Skip to main content
Log in

Production of Estonian case-inflected nouns shows whole-word frequency and paradigmatic effects

  • Published:
Morphology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most psycholinguistic models of lexical processing assume that the comprehension and production of inflected forms is mediated by morphemic constituents. Several more recent studies, however, have challenged this assumption by providing empirical evidence that information about individual inflected forms and their paradigmatic relations is available in long-term memory (Baayen et al. 1997; Milin et al. 2009a, 2009b). Here, we investigate how whole-word frequency, inflectional paradigm size and morphological family size affect production latencies and articulation durations when subjects are asked to read aloud isolated Estonian case-inflected nouns. In Experiment 1, we observed that words with a larger morphological family elicited shorter speech onset latencies, and that forms with higher whole-word frequency had shorter acoustic durations. Experiment 2, for which we increased statistical power by using 2,800 words, revealed that higher whole-word frequency, inflectional paradigm size, and morphological family size reduced both speech onset times and acoustic durations. These results extend our knowledge of morphological processing in three ways. First, whole-word frequency effects of inflected forms in morphologically rich languages are not restricted to a small number of very high-frequency forms, contrary to previous claims (Niemi et al. 1994; Hankamer 1989; Yang 2016). Second, we replicated the morphological family size effect in a new domain, the acoustic durations of inflected forms. Third, we showed that a novel paradigmatic measure, inflectional paradigm size, predicts word naming latencies and acoustic durations. These results fit well with Word-and-Paradigm morphology (Blevins 2016) and argue against strictly (de)compositional models of lexical processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. http://www.cl.ut.ee/korpused/grammatikakorpus/ (15.04.2017).

  2. jalgadel and jalul both express adessive plurals, but jalgadel has more external locational meaning (something is on the feet) and jalul more a idiosyncratic meaning as ‘back on the feet’.

  3. Hence, some inflected forms were counted neither as a part of inflectional paradigm nor orthographic neighbourhood count.

References

  • Alegre, M., & Gordon, P. (1999). Frequency effects and the representational status of regular inflections. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 41–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, D., Tomaschek, F., Lopez, F., Sering, T., & Baayen, R. H. (2017). Words from spontaneous conversational speech can be recognized with human-like accuracy by an error-driven learning algorithm that discriminates between meanings straight from smart acoustic features, bypassing the phoneme as recognition unit. PLoS ONE, 12(4), e0174,623. http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0174623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnon, I., & Cohen Priva, U. (2013). More than words: the effect of multi-word frequency and constituency on phonetic duration. Language and Speech, 56(3), 349–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnon, I., & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language, 62(1), 67–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychology Research, 3, 12–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R. (1997). Singulars and plurals in Dutch: evidence for a parallel dual route model. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 94–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., McQueen, J., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R. (2003). Frequency effects in regular inflectional morphology: revisiting Dutch plurals. In R. H. Baayen & R. Schreuder (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (pp. 355–390). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Wurm, L. H., & Aycock, J. (2007). Lexical dynamics for low-frequency complex words: a regression study across tasks and modalities. The Mental Lexicon, 2, 419–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Levelt, W., Schreuder, R., & Ernestus, M. (2008). Paradigmatic structure in speech production. Proceedings of Chicago Linguistic Society, 43(1), 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Milin, P., Filipović-Ðurđević, D., Hendrix, P., & Marelli, M. (2011). An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review, 118(3), 438–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Sering, T., Shaoul, C., & Milin, P. (2017a). Language comprehension as a multiple label classification problem. In Proceedings of the 32nd international workshop on statistical modelling (IWSM). The Netherlands: Johann Bernoulli Institute, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen. 3–7 July, 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Vasishth, S., Bates, D., & Kliegl, R. (2017b). The cave of shadows: addressing the human factor with generalized additive mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 206–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balling, L., & Baayen, R. H. (2008). Morphological effects in auditory word recognition: evidence from Danish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23, 1159–1190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balling, L. W., & Baayen, R. H. (2012). Probability and surprisal in auditory comprehension of morphologically complex words. Cognition, 125(1), 80–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D., Cortese, M., Sergent-Marshall, S., Spieler, D., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition for single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133, 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bannard, C., & Matthews, D. (2008). Stored word sequences in language learning: the effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science, 19, 241–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bien, H., Levelt, W., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Frequency effects in compound production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 17,876–17,881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bien, H., Baayen, R. H., & Levelt, W. J. (2011). Frequency effects in the production of Dutch deverbal adjectives and inflected verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(4–6), 683–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P. (2003). Stems and paradigms. Language, 79, 737–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P. (2013). Word-based morphology from Aristotle to modern WP. In K. Allan (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of the History of Linguistics (pp. 375–395). Oxford: Oxfort University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P. (2016). Word and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Blevins, J. P., Milin, P., & Ramscar, M. (2017). The Zipfian paradigm cell filling problem. In J. Kiefer, J. P. Blevins, & H. Bartos (Eds.), Perspectives on morphological organization: Data and analyses. Leiden: Brill. Chap. 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, N. K., Caselli, M. K., & Cohen-Goldberg, A. M. (2016). Inflected words in production: evidence for a morphologically rich lexicon. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(3), 432–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cotterell, R., Kirov, C., Sylak-Glassman, J., Walther, G., Vylomova, E., Xia, P., Faruqui, M., Kübler, S., Yarowsky, D., Eisner, J., & Mans, H. (2017). CoNLL-SIGMORPHON 2017 shared task: universal morphological reinflection in 52 languages. Preprint, arXiv:170609031.

