Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

On Solid Ground: Evaluating the Effects of Foundational Arguments on Human Rights Attitudes

  • Published:
Human Rights Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What makes some human rights campaigns for the physical integrity rights of prisoners more effective than others? Despite various normative arguments condemning these practices, only limited systematic analysis documents the relative effectiveness of different arguments on individuals. This is surprising, because the success of human rights campaigns depends on getting individuals to care about and support policy positions that protect human rights. We constructed an experiment to compare the effects of six different arguments against prisoner abuse and torture. We found that an argument which emphasized the suffering of the prisoner had a consistently positive and significant effect on opposition to torture and prisoner abuse. However, this effect was largely contingent on subjects’ political ideology. Political conservatives actually became less opposed to torture, on average, after reading the same argument emphasizing the prisoners’ suffering or the sacredness of human beings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Graph 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. “Framing effects” happen “when (often small) changes in the presentation of an issue or an event produce (sometimes large) changes of opinions” (Chong and Druckman 2007, 104)

  2. Alternatively, others argue that foundational arguments should be set aside (Ignatieff 2001).

  3. Some suggest that different foundations can be useful for some individuals but not others. For example, Hollinger (2001) argues that religious individuals can and should rely on religious foundations and that secular people can and should rely on secular human rights foundations.

  4. Other human rights experimental designs utilize control groups that do not receive any experimental stimulus (McEntire et al. 2015).

  5. Recent intelligence shows that torture is an ineffective method of interrogation to uncover accurate intelligence (Costanzo and Gerrity 2009). Nonetheless, many Americans still believe that torture is justified and does result in credible and important information (Pew Research Center 2014).

  6. The full experimental treatments are on file with the authors.

  7. We thank one anonymous reviewer for mentioning the possible disconnection between a policy leader and a concept such as sacredness. We contend that it is common for political leaders and policy experts in the USA (and elsewhere) to invoke religious language when making arguments to the public. Relatedly, religious elites from religious organizations (who can also be policy experts) publicly disseminate messages that may have political implications. Examples include the US Constitution drawing from both Protestant ethics and enlightenment philosophy that liberation theology greatly influenced human rights causes in South Africa, and the Second Vatican Council’s contributions to the fall of communism (Ibrahim 2015).

  8. Restricted models without these controls are substantively consistent with the models presented in Table 2.

  9. Predicted probabilities for all models were calculated by holding dichotomous variables (ideology, gender) at their modes and holding ordered categorical variables (age, religiosity) at their median values.

  10. Predicted values for each experimental group were estimated from Appendix Table 4, Model 2A.

References

  • Alford CF (2010) Narrative, nature, and the natural law: From Aquinas to international human rights. Palgrave Macmillan, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Amnesty International. 2014. Stop torture global survey: Attitudes to torture. http://www.amnestyusa.org/pdfs/GlobalSurveyAttitudesToTorture.2014.pdf. Accessed 1 January 2015

  • Appiah KA (2001) Grounding human rights. In: Ignatieff Human rights as politics and idolatry. Princeton University Press, Princeton, pp 101–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Beitz C (2011) The idea of human rights. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Num Bloom P, Arikan G (2012) Religion and support for democracy: A cross-national test of the mediating mechanisms Br J Political Sci 43: 375–397

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS (2012) Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s mechanical Turk. Political Anal 20: 351–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bosch OJ, Revilla M, DeCastellarnau A, Weber W (2018) Measurement reliability, validity, and quality of slider versus radio button scales in an online probability-based panel in Norway. Soc Sci Comput Rev. https://doi.org/10.1177/0894439317750089

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braun J, Arves S (2017) Tailoring the message: How the political left and right think differently about human rights. OpenGlobalRights: https://www.openglobalrights.org/Tailorin-%20the-message-How-the-political-left-and-right-think-differently-about-human-rights/. Accessed 2 January 2019

  • Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD (2011) Amazon’s mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci 6: 3–5

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chenoweth E, Stephan MJ (2011) Why civil resistance works. Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Chilton AS (2014) The influence of international human rights agreements on public opinion: An experimental study. Chicago J Int Law 15: 110–137

    Google Scholar 

  • Chong D, Druckman JN (2007) Framing theory. Annu Rev Political Sci 10: 103–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chyung SY, Swanson I, Roberts K, Hankinson A (2018) Evidence-based survey design: the use of continuous rating scales in surveys. Performance Improv. 57: 38–48

