Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T13:06:22.962Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive decline among European retirees: impact of early retirement, nation-related and personal characteristics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 March 2021

Sara Carmel*
Affiliation:
Center for Multidisciplinary Research in Aging, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel Department of Public Health, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel
Aviad Tur-Sinai
Affiliation:
Department of Health Systems Management, The Max Stern Yezreel Valley College, Yezreel Valley, Israel
*
*Corresponding author. Email: sara@bgu.ac.il
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Our study aimed to enhance understanding of memory decline (MD) in old age by evaluating longitudinal effects of personal and national contributing factors. We used data collected by the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) from 12 European countries and Israel. Our sample included 11,930 retirees aged 50+, interviewed at baseline and four years later. MD was evaluated by the change in the number of recalled words from first to second interview. Except for gender, all of our explanatory variables had a significant unique effect on MD – age, education, type of occupation, European geographical region, early retirement, time elapsed from retirement, reason for retirement, active lifestyle, re-employment, health/function status, depressive symptoms, and decline in physical and mental health – over the four years of the study. Our findings indicate that MD can be postponed by national policies such as those which prolong years of education and participation in the workforce, and by social interventions directed to promote active lifestyles in late life, especially in Mediterranean and Eastern European nations.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Ageing and Society

Introduction

Since the mid-20th century, we have been witness to a worldwide unprecedented and continuing increase in life expectancy. However, the achievement of reaching this long-desired goal is accompanied by a decline in physical and cognitive functioning, and attenuation in meaningful activities and social contacts in later life (e.g. World Health Organization (WHO), 2017a, 2017b; Zeng et al., Reference Zeng, Feng, Hesketh, Christensen and Vaupel2017). The harmful effects of these developments on the ever-growing population of older adults, their families and society impose significant challenges for ageing nations, some of which are striving to ensure a reasonable level of quality-of-life for all parties involved, while attempting to reduce the related economic burden.

The process of decline in health and functioning in later life is a multifactorial phenomenon affected by biological changes, physical and social environments, personality, education and lifestyle, along with cultural features and societal policies, many of which are interrelated (e.g. Scarmeas and Stern, Reference Scarmeas and Stern2003; Adam et al., Reference Adam, Bonsang, Germain and Perelman2007). Some of the detrimental effects of these aspects can be prevented or reduced by societal policies regarding retirement, pensions, taxation, etc. (Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Rechel et al., Reference Rechel, Grundy, Robine, Cylus, Mackenbach, Knai and McKee2013), as well as by psycho-social interventions on various societal levels – national, community and personal (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Hertzog, Small and Dixon1999; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017).

Currently, cognitive decline (CD) is one of the major problems faced by ageing societies. The prevalence of CD increases significantly with ageing (WHO, 2017a; Alzheimer's Association, 2018). With the exception of one study (Coe et al., Reference Coe, von Gaudecker, Lindeboom and Maurer2012), mandatory retirement and early retirement policies have been repeatedly reported as macro-factors that are negatively associated with people's cognitive functioning (e.g. Adam et al., Reference Adam, Bonsang, Germain and Perelman2007; Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Stachowsky, Infurna, Faul, Grosch and Tetrick2014; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss, Gronseth, Marson, Pringsheim, Day and Sager2018). For many people, retirement is a significant event causing major changes in routine life, which may accelerate the progression of CD (Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012). However, two more recent meta-analyses (Meng et al., Reference Meng, Nexo and Borg2017; Zulka et al., Reference Zulka, Hansson and Hassing2019) report conflicting findings regarding the relationship between retirement and CD across large-scale international studies. Such inconsistencies lead to a need for studies directed towards understanding the exact mechanisms which can explain the contradictory relationships between retirement and CD.

One of the explanations for these inconsistencies can be the fact that most of the studies in this area did not include relevant life changes after retirement and their positive or negative influences on CD (e.g. in Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Zulka et al., Reference Zulka, Hansson and Hassing2019). For instance, there are indications that an active lifestyle including leisure activities, cognitive stimulation and physical exercise can moderate and even improve cognitive functioning in later life and after retirement (e.g. Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006; Hertzog et al., Reference Hertzog, Kramer, Wilson and Lindenberger2008; Koepsell and Monsell, Reference Koepsell and Monsell2012; Lifshitz-Vahav et al., Reference Lifshitz-Vahav, Shrira and Bodner2017). Further research is therefore needed in order to enhance this body of knowledge.

The purpose of this study is to evaluate longitudinal relationships between a series of well-established correlates of CD and memory decline (MD) among retirees living in various European countries. We intend to assess the unique long-term associations of a series of national, personal and behavioural factors with MD, as well as the associations of changes in relevant factors over four years.

CD in old age

Dementia and depression are the most common mental health diseases in late life (WHO, 2017b). These conditions are often interrelated (Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011), and both lead to decline in functioning of older adults and in their independence. High levels of incidence and prevalence of dementia diseases are reported globally (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017). By the end of 2017, approximately 50 million people worldwide were estimated to have dementia, with an estimated 10 million additional people diagnosed with dementia each year (WHO, 2017a). The prevalence of dementia diseases increases sharply with age (Ferri et al., Reference Ferri, Prince, Brayne, Brodaty, Fratiglioni, Ganguli, Hall, Hasegawa, Hendrie, Huang and Jorm2005; WHO, 2017a; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss, Gronseth, Marson, Pringsheim, Day and Sager2018). For instance, in North American and European countries, this percentage increases from a range of 1.3–1.7 per cent among people aged 65–69 to a range of 24.5–30.1 per cent among people aged 85 and older (Ferri et al., Reference Ferri, Prince, Brayne, Brodaty, Fratiglioni, Ganguli, Hall, Hasegawa, Hendrie, Huang and Jorm2005). Cognitive impairment conditions are especially difficult to cope with (Reisberg et al., Reference Reisberg, Ferris, De Leon and Crook1982; George and Gwyther, Reference George and Gwyther1986) due to the symptoms involved, which include losses in memory, orientation in time and place, personality and communication abilities, and later in the process, also decline in physical functioning, all of which lead to complete dependency (Alzheimer's Association, 2018). The current absence of effective medical means for prevention or cure of dementia diseases intensifies the threat of dementia and the related burden and frustration for all parties involved.

Facets of cognitive impairment in old age

Cognitive impairment is assessed based on various dimensions such as memory, fluency, reasoning, executive functioning, numeric abilities, speed of processing, etc. (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1985; Banks and Mazzonna, Reference Banks and Mazzonna2012; Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012). Our study focuses only on the weakening of memory, which is one of the earliest symptoms to be recognised as a sign of decline in cognitive functioning. This symptom is often overlooked because the onset of forgetfulness is gradual (WHO, 2017a), begins rather early in life (Schaie, Reference Schaie1989) and is often perceived as a part of normal ageing. In the last century, various types of memory have been studied, including primary, procedural, semantic and declarative (e.g. Tulving, Reference Tulving and Gazzaniga1995). One of the main distinctions in memory types is between episodic memory referring to long-term memory of experienced events that can be located in time and space, and working memory defined as the temporary maintenance, storage and updating of representations. Weakening in both types of memory begins around age 60 and continues to worsen with age (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012).

