.S., 2000. The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39, 453563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moncel, M.H., Moigne, A.M., Youcef, S. & Combier, J., 2011. The emergence of Neanderthal technical behaviour: new evidence from Orgnac 3 (Level 1, MIS 8), Southeastern France. Current Anthropology 52, 3775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadasdy, P., 2007. The gift in the animal: the ontology of hunting and human-animal sociality. American Ethnologist 34, 2543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naveh, D. & Bird-David, N., 2014. How persons become things: economic and epistemological changes among Nayaka hunter-gatherers. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 20, 7492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neubauer, S., Hublin, J.J. & Gunz, P., 2018. The evolution of modern human brain shape. Science Advances 4, eaao5961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Bleness, M., Searles, V., Varki, A., Gagneux, P. & Sikela, J.M., 2012. Evolution of genetic and genomic features unique to the human lineage. Nature Reviews Genetics 13, 853–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakley, K., Andrews, P., Keeley, L.H. & Desmond, C., 1977. A reappraisal of the Clacton spearpoint. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society 43, 1330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, A., Polly, P.D. & Bruner, E., 2020. Temporal lobe evolution in Javanese Homo erectus and African Homo ergaster: inferences from the cranial base. Quaternary International. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.quaint.2020.07.048Google Scholar
Peresani, M., Vanhaeren, M., Quaggiotto, E., Queffelec, A. & d'Errico, F., 2013. An ochered fossil marine shell from the Mousterian of Fumane Cave, Italy. PLoS ONE 8, e68572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomeroy, E., Bennett, P., Hunt, C.O., et al. , 2020. New Neanderthal remains associated with the ‘flower burial’ at Shanidar Cave. Antiquity 94, 1126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Povinelli, E., 1995. Do rocks listen? The cultural politics of apprehending Australian Aboriginal labor. American Anthropologist 97, 505–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radovčić, D., Sršen, A.O., Radovčić, J. & Frayer, D.W., 2015. Evidence for Neandertal jewelry: modified white-tailed eagle claws at Krapina. PLoS ONE 10, e0119802.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, D. & Krbetschek, M., 2015. The age of the Lower Paleolithic occupation at Schöningen. Journal of Human Evolution 89, 4656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, D., Grün, R., Joannes-Boyau, R., et al. , 2017. The age of the hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, and the origins of the Middle Stone Age. Nature 546, 293–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rios-Garaizar, J., López-Bultó, O., Iriarte, E., et al. , 2018. A Middle Palaeolithic wooden digging stick from Aranbaltza III, Spain. PLoS ONE 13, e0195044.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, M.B. & Parfitt, S.A. (eds.), 1999. Boxgrove: A Middle Pleistocene hominid site at Eartham Quarry, Boxgrove, West Sussex. (Archaeological Report 17.) London: English Heritage.Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Hidalgo, A., Saladié, P., Ollé, A., Arsuaga, J.L., Bermúdez de Castro, J.M. & Carbonell, E., 2017. Human predatory behavior and the social implications of communal hunting based on evidence from the TD10.2 bison bone bed at Gran Dolina (Atapuerca, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution 105, 89122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roebroeks, W. & Villa, P., 2011. On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 5209–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romagnoli, F., 2015. A second life: recycling production waste during the Middle Palaeolithic in layer L at Grotta del Cavallo (Lecce, Southeast Italy). Quaternary International 361, 200211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romagnoli, F., 2018. The socio-economic significance of Neanderthal shell technology, in Palaeolithic Italy: Advanced studies on early human adaptations in the Apennine peninsula, eds Borgia, V. & Cristiani, E.. Leiden: Sidestone, 125–46.Google Scholar
