Skip to main content
Log in

Life History-related Traits Predict Preferences for Dominant or Prestigious Leaders

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Evolutionary Psychological Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Dominance and prestige, as two distinct status-attaining qualities, are present in modern-day leaders at various levels of social hierarchies to various degrees. From an evolutionary perspective, we speculate that individuals’ preference for dominant (prestigious) leaders can be partly predicted by “fast” (“slow”) life history–related traits. Moreover, we predict that the link between fast traits and the preference for dominance would be stronger when individuals face uncontrollable dangers resembling the evolutionary challenges faced by our ancestors in a less structured and predictable world. Two experiments tested these speculations. Experiment 1 (N = 67) used the Implicit Association Test (IAT) technique and showed that people implicitly associate dominance (prestige) with negative (positive) evaluations, and such association was stronger for individuals exhibited slow life history–related psychosocial traits. Experiment 2 (N = 95) replicated this finding using explicit leader choices in response to hypothetical scenarios. Moreover, Experiment 2 demonstrated that individuals with faster psychosocial traits showed a stronger preference for dominant leaders in the face of experimentally primed danger than in a control condition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Data for both experiments reported in the manuscript is available on Mendeley Data at https://data.mendeley.com/datasets/zbgnrg5w7g/2.

Notes

  1. We do not regard our psychometric measure of life history in this research as reflecting human life-history strategy at population or species levels. Nor do we assume that these measures capture all the variances in individuals’ life history. Because these measures are theoretically linked to the fundamental tradeoff between current and future fitness, we adopted the recommended wording of Del Giudice (in press) and named the relevant variable in our studies “life history-related traits” to avoid the misconception.

References

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization: Nan Zhu, Binbin Chen, and Lei Chang; Methodology: Nan Zhu, Binbin Chen, and Hui Jing Lu; Formal analysis and investigation: Nan Zhu and Hui Jing Lu; Writing—original draft preparation: Nan Zhu; Writing—review and editing: Binbin Chen, Hui Jing Lu, and Lei Chang; Supervision: Lei Chang.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lei Chang.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Approval

The research was waived from ethical approval by the Institutional Review Board of Fudan University.

Consent to Participate

All participants signed a written consent form indicating their understanding of the research and their rights, which was read to them by the researchers before participating in the experiments.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 33 KB)

Appendix 1: Examples of the Leadership Preference Scenario Task (Experiment 2)

Appendix 1: Examples of the Leadership Preference Scenario Task (Experiment 2)

Example Scenario and Questions:

Students attending a natural science course are asked to elect a team leader for a group research assignment. Two students, A and T, were candidates for this role.

A is admired by others for being knowledgeable and skillful but is also shame and modest. He/she sometimes let go of rewards. However, when others encounter an academic problem and ask for his/her help, he/she would always answer it patiently. He/she would never demand others to do something and would not criticize or punish others for wrongdoing.

T is known to be assertive, proactive, and enjoys expression and controlling other individuals. He/she seeks opportunities aggressively regardless of what others may want. T is good at commanding other people and can pressure others into doing what he/she wants. T is arrogant toward those who are below him/her, and would not hesitate to criticize others for their wrongdoings.

Who would you choose to be the team leader?

□ A □ T □ Don’t Know.

Who would you expect to be the team leader, given your experience of similar situations?

□ A □ T □ Don’t Know.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zhu, N., Chen, B., Lu, H.J. et al. Life History-related Traits Predict Preferences for Dominant or Prestigious Leaders. Evolutionary Psychological Science 7, 284–297 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-020-00274-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-020-00274-6

Keywords

Navigation