A diagnosis of mild decline in a number of cognitive functions including memory and orientation in time and/or familiar places is called Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) (WHO, 2017a). The prevalence of MCI increases with age, starting with 6.7 per cent among the young-old (aged 60–64) and escalating to more than 25 per cent in the age range of 80–84 (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss, Gronseth, Marson, Pringsheim, Day and Sager2018). People diagnosed with MCI are at increased risk of developing dementia diseases over a relatively short period in comparison to people in the same age group who are not diagnosed with MCI (Roberts and Knopman, Reference Roberts and Knopman2013; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Lopez, Armstrong, Getchius, Ganguli, Gloss, Gronseth, Marson, Pringsheim, Day and Sager2018). However, people with MCI may benefit from interventions such as cognitive training and physical exercise, which can slow the pace of further decline and, in some cases, even improve the clinical expression of cognitive impairment (Koepsell and Monsell, Reference Koepsell and Monsell2012; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017). For this reason, early diagnosis of MCI is important.

National and personal factors related to CD

National policies and cultural aspects play an important role in decisions that may appear to be purely related to personal choice. Mandatory retirement and benefits provided to early retirees are good examples of macro factors influencing personal choices. Considering that retirement accelerates decline in cognitive functioning, early retirement causes this degenerative process to commence even earlier (Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012). Furthermore, according to Rohwedder and Willis (Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010), CD may even begin when people are still on the job – known as ‘on-the-job retirement’. This happens because awareness of approaching retirement causes workers to stop investing in work-related training and developmental activities. Study results indicate that the largest drop in CD occurs with retirement (Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012), and that delaying retirement by one year is associated with a 3.1 per cent lower risk of dementia even when adjusting for potential confounders (Dufouil et al., Reference Dufouil, Pereira, Chêne, Glymour, Alpérovitch, Saubusse, Risse-Fleury, Heuls, Salord, Brieu and Forette2014). These findings, along with the indication that CD increases the longer people are in retirement (Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012), lead to the conclusion that national encouragement to delay retirement for as long as possible has the potential to postpone CD among elderly persons (Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010). However, longitudinal comparisons among countries on these issues are lacking (Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012).

An additional factor which exemplifies a country's influence on personal choices is the educational system. For instance, Banks and Mazzonna (Reference Banks and Mazzonna2012) report that the reform in England of adding one year of mandatory education resulted in improved cognitive functioning years later, especially among less-educated men. The positive roles of education and occupational attainment in the preservation of cognitive functioning endure even after retirement (Scarmeas and Stern, Reference Scarmeas and Stern2003; Banks and Mazzonna, Reference Banks and Mazzonna2012; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017; Stern, Reference Stern2012; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Stachowsky, Infurna, Faul, Grosch and Tetrick2014; Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Oris, Fagot, Maggiori and Kliegel2016). Stern (Reference Stern2012) suggests that higher levels of education and lifelong experience in occupational attainments and/or intensive leisure activities provide a greater cognitive reserve, which in turn increases tolerance to pathology and enables ageing people to be more resilient to degenerative brain changes. Such reserves delay or mask the clinical appearance of dementia symptoms. Thus, people with higher levels of education and intellectually stimulating cognitive activities such as those in higher levels of occupation prior to retirement, suffer from greater underlying disease pathology (Meng and D'Arcy, Reference Meng and D'Arcy2012), and a steeper level of CD, in comparison to people with less cognitive reserve (Stern, Reference Stern2012).

Regarding physical and mental health, studies show that changes in mood, exposure to difficult life events or acute physical conditions have been reported as causes of temporary and reversible CD (e.g. Fong et al., Reference Fong, Tulebaev and Inouye2009). More severe and enduring decline in cognitive performance has been found to correlate with decline in objectively evaluated physical health (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017; Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Ghisletta, Ballhausen, Fagot, Vallet, Baeriswyl, Sauter, Oris, Maurer and Kliegel2018), self-perceived health, the presence of stress, chronic psychological distress and behavioural disorders such as apnea (e.g. Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Caracciolo, Brayne, Gauthier, Jelic and Fratiglioni2014), as well as more severe conditions including various chronic diseases and comorbidity with its related polypharmacy (De Ronchi et al., Reference De Ronchi, Palmer, Pioggiosi, Atti, Berardi, Ferrari, Dalmonte and Fratiglioni2007; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017; Alzheimer's Association, 2018; Mangin et al., Reference Mangin, Bahat, Golomb, Mallery, Moorhouse, Onder, Petrovic and Garfinkel2018). In some studies, a causal relationship has been demonstrated between emotional disorders and cognitive performance. For instance, depression was reported as a risk factor, prodromal and accompanying symptom of CD (Paterniti et al., Reference Paterniti, Verdier-Taillefer, Dufouil and Alpérovitch2002; Dotson et al., Reference Dotson, Beydoun and Zonderman2010) and Alzheimer's dementia (Enache et al., Reference Enache, Winblad and Aarsland2011).

Concerning demographic characteristics, as presented above, the most conspicuous relationship is reported between age and the process of ageing and CD, while inconsistent findings are reported for the association between gender and CD (e.g. Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Caracciolo, Brayne, Gauthier, Jelic and Fratiglioni2014; Ding et al., Reference Ding, Zhao, Qihao Guo, Meng, Wang, Luo, Mortimer, Borenstein and Hong2015).

National differences in cognitive functioning

Countries differ in reported incidence and prevalence of MCI (Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Roberts and Knopman, Reference Roberts and Knopman2013), as well as in the prevalence of various types of CD. For instance, an analysis of data from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) indicates that except for orientation in time, people living in Mediterranean countries have lower scores in all cognitive domains (Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012). The reasons for such differences may derive from the multiplicity of cultural, national and personal factors affecting CD. On the national level, factors such as employment, pension and taxation policies, political conditions, health and education systems, cultural perception such as preference for active or inactive lifestyle, and nutrition habits following retirement may be associated with patterns of behaviour which in turn influence cognitive functioning. For instance, although active lifestyle is associated with lower risk for CD, people in Southern European countries have opposite perceptions regarding leading an active lifestyle after retirement compared to people in Northern countries (Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008). Knowledge about the effects of such cultural aspects on CD following retirement is limited and needs to be expanded upon.

On the personal level, national variability in cognitive functioning may be explained by genetics, physical and mental health status, subjective wellbeing and health-related lifestyle – including level of involvement in physical, intellectual and social activities. For instance, lower estimates of MCI prevalence in Finland and Germany compared to the USA on comparable measures (Ward et al., Reference Ward, Arrighi, Michels and Cedarbaum2012) can be explained by various interactions among the national characteristics related to CD in old age (retirement policies, education systems and other relevant policies), as well as genetic and lifestyle-related cultural features.

The diversity in incidence and prevalence of MCI among countries may also derive from methodological aspects such as lack of agreement on terminology of MCI (Ward et al., Reference Ward, Arrighi, Michels and Cedarbaum2012), differences in the criteria used for diagnosing MCI, and in research methods including variability in study designs, samples and statistical analyses of specific age groups (Roberts and Knopman, Reference Roberts and Knopman2013).

Previous international comparisons of CD determinants focused mainly on national regulations governing mandatory retirement policies, pension eligibility rules, taxation, and economic characteristics such as income, consumption and wealth, job characteristics and educational systems (e.g. Adam et al., Reference Adam, Bonsang, Germain and Perelman2007; Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008; Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Banks and Mazzonna, Reference Banks and Mazzonna2012; Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Stachowsky, Infurna, Faul, Grosch and Tetrick2014; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017). Less attention has been paid to the effects of health systems and cultural and health-related behaviours on CD, as well as to the interactions among these factors (Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008; Winblad et al., Reference Winblad, Amouyel, Andrieu, Ballard, Brayne, Brodaty and Fratiglioni2016).