Romagnoli, F., Baena, J. & Sarti, L., 2016. Neanderthal retouched shell tools and Quina economic and technical strategies: an integrated behaviour. Quaternary International 407, 2944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romagnoli, F., Baena, J., Pardo Naranjo, A.I. & Sarti, L., 2017. Evaluating the performance of the cutting edge of Neanderthal shell tools: a new experimental approach. Use, mode of operation, and strength of Callista chione from a behavioural, Quina perspective. Quaternary International 427, 216–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romagnoli, F., Nishiaki, Y., Rivals, F. & Vaquero, M., 2018a. Time uncertainty, site formation processes, and human behaviours: new insights on old issues in High-Resolution Archaeology. Quaternary International 474, 99102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romagnoli, F., Gómez de Soler, B., Bargalló, A., Chacón, M.G. & Vaquero, M., 2018b. Here and now or a previously planned strategy? Rethinking the concept of ramification for micro-production in expedient contexts: implications for Neanderthal socio-economic behaviour. Quaternary International 474, Part B, 168–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosell, J., Blasco, R., Fernández Peris, J., Carbonell, E., Barkai, R. & Gopher, A., 2015. Recycling bones in the Middle Pleistocene: some reflections from Gran Dolina TD10-1 (Spain), Bolomor Cave (Spain) and Qesem Cave (Israel). Quaternary International 361, 297312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rougier, H., Crevecoeur, I., Beauval, C., et al. , 2016. Neanderthal cannibalism and Neanderthal bones used as tools in Northern Europe. Scientific Reports 6, 29005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxenian, A.L., 1994. Regional Advantage: Culture and competition in Silicon Valley and Route 128. Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schoch, W.H., Bigga, G., Böhner, U., Richter, P. & Terberger, T., 2015. New insights on the wooden weapons from the Paleolithic site of Schöningen. Journal of Human Evolution 89, 214–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharon, G., 2009. Acheulian giant-core technology. A worldwide perspective. Current Anthropology 50, 335–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimelmitz, R., 2015. The recycling of flint throughout the Lower and Middle Paleolithic sequence of Tabun Cave, Israel. Quaternary International 361, 3445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, G.M., 2003. Damage inflicted upon animal bone by wooden projectiles: experimental results and archaeological implications. Journal of Taphonomy 1, 312.Google Scholar
Somel, M., Liu, X. & Khaitovich, P., 2013. Human brain evolution: transcripts, metabolites and their regulators. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14, 112–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soressi, M., McPherron, S.P., Lenoir, M., et al. , 2013. Neanderthals made the first specialised bone tools in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110, 14186–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stout, D. 2011. Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366, 1050–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trinkaus, E., 2018. An abundance of developmental anomalies and abnormalities in Pleistocene people. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115, 11941–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verri, G., Barkai, R., Gopher, A., et al. , 2005. Flint procurement strategies in the late Lower Palaeolithic recorded by in situ produced cosmogenic 10Be in Tabun and Qesem Caves (Israel). Journal of Archaeological Science 32, 2017–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villa, P. & d'Errico, F., 2001. Bone and ivory points in the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic in Europe. Journal of Human Evolution 41, 69112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weedman, K.J., 2006. An ethnoarchaeological study of hafting and stone tool diversity among the Gamo of Ethiopia. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 13, 189238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V. & Marshall-Pescini, S., 2003. Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology 12, 92105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D.E., Badal-García, E., et al. , 2010. Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 1023–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zupancich, A., Nunziante-Cesaro, S., Blasco, R., et al. , 2016. Early evidence of stone tool use in bone working activities at Qesem Cave, Israel. Scientific Reports 6, 37686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zutovski, K. & Barkai, R., 2016. The use of elephant bones for making Acheulian handaxes: a fresh look at old bones. Quaternary International 406, 227–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. (A) Castel di Guido site (Italy): a handaxe made of an elephant bone. (Modified from Zutovski & Barkai 2016); (B) Schöningen site (Germany): a horse bone used as an anvil; (C): Schöningen site (Germany): a horse metacarpal with knapping damage (B & C modified from Kolfschoten et al.2015); (D) Schöningen site (Germany): detail of a spearhead. (Modified from Schoch et al.2015); (E) Trinil site (Java): shell tool. Scale bar 1 cm. (Modified by Joordens et al.2015.)