  • De Jong, N. H. (2002). Morphological families in the mental lexicon. MPI series in psycholinguistics. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, N. H., Feldman, L. B., Schreuder, R., Pastizzo, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2002). The processing and representation of Dutch and English compounds: peripheral morphological, and central orthographic effects. Brain and Language, 81, 555–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fasiolo, M., Goude, Y., Nedellec, R., & Wood, S. N. (2016). Fast calibrated additive quantile regression. R package version 1.0.

  • Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd edn.). Thousand Oaks: Sage. http://socserv.socsci.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fruchter, J., & Marantz, A. (2015). Decomposition, lookup, and recombination: meg evidence for the full decomposition model of complex visual word recognition. Brain and Language, 143, 81–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halle, M., & Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In K. Hale & S. J. Keyser (Eds.), Current studies in linguistics: Vol. 24. The view from building 20: essays in linguistics in honor of Sylvain Bromberger (pp. 111–176). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanique, I., & Ernestus, M. (2012). The role of morphology in acoustic reduction. Lingue E Linguaggio, 11(2), 147–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hankamer, J. (1989). Morphological parsing and the lexicon. In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representation and process (pp. 392–408). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (1990). Generalized additive models. New York: Wiley Online Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. B. (2001). Lexical frequency in morphology: is everything relative? Linguistics, 39, 1041–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendrix, P. (2015). Experimental explorations of a discrimination learning approach to language processing. PhD thesis, University of Tübingen.

  • Hothorn, T., Buehlmann, P., Dudoit, S., Molinaro, A., & Van Der Laan, M. (2006). Survival ensembles. Biostatistics, 7, 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A. (2012). Phrase frequency effects in language production. PLoS ONE, 7(3), 202, e3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janssen, N., Bi, Y., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A tale of two frequencies: determining the speed of lexical access for Mandarin Chinese and English compounds. Language and Cognitive Processes, 23(7–8), 1191–1223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaalep, H. J. (1997). An Estonian morphological analyser and the impact of a corpus on its development. Computers and the Humanities, 31(2), 115–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karlsson, F. (1986). Frequency considerations in morphology. STUF – Language Typology and Universals, 39(1–4), 19–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karlsson, F., & Koskenniemi, K. (1985). A process model of morphology and lexicon. Folia Linguistica, 19(1–2), 207–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keuleers, E. (2013). vwr: useful functions for visual word recognition research. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vwr, R package version 0.3.0.