Based on the previous reports’ findings regarding associations between retirement and CD, and related explanative theories such as ‘cognitive reserve’, ‘use it or lose it’ or ‘on-the-job retirement’, as well as the rather scarce literature on factors attenuating CD, our study aims to simultaneously assess the unique longitudinal contribution of early retirement and a series of established personal and national policy characteristics to the explanation of changes in MD over a relatively short period (four years) among European retirees living in various countries. Specifically, we hypothesised that among retirees, the explanatory power of early retirement as a factor associated with memory change after four years would remain statistically significant even when controlling for indicators of physical and mental health, lifestyle, socio-demographic characteristics, time since retirement, reason for retirement and the changes in these factors over four years, as well as national affiliation. We also hypothesised that in these multivariate analyses the explanatory power of early retirement would differ among retirees living in groups of nations which vary in their welfare regimes, but would remain powerful in each of these groups and add to the explanation of MD.

Method

Participants and procedures

For this study we used the SHARE database. SHARE is a multi-disciplinary, cross-national bank of survey data on health, psycho-social and economic variables gathered in several waves (Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brandt, Hunkler, Kneip, Korbmacher, Malter, Schaan, Stuck and Zuber2013).

The current study is based on data from two interviews collected at approximately four-year intervals. For the 12 European countries included in this study (Austria, Germany, Sweden, Netherlands, Spain, Italy, France, Denmark, Switzerland, Belgium, Czech Republic, Poland), we used data from Wave 2 (Time 1 (T1) in our study – gathered during 2006–2007) and from Wave 4 (Time 2 (T2) in our study – collected in 2011). For Israel, we used data from Wave 1 (T1 – collected during 2005–2006) and from Wave 2 (T2 – 2009–2010). This enabled us to estimate longitudinal relationships between variables evaluated at baseline and MD, as well as changes over a four-year period in some of the relevant variables and MD. Our sample was comprised of 11,930 persons aged 50+ who were retirees at T1 and were interviewed again four years later.

Measures

Memory change: number of recalled words

In each of the SHARE waves a list of ten words was read to the participants, who were asked to immediately repeat all of the words that they remembered. Next, they were asked some questions on other topics following which they were asked to repeat the previously presented words that they still remembered. The final score of recalled words was the sum of the number of words remembered both times, ranging from 0 (no words remembered) to 20 (all words remembered). A more detailed explanation regarding the methodological aspects of measuring cognitive ability using the SHARE data is provided by Börsch-Supan et al. (Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008). Our dependent variable was calculated as the change in the recalled words from T1 to T2. The scale for this variable ranged from −20 indicating that the participant did not remember any of the words at T1, but remembered them all at T2 (= maximal cognitive gain), 0 denoting that the participant remembered the same number of words at both times (no change) and +20 denoting that the participant remembered all of the words at T1 and none at T2 (= maximal CD).

Socio-demographic characteristics

Data were collected on age, gender, education (years of schooling), early retirement from the labour force, years from retirement at T1 and job before retirement. Early retirement was constructed as a dichotomous variable. First, age of retirement was calculated based on the reported date of retirement and date of birth. This actual retirement age was then compared to the official retirement age in the participant's country in the year that the person retired. The final dummy variable was constructed as 0 when the participant retired on time or belatedly, according to the official retirement age, or 1 when he or she retired early (Litwin and Tur-Sinai, Reference Litwin and Tur-Sinai2015). Years from retirement were estimated by the years that passed since the participant retired from the labour force until T1. Job before retirement was constructed as a dichotomous variable: 0 when the participant was considered a blue-collar worker such as a skilled agricultural or fishery worker, craft and related trades worker, plant and machine operator or assembler, elementary occupation, etc., and 1 when the participant engaged in a white-collar job such as a legislator, senior official or manager, professional, technician or associate professional, clerk, service worker, shop and market sales worker, etc.

Physical and mental health status

Health variables at T1 included self-rated health on a 1–5 scale (ranging from 1 excellent to 5 poor); disability in activities of daily living (ADL) was assessed by the number of reported difficulties with getting dressed, bathing, ambulating at home, eating, sitting down and standing up, and using the bathroom; instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) were assessed by the number of reported difficulties with performing daily tasks related to maintaining an independent lifestyle, this includes difficulty with preparing hot meals, shopping for groceries, making telephone calls, taking medications, doing work around the house or garden and managing money; depressive symptoms were evaluated by the EURO-D scale – a validated measure of depression, which is based on assessments of 12 self-reported depressive symptoms such as trouble sleeping, irritability, appetite, fatigue and ability to concentrate. The scale ranges from 0 to 12 (Prince et al., Reference Prince, Reischies, Beekman, Fuhrer, Jonker, Kivela, Lawlor, Lobo, Magnusson, Fichter and Van Oyen1999).

Active lifestyle

Active lifestyle was assessed by the number of reported activities from a list including volunteer labour, charitable activity, in-service or training activity, involvement in sports clubs, social clubs or some other club, religious organisations, or some political or community activity; post-retirement employment was assessed based on responses to a question asking about the participant's employment status at T2 (scored as 0 for not being employed and 1 for being employed); reason for retirement was assessed based on responses to the question: ‘for which reasons did you retire?’ Responses to this question received one of three scores: 0 for retirement due to structural reasons (e.g. becoming eligible for public pension, private occupational pension or private pension, having been offered an early retirement option/window with special incentives, or being made redundant); 1 for retirement due to health reasons (retiring because of one's own ill health, or the ill health of a relative or friend); 2 for both structural and health reasons (at least one of the structural reasons in addition to at least one of the health reasons). The list of response options also included wishes to retire at the same time as a spouse, spend more time with family and to enjoy life. Because only 61 participants reported any of these last three reasons, we did not include them in our analyses.

Country/region

The 13 countries included in the SHARE data were divided into four geographical regions according to classification based on groupings of countries with similar socio-economic regimes (Antonova et al., Reference Antonova, Aranda, Havari and Pace2015): Eastern (Czech Republic, Poland), Central (Austria, Germany, France, Switzerland, Belgium), Northern (Sweden, Netherlands, Denmark) and Mediterranean (Spain, Italy, Israel).

Changes in physical and mental health and functioning

Changes were assessed based on responses to the same questions at T1 and T2. The potential scale for change in self-rated health ranged from −5 to +5, for change in ADL from −6 to +6; for change in IADL the scale ranged from −7 to +7; for change in depressive symptom the scale ranged from −12 to +12. The negative scores denoted improvement in health/function at T2, 0 denoted no change, and positive scores denoted decline in health and/or function.

Changes in active lifestyle

Changes were assessed based on responses at T1 and T2 to the same question regarding number of various activities. The potential scale for changes in active lifestyle ranged from −7 to +7: negative scores denoting engagement in more activities at T2, 0 denoting no change and positive scores denoting engagement in less activities at T2.

Data analysis methods

Univariate associations between MD – the dependent variable – and the independent explanatory variables were examined using Pearson, Spearman or chi-square tests. Hierarchical multivariate linear regression analyses were performed to explain changes in memory over the four-year period. In the first ordinary least squares model, the change in memory was analysed as a function of the series of explanatory variables assessed at T1, and the changes that occurred in them between T1 and T2. To the second model, we added the variables of active lifestyle after retirement, change in active lifestyle and employment. To the third model we added the blocks of countries divided according to welfare regimes and to the fourth model we added interactions between the welfare regimes and early retirement status. Data were analysed using Stata version 15.1 (StataCorp, College Station, TX).