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifford S, Jewell RM, Waggoner PD (2015) Are samples drawn from mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology. Res Politics 1: 1–9

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J (1992) A power primer. Psychol Bull 112: 155–159

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad CR, Moore WH (2010) What stops torture? Am J Political Sci 54: 459–476

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costanzo MA, Gerrity E (2009) The effects and effectiveness of using torture as and interrogation device: Using research to inform the policy debate. Soc Issues Policy Rev 3: 179–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davenport C (2007) State repression and the domestic democratic peace. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Demeritt JHR (2012) International organizations and government killing: Does naming and shaming save lives? Int Interact: Empir Theoretical Res Int Relations 38: 597–621

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaz-Verizades J, Widamen KF, Little TD, Gibbs KW (1995) The measurement and structure of human rights attitudes. J Soc Psychol 135: 313–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donnelly J (1989) Universal human rights in theory and practice. Cornell University Press Ithaca

    Google Scholar 

  • Donnelly J (2013) International human rights. Westview Press, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Druckman JN (2001) On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? J Politics 63:1041–1066

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fariss CJ (2014) Respect for human rights has improved over time: Modeling the changing standard of accountability. Am Political Sci Rev 108: 297–318

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman S (2003). Values, ideology, and the structure of political attitudes. In. Sears DO, Huddy L, Jervis R (eds.) Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 477–508

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman S, and Johnston C (2014). Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity. Political Psychol 35: 337–358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forsythe DP (1991) The internationalization of human rights. Lexington Books, Lexington

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsythe DP (2011) The politics of prisoner abuse: The United States and enemy prisoners after 9/11. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Getz I (1985) Moral reasoning, religion, and attitudes towards human rights. Dissertation, University of Minnesota

  • Goodhart M (2014) Recent works on dignity and human rights: A road not taken. Perspect Politics 4: 846–856

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham J, Haidt J, Nosek BA (2009) Personality processes and individual differences: Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. J Pers Soc Psychol 96 (5): 1029–1046

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gronke P, Rejali D, Drenguis D, Hicks J, Miller P, Nakayama B (2010) U.S. Public Opinion on Torture, 2001–2009 2010. PS: Political Sci Politics 43:437–444

  • Hafner-Burton EM (2008) “Sticks and stones: naming and shaming the human rights enforcement problem. Int Organ 62: 689–716

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hafner-Burton EM, Tsutsui K (2005) Human rights in a globalizing world: The paradox of empty promises. Am J Soc 110(5): 1373–1411

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hibbing JR, Smith KB, Alford JR (2013) Predisposed: Liberals, conservatives, and the biology of political differences. Routledge, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hibbing JR, Smith KB, Alford JR (2014) Differences in negativity bias underlie variation in political ideology. Behav Brain Sci 37:297–307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hillebrecht C (2012) Implementing international human rights law at home: Domestic politics and the European Court of Human Rights. Hum Rights Rev 13: 279–301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollinger DA (2001) Debates with the PTA and Others In: Ignatieff Human rights as politics and idolatry. Princeton University Press, Princeton, 117–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Huff C, Tingley D (2015) Who are these people? Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents. Res Politics 2: 1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt L (2007) Inventing human rights: A history. W.W. Norton & Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ibrahim, AM (2015). Religion inspires in ways that international law does not. OpenGlobalRights. https://www.openglobalrights.org/religion-inspires-in-ways-that-international-law-does-not/. Accessed 1 October 2018

  • Ignatieff M (2001) Introduction In: Ignatieff Human rights as politics and idolatry. Princeton University Press, Princeton, pp: vii-xxvii

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivkovic M (2016) In southeastern Europe, data helps bolster LGBTI rights. OpenDemocracy. https://opendemocracy.net/ openglobalrights/marko-ivkovic/in-southeastern-europe-data- helps-bolster-lgbtirights. Accessed 1 January 2018

  • Janta-Lipinski L (2015) Know thy audience: Effective messaging in human rights campaigns. OpenDemocracy.https://www.opendemocracy.net/openglobalrights/laurence-janta-lipinski /knowthy-audience-effective-messaging-in-human-rights-campa. Accessed 1 January 2018

  • Jost JT, Krochik M (2014) Ideological differences in epistemic motivation: Implications for attitude structure, depth of information processing, susceptibility to persuasion, and stereotyping. In: Elliot A (ed) Advances in motivation science, 1st edn. Amsterdam, pp 181–257

