Skip to main content
Log in

Pseudo-ABA patterns in pronominal morphology

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, I present an analysis of pseudo-ABA patterns of morphology found in pronominal forms. I argue that an analysis that assumes unrestricted phonologically null allomorphy or unrestricted impoverishment overgenerates, allowing all the logically possible patterns of syncretism to appear. An analysis that includes spanned portmanteau exponents generates all and only the attested patterns of syncretism. Pseudo-ABA patterns arise when the complete pronominal tree (the structure for the anaphor) is exponed by a spanned exponent for [a [d]] and an exponent for p. Spanned portmanteau exponents are compatible with an analysis in which a and d are cyclic nodes, and one in which they are not. However, the analysis in which a and d are cyclic nodes is incompatible with another morphological behaviour of pronominal forms, namely variable exponence. To provide a unified analysis of pseudo-ABA patterns and variable exponence, a and d cannot be cyclic nodes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term anaphor for locally-bound variables is well-established, being derived from the Ancient Greek aná ‘up’ and phérō ‘I carry’. Continuing with Ancient Greek, diaphor is a neologism, formed from diá, ‘at variance’ and phérō. This ensures that the term diaphor is consistent with the rest of the phor family: anaphor, cataphor, endophor, exophor, logophor.

  2. I elicited the data for my (2020) survey in the following manner: The informant was told a story that disambiguates local binding (in which a person P is told about three people, X, Y and Z, such that X loves X, Y doesn’t love Y, and Z doesn’t love Z), and then asked to translate the corresponding sentence (P thinks that only X loves x-self) into their native language so that the translation is true for the story. This was then repeated for the stories and target sentences of the diaphor and pronoun. All data discussed in this paper can be found in the appendices of Middleton (2020): https://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/id/eprint/10105591/.

  3. Technically, the φ-features c-command p (Middleton 2020), as shown below when the 1st person pronouns of Tok Pisin, Dolakha Newar and Macushí are examined in (5), but as this level of decomposition is not necessary for the analysis of the patterns of syncretism found in pronouns, diaphors and anaphors, I don’t represent it in (2).

  4. This process is cyclic, but the amount of structure that falls into a single cycle can vary; the second half of this paper addresses the size of a cycle for pronominal structures.

  5. I will return to these examples later in the paper.

  6. It would also be possible to posit two phonologically overt allomorphs of p (, and òun in the presence of d), and have d realised by a phonologically null exponent; both analyses produce the required paradigm.

  7. I assume that all pronominals merge with the Case head K when complete.

  8. Thanks to Klaus Abels, Yasu Sudo and Stan Zompì for pointing this problem out to me.

  9. This is simply an extension of underspecification. The English 3\(^{ \text{rd}}\) person feminine accusative and 3\(^{\text{rd}}\) person feminine genitive pronouns are identical: her. This is accounted for in the Distributed Morphology framework by the case features acc and gen not receiving phonological exponence at PF when the pronoun is 3\(^{\text{rd}}\) person feminine. By extension, then, it is perfectly possible that a terminal node hosts a feature or bundle of features, none of which receive a phonological exponent at PF. In other words, there is no rule in Distributed Morphology like (i).

    figure i
  10. The seven possible realisations of the anaphor do not result in 14 possible ABC and ABA patterns, because two are ABB patterns: when both the diaphor and anaphor are {x y} or {y}.

  11. Exponents that spell out multiple nodes that form a constituent are simply a special type of span.

  12. A reviewer notes that there is the homophony loophole: a true or pseudo-ABA pattern of syncretism could arise if the exponent for [a [d [p]]] or the exponent for a is accidentally homophonous with the exponent for p. Bobaljik 2012 notes the same problem for *ABA suppletion patterns in the adjectival domain, and argues that while the grammar doesn’t exclude this possibility per se, there is a general antihomophony bias in acquisition which accounts for the absence of homophonous exponents (Bobaljik 2012:35).

  13. The alternative would be to conclude that the Aspect node is absent from the verbal tree when the Tense is Future; this would require an impoverishment rule deleting ASP in the context of FUT, which is impossible due to the Russian Doll Deletion Constraint (Ackema and Neeleman 2018); see Example (54) in Sect. 5.