Results

Descriptive characteristics of the research population at baseline (T1) are presented in Table 1. The average number of words remembered (about nine out of 20) indicates medium or poor retention. More than 87 per cent (N = 10,379) of respondents noted that they had retired from work at or after the official age. Among participants who retired early, the average number of years of potential work lost due to early retirement was 7.3 years (standard deviation (SD) = 4.9). The average age was close to 69 (SD = 9.2, range = 52–84) and more than half of the participants were women. The average number of years of education was 11.2 (SD = 4.3). About 70 per cent of the participants rated their health as ‘good’, ‘very good’ or ‘excellent’, and most participants reported no ADL or IADL limitations. The reported number of depressive symptoms was very low as well. About 77 per cent of the respondents reported that they had retired due to structural reasons, 19 per cent because of health reasons and 4 per cent due to both structural and health reasons. Less than half of the participants reported having an active lifestyle.

Table 1. Comparison between the study participants (interviewed at Time (T1) and Time (T2)) and those who dropped out of the study at T2

Notes: SD: standard deviation. ADL: activities of daily living. IADL: instrumental activities of daily living.

Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

A significant number of participants (4,363) dropped out at T2, most of them due to non-response (81.3%) and others due to death (18.7%). In this study, we included data only from the retirees who participated in the study at both T1 and T2. Examination shows that the number of recalled words among those who responded at both times was significantly higher than among the drop-out respondents at T1. Compared to participants who remained in the study, people who dropped out, despite being younger in age, were a weaker group in terms of physical and mental health and functioning, and were less active in their daily lives (Table 1). In addition, among those who participated at both T1 and T2, we excluded a relatively small number of people (61) who reported that they retired early in order to spend more time with family, to enjoy life or at the same time as their spouses – categories which had no relevance to health or laws for mandatory retirement.

A comparison among social and geographical regions for the T1 study variables is presented in Table 2. The average score for memory was significantly higher among retirees who lived in the Northern European bloc of countries in comparison to that of retirees who lived in all other regions, especially in comparison to those who lived in the Mediterranean countries. Regarding early retirement, while 18.5 per cent of participants from Eastern European countries retired before reaching the official retirement age, only around 11 per cent reported early retirement in the other regions. With the exception of age and gender, the comparison among geographical regions revealed significant variance in most socio-demographic characteristics and health factors. While the average education level of participants living in Northern and Central European countries was nearly identical, it was significantly lower among retirees living in Eastern European and Mediterranean countries. Nearly 49 per cent of participants from Eastern European countries and about 35 per cent from Mediterranean countries assessed their state of health as medium or poor, compared to only around 21–26 per cent of participants living in Northern and Central European countries. Accordingly, participants from Eastern European countries also reported a significantly higher number of ADL and IADL limitations compared to participants living in the other European regions. The level of depressive symptoms was also higher among participants living in the Eastern European countries than among those living in Northern Europe.

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics (at Time 1) of the sample by blocks of similar nations

Notes: SD: standard deviation. ADL: activities of daily living. IADL: instrumental activities of daily living.

Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

Regarding reasons for retirement, while 23.5 per cent of participants from Northern countries reported retiring due to health problems, similar reasons were reported only by around 13.9 per cent in Eastern European countries. Retiring for structural reasons was reported by 82.7 per cent of participants from Eastern European countries compared to around 71 per cent in Northern countries. The number of years since retirement was higher among participants living in Mediterranean and Eastern European countries than among participants living in Northern Europe. In all four regions, most of the retirees were employed in white-collar jobs before retirement; a higher proportion of those employed in blue-collar jobs before retirement were from Eastern European countries. The average score for leading an active lifestyle was higher among the participants from Northern Europe and lower among the participants from Eastern Europe.

Turning to the dynamic aspects of the study, the distribution of the memory change variable was found to be close to normal, with an average score of −0.052 (SD = 3.45) and a median of 0 (see Figure 1). Over the years, a stable level of memory was found among only 13 per cent of the participants, whereas the proportion of participants whose memory improved (about 43%) was found to be almost identical (44%) to that of participants with MD (Table 3). The distributions of the changes in the study variables show that they are centred to the mean (see Figure 2).

Figure 1. Distribution of change in memory during the four years of the study among retirees.

Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Figure 2. Changes in study variables between Time 1 and Time 2. (a) Self-perceived health change; (b) depressive symptoms change; (c) activities of daily living change; (d) instrumental activities of daily living change.

Table 3. Changes in the study variables between Time 1 and Time 2 (T2)

Notes: ADL: activities of daily living. IADL: instrumental activities of daily living.

Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Significance level: *** p < 0.001.

Around 32 per cent of participants reported deterioration in their self-rated health, while 23 per cent reported an improvement. Deterioration in ADL was found among 8.6 per cent and improvement in ADL among 5 per cent of participants. Deterioration in IADL was found among 13.1 per cent of participants and improvement in IADL was found among 6.2 per cent of participants. Depressive symptoms worsened among 40 per cent of the participants but improvement was reported by 35 per cent of them. About 25 per cent of participants in all regions reported that they returned to the labour force over the years and 43.5 per cent reported improvement in active lifestyle (Table 3).

A comparison among the geographical regions of European countries showed an inter-regional variance. The Northern countries were typified by the highest percentage of participants who reported decline in their memory over time, whereas the percentage of those who reported improvement in memory was found to be highest in Central European countries. This is probably because the average score of recalled words at baseline was highest in Northern nations. However, the ratio of participants who noted deterioration in ADL or in IADL was lowest in Northern countries and highest in Eastern European countries. The highest percentage of improvement in active lifestyle was reported in Eastern nations (61.8%) and the lowest in Mediterranean countries (35.5%). Daily involvement in sports or social clubs, and volunteering were the most frequently reported activities (16.7 and 16.6%, respectively). The ratio of participants who retired but returned to work was found to be lowest (15.3%) in Eastern European countries and highest (31.7%) in the Northern countries.

Univariate analyses between the independent variables and memory change showed that except for gender, all of the study variables significantly correlated with memory change (MC). More specifically, a significantly greater MD was found among participants who reported early retirement along with lack of active lifestyle or who reported stopping their active lifestyle at T2, while returning to employment after retirement correlated with decreases in MD. Participants who reported that their reason for retirement was related to health problems were found to have a significantly greater decline in memory compared to participants who retired due to structural reasons. Poor physical and mental health variables and the declines in them after four years correlated significantly with MD, so that the worse the reported health and function (self-rated health, ADL and IADL) at T1, and the greater the decline in them, as well as the increase in depressive symptoms, the greater also the decline in memory.

Hierarchical regression models were constructed to assess the unique contribution of each of the study variables, including changes in some of them over the four years of the study (changes in active lifestyle, employment, and physical and mental health), to the explanation of change in the number of remembered words between T1 and T2 (Table 4).

Table 4. Ordinary least squares regression models explaining memory change in number of recalled words between Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2)

Notes: The dependent variable is the change in memory (number of recalled words) between T1 and T2. Higher score represents a decline in memory. Higher score in each health variable (including the change variables) represents a poorer condition. 1. Gender: 1 = male, 0 = female. 2. Aged 50+ at T1. 3. Retirement reason: 0 = structural reason, 1 = health reason, 2 = structural and health reasons. 4. Job before retirement: 0 = blue collar, 1 = white collar. ADL: activities of daily living. IADL: instrumental activities of daily living. Ref.: reference category.

Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Significance levels: * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

The results of the regression analyses (Model 1) indicate that the higher the number of words recalled at T1, the greater the MD at T2 (β = 0.564). While gender was not related to memory change over time, we found that memory declined with age (β = 0.192) but improved the higher the participants’ level of education was (β = −0.148). Regarding health and function, the poorer the reported health was at T1, the greater the MD was over time (β = 0.081). Similarly, decline in self-rated health over time correlated with MD over time (β = 0.057). The more IADL limitations a person had at T1, the greater the MD at T2 (β = 0.079). Increase in IADL limitations over time was also related to decline in memory (β = 0.109). The higher the reported number of depressive symptoms at T1, the greater the MD at T2 (β = 0.053). Consistently, the worse a person's depression was after four years, the greater the memory loss (β = 0.080). MD was higher among participants who reported retiring due to health issues or because of a combination of health and structural reasons. Although we controlled for all of the above-mentioned personal variables, as well as the changes in them over time, MD was greater among participants who reported early retirement at baseline (β = 0.048). The higher the number of years from retirement at T1, the greater the MD at T2 (β = 0.023). Finally, MD was higher among participants who were employed in blue-collar jobs than among those who were employed in white-collar jobs before retirement (β = −0.005) (Table 4, Model 1). In the second regression equation we added the variables of active lifestyle, change in active lifestyle and employment after retirement from the labour force (Table 4, Model 2). Our results also indicate that compared to active retirees, participants who were not active in their daily lives (such as in volunteering, charitable service, in-service or training activities, physical exercise, social activities, religious or political organisation, or other community activities) or whose number of fields of activities decreased during the four years of the study (at T2) reported greater MD (β = −0.085 and β = 0.053, respectively). Participants who chose to return to the labour force after retirement suffered from less MD over time (β = −0.009). Model 3 examines the contribution of living in a certain group of countries, beyond the contributions of all of the other personal variables. In contrast to the previous finding based on a univariate comparison of all retirees in the four European regions (Table 3), which indicated that MD was greatest in Northern countries, this model shows that when early retirement is controlled for, the degree of MD over time is lower among retirees living in the Central or Northern regions compared to retirees living in the Mediterranean region of Europe (β = −0.075 and β = −0.044, respectively). Finally, in order to assess whether geographic region of residence has a specific contribution to MD among early retirees compared to people who retired at or after the official retirement age, we added an interaction variable between region of residence and early retirement. Since this variable captures all possibilities of early retirement, it should not be generalised together with the ‘early retirement’ variable; therefore, we removed the latter variable from the frame of estimation in the model. Model 4 shows that in all the geographical regions the tendency for MD over time is stronger among participants who reported early retirement in comparison to participants who retired on time or later. It should be noted that although the increase in the explained variability in Models 3 and 4 is quite low, the unique contributions of the additional variables are statistically significant.

Discussion

This study was designed to evaluate determinants of MD after four years among European and Israeli retirees by simultaneously assessing the explanatory power of a relatively wide series of personal and nation-related characteristics evaluated at baseline (socio-demographic, physical and mental health, active lifestyle, early retirement, reason for retirement, time passed from retirement and national social policy/geographical region), as well as the explanatory power of changes in active lifestyle, and physical and mental health four years after retirement.

Our results lend support to some of the previous studies by showing that in general, gender is not related to MD. For instance, although our findings refer only to MD, no significant gender difference has been found in the prevalence or incidence of Alzheimer's disease, which is the most prevalent dementia disease in old age (Hebert et al., Reference Hebert, Scherr, McCann, Beckett and Evans2001). Also, similar to previous reports (e.g. Spaniol et al., Reference Spaniol, Madden and Voss2006; Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012), we found that ‘working memory’ does decline with ageing among retirees aged 50 years and older. After a follow-up of only four years, a significant decline in working memory occured among about 44 per cent of European retirees, which indicates that in our range of ages (52–84), this process occurs quite rapidly and supports previous findings regarding retirees (e.g. Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017). However, we also found a significant percentage of retirees whose memory had improved over time (43%), along with improvements in health and depressive symptoms (among about 32 and 35%, respectively). This raises the importance of studying the factors that contribute to both the decline and the improvement in memory over time, factors which may not always be the same. For instance, while CD can occur following retirement (Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010; Bonsang et al., Reference Bonsang, Adam and Perelman2012), improvements can derive from interventions directed towards enhancing specific areas of functioning, such as cognitive exercise (e.g. Koepsell and Monsell, Reference Koepsell and Monsell2012; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017). Interventions such as these may be of particular importance after retirement.

Results of our univariate and multivariate analyses further indicate that MD is greater among early retirees than among people who retire on time or later, as reported earlier (e.g. Rohwedder and Willis, Reference Rohwedder and Willis2010). Also, MD is greater among people who have retired earlier due to health problems, and among retirees who stay in retirement longer, in support of Mazzonna and Peracchi's (Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017) findings. The decline in cognitive functioning that comes after leaving the workforce probably derives from the related losses of social relationships, purpose in life, self-esteem and decline in income, all of which are major contributors to people's subjective wellbeing (e.g. Carmel, Reference Carmel2001; Diener et al., Reference Diener, Oishi and Lucas2003; Carmel et al., Reference Carmel, Granek and Zamir2016). These factors, along with various indicators of subjective wellbeing, have significant direct and indirect effects on mental and physical health (e.g. Steptoe and Wardle, Reference Steptoe and Wardle2005; Blanchflower and Oswald, Reference Blanchflower and Oswald2008; Tovel et al., Reference Tovel, Carmel and Raveis2017). Accordingly, and similar to previous reports (Davydow et al., Reference Davydow, Zivin, Katon, Pontone, Chwastiak, Langa and Iwashyna2014; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Caracciolo, Brayne, Gauthier, Jelic and Fratiglioni2014; Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Alzheimer's Association, 2018), we also found significant associations between indicators of physical health/functioning and depression, and between depression evaluated at baseline, and MD after four years. These identified associations are in line with previous reports about the associations between depression and CD (e.g. Steffens and Potter, Reference Steffens and Potter2008; Dotson et al., Reference Dotson, Beydoun and Zonderman2010; Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011).

The bidirectional relationships between MD and physical or emotional health were also found in evaluating the changes in these variables over time, so that the greater the decline in physical health and functioning and in depressive symptoms during the four years of study, the greater the MD was. We also found support for previous reports on the attenuating power of both level of education and occupational attainments for CD (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017). These repeated findings lend support to the theory of cognitive reserve, according to which people with high levels of education, and people who have careers with demanding intellectual requirements, accumulate a cognitive reserve that enables them to be more resilient in the face of degenerative brain changes (Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Oris, Fagot, Maggiori and Kliegel2016). The cognitive reserve often masks the underlying disease pathology (Meng and D'Arcy, Reference Meng and D'Arcy2012), causing steeper CD (Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Ghisletta, Ballhausen, Fagot, Vallet, Baeriswyl, Sauter, Oris, Maurer and Kliegel2018) and delayed diagnoses of dementia (Stern, Reference Stern2012). Accordingly, we found that type of occupation – white- versus blue-collar employment, which regularly correlates with level of education – has a unique contribution to MD, so that MD is greater among blue-collar workers in comparison to white-collar workers. This finding contradicts the finding of Mazzonna and Peracchi (Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2017). However, our studies differ in almost all the methodologically important factors: the composition of the sample, the years of the SHARE data collection, the included countries, the outcome variable, and probably the internal composition and frequencies of the various occupations within each group of workers.

As expected, based on previous findings (e.g. Scarmeas and Stern, Reference Scarmeas and Stern2003; Adam et al., Reference Adam, Bonsang, Germain and Perelman2007; Hertzog et al., Reference Hertzog, Kramer, Wilson and Lindenberger2008; Banks and Mazzonna, Reference Banks and Mazzonna2012; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012; Meng and D'Arcy, Reference Meng and D'Arcy2012; Lifshitz-Vahav et al., Reference Lifshitz-Vahav, Shrira and Bodner2017), an active physical and/or social lifestyle after retirement was also found to correlate with MD.