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost JT, Nosek BA, Gosling SD (2008) Ideology: Its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci 3: 126–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karatnycky A, Ackerman P (2005) How freedom is won: From civic resistance to durable democracy. Freedom House

  • Kateb G (2011) Human dignity. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendall-Taylor N (2016) To Advance More Humane Refugee Policies, we must reframe the debate. OpenDemocracy. https://www.opendemocracy.net/openglobalrights/nat-kendall-taylor/to-advancemore-humane-refugee-policies-we-must-reframe-debate. Accessed 1 January 2018

  • Kohen A (2007) In defense of human rights: A non-religious grounding in a pluralistic world. Routledge, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Koo JW, Cheong BE, Ramirez FO (2015) Who thinks and behaves according to human rights?: Evidence from the Korean national human rights survey” Korea Observer 46: 53–54.

  • Kreps S (2014) Flying under the radar: A study of public attitudes towards unmanned Aerial Vehicles Res Politics 1:1–7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krys R (2015) Research-based messaging changes public support for human rights. openDemocracy. Accessed August 1, 2016. https://www.opendemocracy.net/ openglobalrights/rachel-krys/researchbased-messaging-changes-public-support -for-human-rights. Accessed 1 January 2018

  • Küng H (1991) Global responsibility: In search of a new World ethic. Crossroad Publishing Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu M, Conrad F (2016) An experiment testing six formats of 101-point rating scales. Comp Human Behav. 55: 364–371

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCright AM., Dunlap RE (2010) Anti-reflexivity the American conservative movement’s success in undermining climate science and policy. Theory Cult Soc 27: 100–133

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McEntire KJ, Leiby M, Krain M (2015) Human rights organizations as agents of change: An experimental examination of framing and micromobilization. Am Political Sci Rev 109: 407–426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFarland S, Mathews M (2005) Who cares about human rights? Political Psychol 26: 365–385

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller P, Gronke P, Rejali D (2014) Torture and public opinion: The partisan dimension. In: Lightcap T, Phiffner J (eds) Examining torture: Empirical studies of state repression. Palgrave Macmillan, New York, pp 11–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullinix K, Leeper T, Druckman J, Freese J (2015) The generalizability of survey experiments. J Experimental Political Sci 2: 109–138

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murdie A, Peksen D (2014) The impact of human rights INGO shaming on humanitarian interventions. J Politics 76: 215–228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis PG (2010) Running experiments on amazon mechanical Turk. Judgm Decis Mak 5: 411–419

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelosi N (2018) “Pelosi Statement on Death of Felipe Alonzo-Gomez at the Border on Christmas”, see: https://www.democraticleader.gov/newsroom/122618/. Accessed 1 January 2019

  • Perry M (1998) The idea of human rights: Four inquires. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center. 2014. About half see CIA interrogation methods as justified

  • Poe SC, Tate CN, Keith LC (1999) Repression of the human right to personal integrity revisited: A global cross-national study covering the years 1976-1993 Int Stud Q 43: 291–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pruce JR (2015) The practice turn in human rights research In: Joel Pruce (ed) The social practice of human rights. Palgrave Macmillan, New York, pp 1–17

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rorty R (1989) Contingency, irony, and solidarity. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rosen M (2012) Dignity: Its history and meaning. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Slaughter JR (2007) Human rights, inc.: The world novel, narrative form, and international law. Fordham University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace GPR (2013) International law and public attitudes toward torture: An experimental study. Int Organ 67: 105–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolterstorff N (2008) Justice: Rights and wrongs. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • World Values Survey Association 2018. “World Values Survey 1981–2014 Longitudinal Aggregate v.20150418.” Aggregate File Produce: JDSystems, Madrid SPAIN. www.worldvaluessurvey.org. Accessed 1 January 2018

Download references

Acknowledgments

Both authors contributed equally to this project. The authors are grateful for helpful feedback received at a University of Maryland Comparative Politics Workshop (2015), the International Studies Association Annual Convention (2015), and The Social Practice of Human Rights Conference (2015). The authors thank the Political Theory Subfield at the University of Maryland for a research grant to carry out the experiment. Experiment approved under University of Maryland IRB no. 560302-7.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen Arves.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Table 3 Feeling thermometer squared
Table 4 Interaction of treatments with ideology on scale variable

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Arves, S., Braun, J. On Solid Ground: Evaluating the Effects of Foundational Arguments on Human Rights Attitudes. Hum Rights Rev 20, 181–204 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12142-019-0545-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12142-019-0545-7

Keywords

Navigation