  14. I will focus on the analysis in Bobaljik (2012), because the differences between this analysis and those in Embick (2010) and Moskal (2015) are to do with non-adjacent nodes conditioning suppletion. Since the data being investigated in the present paper concerns syncretism, not suppletion, Bobaljik’s (2012) analysis suffices to represent cyclic analyses more generally.

  15. An impoverishment rule can, of course, apply 100% of the time, which accounts for occasions when impoverishment rules appear to be deterministic.

References

  • Abbott, M. (1991). Macushi. In D. C. Derbyshire & G. K. Pullum (Eds.), Handbook of Amazonian languages 3 (pp. 23–160). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ackema, P., & Neeleman, A. (2018). Features of Person. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Adesola, O. P. (2006). A-bar dependencies in the Yoruba reference-tracking system. Lingua, 116(12), 2068–2106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, J. (2012). Universals in comparative morphology: Suppletion, superlatives, and the structure of words. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, J. (2000). The ins and outs of contextual allomorphy. University of Maryland, Working Papers in Linguistics, 10, 35–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caha, P. (2020). The Nanosyntax of Case. PhD Thesis, University of Tromso.

  • Chomsky, N. (2001). Derivation by Phase. In M. Kestowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A Life is Language (pp. 1–52). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, G. (1999). Adverbs and functional heads: A cross-linguistic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P., & Hermon, G. (2005). The typology of Malay reflexives. Lingua, 115(5), 627–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P., Hermon, G., Tjung, Y., Sim, C. Y., & Kim, C. (2007). Anaphoric expressions in the Peranakan Javanese of Semarang. Munich: Lincom.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, P., Hermon, G., & Yanti (2015). Grammar of binding in the languages of the world: Innate or learned? Cognition, 141, 138–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D. (2010). Localism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Marantz, A. (2008). Architecture and blocking. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 1–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genetti, C. (2007). A grammar of Dolakha Newar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J. (1963). Some universals of grammar with particular reference to the order of meaningful elements. In J. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals of language, 73–113. Cambridge: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), The view from building 20 (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harbour, D. (2014). Paucity, abundance, and the theory of number. Language, 90(1), 185–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harbour, D. (2016). Impossible persons. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, B. G. (1995). Georgian: A structural reference grammar (Vol. 2). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, P. (1973). “Elsewhere” in phonology. In S. A. Anderson & P. Kyparsky (Eds.), A Festschrift for Morris Halle (pp. 93–106). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, A. (2007). Phases and words. In Phases in the theory of grammar (pp. 191–222). Republic of Korea: Dong-In Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mascaró, J. (1976). Catalan phonology and the phonological cycle. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Middleton, H. J. (2020). *ABA syncretism patterns in pronominal morphology. PhD Thesis, University College London.

  • Moskal, B. A. (2015). Domains on the border: Between morphology and phonology. Storrs: University of Connecticut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moskal, B. (2018). Excluding exclusively the exclusive: Suppletion patterns in clusivity. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics, 3(1).

  • Nevins, A., & Parrott, J. K. (2010). Variable rules meet Impoverishment theory: Patterns of agreement leveling in English varieties. Lingua, 120(5), 1135–1159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, H. (2008). Aspects of the morphology and phonology of phases. Doctoral dissertation, McGill University.

  • Radkevich, N. (2010). On location: The structure of case and adpositions. Storrs: University of Connecticut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. W., Moskal, B., Xu, T., Kang, J., & Bobaljik, J. D. (2019). Case and number suppletion in pronouns. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 37(3), 1029–1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenonius, P. (2012). Spanning. Ms., University of Tromsø, available at ling.auf.net/lingBuzz/001501.

  • Veselinova, L. N. (2006). Suppletion in verb paradigms: Bits and pieces of the puzzle. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zompì, S. (2019). Ergative is not inherent: Evidence from *ABA in suppletion and syncretism. Glossa, A Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thank you to Jonathan Bobaljik, Daniel Harbour, Beata Moskal, Ad Neeleman, Stanislao Zompì and two anonymous reviewers for many helpful comments on and discussion of the ideas presented here. All errors are my own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Middleton, J. Pseudo-ABA patterns in pronominal morphology. Morphology 31, 329–354 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-021-09377-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-021-09377-7

Keywords

Navigation