Following our division of European countries according to a classification based on four groupings of countries with similar socio-economic characteristics (Antonova et al., Reference Antonova, Aranda, Havari and Pace2015), our results indicate that while the four regions do not differ in age or gender, retirees’ memory is better and the level of education is higher in the Northern and Central countries compared to the Mediterranean countries. In addition, the highest percentage of early retirees is reported in Eastern countries, as well as the lowest percentage of participants who are employed at T2. Furthermore, our findings also show the highest prevalence of ADL limitations and depressive symptoms among retirees in this same block of countries. In addition, the percentage of people reporting at T2 that they are leading an active lifestyle is highest in Northern and Central European countries and lowest in the Mediterranean countries. All of these findings indicate that in comparison to the Northern and Central European nations, Eastern European nations and to a lesser degree the Mediterranean countries are disadvantaged in fundamental indicators of quality of life, including education and physical and mental health status. Furthermore, according to our findings and previous reports (e.g. Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008; Mazzonna and Peracchi, Reference Mazzonna and Peracchi2012), these nations rank lowest on the factors that can attenuate CD, including education level, retiring on time or later, and maintaining an active lifestyle. Thus, the identified differences in memory level between the Northern and Central European nations in comparison to the Eastern and Mediterranean nations can be partially explained by these nations’ rankings on both types of factors: factors associated with CD and those associated with preservation of cognitive functions or postponing CD.

Regarding MD over a four-year period, our findings indicate that the regional division of countries adds an additional unique and statistically significant contribution to the explanation of memory change, even when controlling for factors such as age, education, physical and mental health, early retirement, time from retirement, reason for retiring and active lifestyle, all of which are significantly associated with MD and well-established correlates of CD. The contribution to the explanation of MD gained through dividing the examined countries into blocks, although relatively small, leads us to suggest the existence of additional socio-cultural features relevant to MD. We grouped the various countries based mainly on a socio-economic perspective (Antonova et al., Reference Antonova, Aranda, Havari and Pace2015). However, countries in the same blocks are also similar in social aspects, cultural perceptions, beliefs and the derived norms of behaviour, all of which are associated with CD. For instance, compared to Northern European countries, in the Mediterranean countries older adults have higher expectations for close and warm family relations, including involvement in family matters and support in old age; probably because of this, they also experience higher levels of loneliness (Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Theou, Pena, Rockwood and Andrew2015). The difference in cultural perceptions of ‘good retirement’ in the various regions is another example which contributes to the understanding of our results regarding the difference between residents of Northern and Mediterranean nations in relation to leading an active lifestyle. While in Mediterranean nations retirement is generally viewed as a time of rest and relaxation, in the Northern countries it is viewed more as a period to enjoy leisure activities, physical exercise and volunteering (Börsch-Supan et al., Reference Börsch-Supan, Brugiavini, Jürges, Kapteyn, Mackenbach, Siegrist and Weber2008). Such unique features should be further explored in each of the four regions.

In sum, except for gender, each of our study variables had a significant unique contribution to the explanation of memory change after four years. However, although we have studied a relatively wide range of personal and nation-related factors, our multivariate regression models have explained 29–30 per cent of the variability in memory change over four years. This indicates that further multi-disciplinary research among ageing populations is needed in order to assess the additional combined genetic, psycho-social and socio-cultural features that directly or indirectly affect MD and CD, and the degree and rate of changes in them over time.

Future studies should also refine the measures of some of the important contributors to cognitive functioning. For instance, we assessed active lifestyle as the number of types of activities in which a person is involved. Considering that lifestyles can be changed through relevant interventions, it is important to deepen the investigation of active lifestyle by increasing the number of studied activities and assessing which of the activities – and what intervals of participation – are effective in reducing CD. The white- versus blue-collar jobs variable can also be improved by more specific evaluations of the various occupations included in each of these categories.

Limitations of the study

Our findings should be cautiously interpreted since we have focused only on a single dimension of cognition: working memory. This limitation prevents us from generalising our findings to a wider range of CD. Furthermore, considering that in our study some participants experienced improvements in memory after four years, and based on previous reports indicating that MD or even a diagnosis of MCI can be reversed (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Caracciolo, Brayne, Gauthier, Jelic and Fratiglioni2014), and despite our finding that the longer the period since retirement, the greater the MD, we cannot assuredly predict the degree of further deterioration of cognitive functioning among retirees, in general, or among early retirees, in particular. Hence, future analyses using similar longitudinal data should examine changes in MD and CD over longer periods of time by using more than two measurements. Considering the apparent bidirectional associations of changes among physical health, depression and cognitive functioning (e.g. Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017), and despite the longitudinal design of the study, we cannot disregard a potential reversed direction of causality from MD to decline in physical and mental health during the four years of the study. Generalisation of the results is also limited due to the attrition at T2 of retirees who were physically and cognitively weaker. In addition, measure of MD may be overly sensitive since change in memory from T1 to T2 is based on a change that can be as minimal as one word recalled/not recalled. However, even when we used a measure of two recalled/not recalled words instead of one, our results were similar in the hierarchical regression analyses. Furthermore, although we used a one-word scale and studied only a single facet of CD, the factors found to uniquely explain MD over time, while controlling for the wide range of other relevant factors (most of which are well established), indicate that such a minor change in recalled words may be a sign of further CD. We hence suggest considering the studied factors for prevention and treatment of CD.

Conclusion

Although there is still much more to learn about the phenomenon of CD over time in older adults, our longitudinal findings show that declines in physical and emotional health and functioning negatively correlate with MD, while education and active lifestyle, through re-employment or other physical/social activities, attenuate these associations. Our comparative analyses among blocks of countries indicate that the quality of a country's educational and medical systems is related to the cognitive functioning of its citizens. Moreover, our analyses show that early retirement plays a unique and important role in MD, and that this occurs in each of the studied European regions even when controlling for many relevant personal and behavioural factors. These findings lend support to our hypotheses and lead us to suggest postponing CD by focusing on what we know through repeated findings and are able to implement.

Considering that CD is a multifactorial phenomenon, nations can act on several levels, including promoting policies directed towards prolonging years of mandatory and free education and improving their health systems (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017). Through laws, pension and taxation policies, nations can increase the number of years of participation in the workforce (Rechel et al., Reference Rechel, Grundy, Robine, Cylus, Mackenbach, Knai and McKee2013). In addition to raising the age of mandatory retirement or cancelling it, nations can introduce policies and programmes designed to encourage people to postpone retirement, for instance, by adjusting work conditions to accommodate the inevitable physical and mental changes which occur at older ages. Changing cultural beliefs and behaviours is difficult, but if such changes are related to health, they can be achieved through public health education messages and primary/secondary prevention programmes. We also suggest promoting interventions which have been reported to be effective in improving cognitive functioning among people with MCI, including interventions based on cognitive exercise (e.g. Belleville et al., Reference Belleville, Gilbert, Fontaine, Gagnon, Ménard and Gauthier2006), physical exercise (e.g. Smith et al., Reference Smith, Nielson, Antuono, Lyons, Hanson, Butts, Hantke and Verber2013) or combined methods (e.g. Byers and Yaffe, Reference Byers and Yaffe2011; Ngandu et al., Reference Ngandu, Lehtisalo, Solomon, Levälahti, Ahtiluoto, Antikainen, Bäckman, Hänninen, Jula, Laatikainen and Lindström2015; Köbe et al., Reference Köbe, Witte, Schnelle, Lesemann, Fabian, Tesky, Pantel and Flöel2016; Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Sommerlad, Orgeta, Costafreda, Huntley, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Burns and Cohen-Mansfield2017). For example, maintaining an active lifestyle and physical health can be facilitated by making exercise facilities and training personnel available and accessible in public settings. Implementing such policies and interventions seems to be of special importance for Mediterranean and Eastern European nations.

Acknowledgements

This paper uses data from SHARE Waves 1, 2 and 4 (DOIs: 10.6103/SHARE.w1.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w2.710, 10.6103/SHARE.w4.710), see Börsch-Supan et al. (2013) for methodological details. The SHARE data collection has been funded by the European Commission through FP5 (QLK6-CT-2001-00360), FP6 (SHARE-I3: RII-CT-2006-062193, COMPARE: CIT5-CT-2005-028857, SHARELIFE: CIT4 CT-2006-028812), FP7 (SHARE-PREP: GA N°211909, SHARE-LEAP: GA N°227822, SHARE M4: GA N°261982, DASISH: GA N°283646) and Horizon 2020 (SHARE-DEV3: GA N°676536, SHARE-COHESION: GA N°870628, SERISS: GA N°654221, SSHOC: GA N°823782) and by DG Employment, Social Affairs & Inclusion. Additional funding from the German Ministry of Education and Research, the Max Planck Society for the Advancement of Science, the U.S. National Institute on Aging (U01_AG09740-13S2, P01_AG005842, P01_AG08291, P30_AG12815, R21_AG025169, Y1-AG-4553-01, IAG_BSR06-11, OGHA_04-064, HHSN271201300071C) and from various national funding sources is gratefully acknowledged (see www.share-project.org). This study was supported by the Center for Multidisciplinary Research in Aging of Ben-Gurion University of the Negev. The authors would like to thank Michelle Levenson and Sara Pollack for their assistance with article preparation.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Ethical standards

Ethical approval was not required for this study.

References

Adam, S, Bonsang, E, Germain, S and Perelman, S (2007) Retirement and cognitive reserve: a stochastic frontier approach applied to survey data. HEC-ULg, Liège, Belgium, CREPP Working Paper 2007/04.Google Scholar
Alzheimer's Association (2018) 2018 Alzheimer's disease facts and figures. Alzheimer's & Dementia 14, 367429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antonova, L, Aranda, L, Havari, E and Pace, N (2015) Is there a European land of opportunity? Cross-country differences in inter-generational mobility in 14 European countries and Israel. In A. Borsch-Supan, T. Kneip, H. Litwin, M. Myck, & G. Weber (eds.) Ageing in Europe – Supporting Policies for an Inclusive Society (pp. 209-222). Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co., Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banks, J and Mazzonna, F (2012) The effect of education on old age cognitive abilities: evidence from a regression discontinuity design. The Economic Journal 122, 418448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgart, M, Snyder, HM, Carrillo, MC, Fazio, S, Kim, H and Johns, H (2015) Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimer's & Dementia 11, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belleville, S, Gilbert, B, Fontaine, F, Gagnon, L, Ménard, É and Gauthier, S (2006) Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 22, 486499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchflower, DG and Oswald, AJ (2008) Hypertension and happiness across nations. Journal of Health Economics 27, 218233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonsang, E, Adam, S and Perelman, S (2012) Does retirement affect cognitive functioning? Journal of Health Economics 31, 490501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Börsch-Supan, A, Brugiavini, A, Jürges, H, Kapteyn, A, Mackenbach, J, Siegrist, J and Weber, G (eds) (2008) First Results from the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (2004–2007): Starting the Longitudinal Dimension. Mannheim, Germany: Mannheim Research Institute for the Economics of Aging.Google Scholar
Börsch-Supan, A, Brandt, M, Hunkler, C, Kneip, T, Korbmacher, J, Malter, F, Schaan, B, Stuck, S and Zuber, S (2013) Data resource profile: the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 9921001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers, AL and Yaffe, K (2011) Depression and risk of developing dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carmel, S (2001) The will to live: gender differences among elderly persons. Social Science & Medicine 52, 949958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmel, S, Granek, L and Zamir, A (2016) Influences of nationalism and historical traumatic events on the will to live of elderly Israelis. The Gerontologist 56, 753761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coe, NB, von Gaudecker, HM, Lindeboom, M and Maurer, J (2012) The effect of retirement on cognitive functioning. Health Economics 21, 913927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davydow, DS, Zivin, K, Katon, WJ, Pontone, GM, Chwastiak, L, Langa, KM and Iwashyna, TJ (2014) Neuropsychiatric disorders and potentially preventable hospitalizations in a prospective cohort study of older Americans. Journal of General Internal Medicine 29, 13621371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Ronchi, D, Palmer, K, Pioggiosi, P, Atti, AR, Berardi, D, Ferrari, B, Dalmonte, D and Fratiglioni, L (2007) The combined effect of age, education, and stroke on dementia and cognitive impairment no dementia in the elderly. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 24, 266273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E, Oishi, S and Lucas, RE (2003) Personality, culture, and subjective well-being: emotional and cognitive evaluations of life. Annual Review of Psychology 54, 403425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ding, D, Zhao, Q, Qihao Guo, Q, Meng, H, Wang, B, Luo, J, Mortimer, JA, Borenstein, AR and Hong, Z (2015) Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community in China: a cross-sectional analysis of the Shanghai Aging Study. Alzheimer's & Dementia 11, 300309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dotson, VM, Beydoun, MA and Zonderman, AB (2010) Recurrent depressive symptoms and the incidence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 75, 634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dufouil, C, Pereira, E, Chêne, G, Glymour, MM, Alpérovitch, A, Saubusse, E, Risse-Fleury, M, Heuls, B, Salord, JC, Brieu, MA and Forette, F (2014) Older age at retirement is associated with decreased risk of dementia. European Journal of Epidemiology 29, 353361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enache, D, Winblad, B and Aarsland, D (2011) Depression in dementia: epidemiology, mechanisms, and treatment. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 24, 461472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferri, CP, Prince, M, Brayne, C, Brodaty, H, Fratiglioni, L, Ganguli, M, Hall, K, Hasegawa, K, Hendrie, H, Huang, Y and Jorm, A (2005) Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. The Lancet 366, 21122117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, GG, Stachowsky, A, Infurna, FJ, Faul, JD, Grosch, J and Tetrick, LE (2014) Mental health demand, retirement, and longitudinal trajectories of cognitive functioning. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 19, 231242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fong, TG, Tulebaev, SR and Inouye, SK (2009) Delirium in elderly adults: diagnosis, prevention and treatment. Nature Reviews Neurology 5, 210-220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, LK and Gwyther, LP (1986) Caregiver well-being: a multidimensional examination of family caregivers of demented adults. The Gerontologist 26, 253259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebert, LE, Scherr, PA, McCann, JJ, Beckett, LA and Evans, DA (2001) Is the risk of developing Alzheimer's disease greater for women than for men? American Journal of Epidemiology 153, 132136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzog, C, Kramer, A, Wilson, R and Lindenberger, U (2008) Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9, 165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, DF, Hertzog, C, Small, BJ and Dixon, RA (1999) Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging. Psychology and Aging 14, 245-263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ihle, A, Oris, M, Fagot, D, Maggiori, C and Kliegel, M (2016) The association of educational attainment, cognitive level of job, and leisure activities during the course of adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: the role of openness experience. International Psychogeriatrics 28, 733740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ihle, A, Ghisletta, P, Ballhausen, N, Fagot, D, Vallet, F, Baeriswyl, M, Sauter, J, Oris, M, Maurer, J and Kliegel, M (2018) The role of cognitive reserve accumulated in midlife for the relation between chronic diseases and cognitive decline in old age: a longitudinal follow-up across six years. Neuropsychologia 121, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köbe, T, Witte, AV, Schnelle, A, Lesemann, A, Fabian, S, Tesky, VA, Pantel, J and Flöel, A (2016) Combined omega-3 fatty acids, aerobic exercise and cognitive stimulation prevents decline in gray matter volume of the frontal, parietal and cingulate cortex in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage 131, 226238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koepsell, TD and Monsell, SE (2012) Reversion from mild cognitive impairment to normal or near-normal cognition: risk factors and prognosis. Neurology 79, 15911598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lifshitz-Vahav, H, Shrira, A and Bodner, E (2017) The reciprocal relationship between participation in leisure activities and cognitive functioning: the moderating effect of self-rated literacy level. Aging & Mental Health 21, 524531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litwin, H and Tur-Sinai, A (2015) The role of the social network in early retirement among older Europeans. Work, Aging, and Retirement 1, 340349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingston, G, Sommerlad, A, Orgeta, V, Costafreda, SG, Huntley, J, Ames, D, Ballard, C, Banerjee, S, Burns, A and Cohen-Mansfield, J (2017) The Lancet International Commission on Dementia Prevention and Care. The Lancet 390, 26732734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mangin, D, Bahat, G, Golomb, BA, Mallery, LH, Moorhouse, P, Onder, G, Petrovic, M and Garfinkel, D (2018) International Group for Reducing Inappropriate Medication Use & Polypharmacy (IGRIMUP): position statement and 10 recommendations for action. Drugs & Aging 35, 575587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzonna, F and Peracchi, F (2012) Aging, cognitive abilities and retirement. European Economic Review 56, 691710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzonna, F and Peracchi, F (2017) Unhealthy retirement? Journal of Human Resources 52, 128-151.Google Scholar
Meng, X and D'Arcy, C (2012) Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLOS ONE 7, e38268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meng, A, Nexo, MA and Borg, V (2017) The impact of retirement on age related cognitive decline – a systematic review. BMC Geriatrics 17, 160, 1-10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ngandu, T, Lehtisalo, J, Solomon, A, Levälahti, E, Ahtiluoto, S, Antikainen, R, Bäckman, L, Hänninen, T, Jula, A, Laatikainen, T and Lindström, J (2015) A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet 385, 22552263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyberg, L, Lövdén, M, Riklund, K, Lindenberger, U and Bäckman, L (2012) Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16, 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paterniti, S, Verdier-Taillefer, MH, Dufouil, C and Alpérovitch, A (2002) Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in elderly people: longitudinal study. British Journal of Psychiatry 181, 406410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, RC, Caracciolo, B, Brayne, C, Gauthier, S, Jelic, V and Fratiglioni, L (2014) Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. Journal of Internal Medicine 275, 214228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, RC, Lopez, O, Armstrong, MJ, Getchius, TS, Ganguli, M, Gloss, D, Gronseth, GS, Marson, D, Pringsheim, T, Day, GS and Sager, M (2018) Practice guideline update summary: mild cognitive impairment. Report of the Guideline Development, Dissemination, and Implementation Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology 90, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prince, MJ, Reischies, F, Beekman, AT, Fuhrer, R, Jonker, C, Kivela, SL, Lawlor, BA, Lobo, A, Magnusson, H, Fichter, M and Van Oyen, H (1999) Development of the EURO-D scale – a European Union initiative to compare symptoms of depression in 14 European centers. British Journal of Psychiatry 174, 330338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rechel, B, Grundy, E, Robine, JM, Cylus, J, Mackenbach, JP, Knai, C and McKee, M (2013) Ageing in the European Union. The Lancet 381, 13121322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reisberg, B, Ferris, SH, De Leon, MJ and Crook, T (1982) The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. American Journal of Psychiatry 139, 11361139.Google ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R and Knopman, DS (2013) Classification and epidemiology of MCI. Clinics in Geriatric Medicine 29, 753772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohwedder, S and Willis, RJ (2010) Mental retirement. Journal of Economic Perspectives 24, 119138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, TA (1985) A Theory of Cognitive Ageing. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Salthouse, TA (2006) Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the ‘use it or lose it’ hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1, 6887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarmeas, N and Stern, Y (2003) Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 25, 625633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaie, KW (1989) The hazards of cognitive ageing. The Gerontologist 29, 484493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, JC, Nielson, KA, Antuono, P, Lyons, JA, Hanson, RJ, Butts, AM, Hantke, NC and Verber, MD (2013) Semantic memory functional MRI and cognitive function after exercise intervention in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 37, 197215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spaniol, J, Madden, DJ and Voss, A (2006) A diffusion model analysis of adult age differences in episodic and semantic long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 32, 101-117.Google ScholarPubMed
Steffens, DC and Potter, GG (2008) Geriatric depression and cognitive impairment. Psychological Medicine 38, 163175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steptoe, A and Wardle, J (2005) Positive affect and biological function in everyday life. Neurobiology of Aging 26, S108S112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y (2012) Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. The Lancet Neurology 11, 10061012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tovel, H, Carmel, S and Raveis, HR (2017) Relationships among self-perception of aging, physical functioning, and self-efficacy in late life. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 74B, 212221.Google Scholar
Tulving, E (1995) Organization of memory: quo vadis. In Gazzaniga, MS (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 839847.Google Scholar
Wallace, LM, Theou, O, Pena, F, Rockwood, K and Andrew, MK (2015) Social vulnerability as a predictor of mortality and disability: cross-country differences in the Survey of Health, Aging, and Retirement in Europe (SHARE). Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 27, 365372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, A, Arrighi, HM, Michels, S and Cedarbaum, JM (2012) Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimer's & Dementia 8, 1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winblad, B, Amouyel, P, Andrieu, S, Ballard, C, Brayne, C, Brodaty, H and Fratiglioni, L (2016) Defeating Alzheimer's disease and other dementias: a priority for European science and society. The Lancet Neurology 15, 455532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (WHO) (2017 a) Dementia. Geneva: WHO. Available at http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementia.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO) (2017 b) Mental Health and Older Adults. Geneva: WHO. Available at http://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-of-older-adults.Google Scholar
Zeng, Y, Feng, Q, Hesketh, T, Christensen, K and Vaupel, JW (2017) Survival, disabilities in activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning among the oldest-old in China: a cohort study. The Lancet 389, 16191629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zulka, LE, Hansson, I and Hassing, LB (2019) Impact of retirement on cognitive function. GeroPsych 32, 187203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Comparison between the study participants (interviewed at Time (T1) and Time (T2)) and those who dropped out of the study at T2

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics (at Time 1) of the sample by blocks of similar nations

Figure 2

Figure 1. Distribution of change in memory during the four years of the study among retirees.Source: Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) – Israel (2005/2006, 2009/2010), Europe (2006/2007, 2011).

Figure 3

Figure 2. Changes in study variables between Time 1 and Time 2. (a) Self-perceived health change; (b) depressive symptoms change; (c) activities of daily living change; (d) instrumental activities of daily living change.

Figure 4

Table 3. Changes in the study variables between Time 1 and Time 2 (T2)

Figure 5

Table 4. Ordinary least squares regression models explaining memory change in number of recalled words between Time 1 (T1) and Time 2 (T2)