  • Keuleers, E., Diependaele, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Practice effects in large-scale visual word recognition studies: a lexical decision study on 14,000 Dutch mono- and disyllabic words and nonwords. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). The British Lexicon Project: lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44(1), 287–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koskenniemi, K. (1984). A general computational model for word-form recognition and production. In Proceedings of the 10th international conference on computational linguistics, association for computational linguistics (pp. 178–181).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Pluymaekers, M., Ernestus, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2006). Morphological predictability and acoustic salience of interfixes in Dutch compounds. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 122, 2018–2024.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Schreuder, R., Bertram, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2009). Reading of multimorphemic Dutch compounds: towards a multiple route model of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 35, 876–895.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lõo, K., Järvikivi, J., & Baayen, R. H. (2017, submitted for publication). Whole-word frequency and inflectional paradigm size facilitate Estonian case-inflected noun processing. Manuscript.

  • Laine, M., Niemi, J., Koivuselkä-Sallinen, P., & Hyönä, J. (1995). Morphological processing of polymorphemic nouns in a highly inflecting language. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12(5), 457–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laine, M., Vainio, S., & Hyönä, J. (1999). Lexical access routes to nouns in a morphologically rich language. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(1), 109–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M. (2003). How word frequency affects morphological processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(03), 213–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, X., & Zhang, D. (1999). Inference in generalized additive mixed models by using smoothing splines. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, Statistical Methodology, 61(2), 381–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marantz, A. (2013). No escape from morphemes in morphological processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(7), 905–916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, G. F., Brinkman, U., Clahsen, H., Wiese, R., & Pinker, S. (1995). German inflection: the exception that proves the rule. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 189–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, P. H. (1974). Morphology: an introduction to the theory of word structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milin, P., Filipović Durdević, D., & Moscoso del Prado Martín, F. (2009a). The simultaneous effects of inflectional paradigms and classes on lexical recognition: evidence from Serbian. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 50–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milin, P., Kuperman, V., Kostić, A., & Baayen, R. H. (2009b). Paradigms bit by bit: an information-theoretic approach to the processing of paradigmatic structure in inflection and derivation. In J. P. Blevins & J. Blevins (Eds.), Analogy in grammar: form and acquisition (pp. 214–252). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Moscoso del Prado Martín, F., Bertram, R., Häikiö, T., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2004). Morphological family size in a morphologically rich language: the case of Finnish compared to Dutch and Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 1271–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moscoso del Prado Martín, F., Kostić, A., & Baayen, R. H. (2004). Putting the bits together: an information theoretical perspective on morphological processing. Cognition, 94, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moscoso del Prado Martín, F., Deutsch, A., Frost, R., Schreuder, R., De Jong, N. H., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Changing places: a cross-language perspective on frequency and family size in Hebrew and Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 496–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulder, K., Dijkstra, T., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). Effects of primary and secondary morphological family size in monolingual and bilingual word processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 59–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niemi, J., Laine, M., & Tuominen, J. (1994). Cognitive morphology in Finnish: foundations of a new model. Language and Cognitive Processes, 9, 423–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pham, H. (2014). Visual processing of Vietnamese compound words: a multivariate analysis of using corpus linguistic and psycholinguistic paradigms. PhD thesis, University of Alberta, Canada.

  • Pham, H., & Baayen, R. H. (2015). Vietnamese compounds show an anti-frequency effect in visual lexical decision. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30(9), 1077–1095.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules: the ingredients of language. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I., Homann, J., & Kunter, G. (2017). Homophony and morphology: The acoustics of word-final S in English. Journal of Linguistics, 53(1), 181–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., Davis, M. H., & New, B. (2004). The broth in my brother’s brothel: morpho-orthographic segmentation in visual word recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 1090–1098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1996). Morpheme frequency in speech production: testing WEAVER. In G. E. Booij, & J. Van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1996 (pp. 135–154). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidtke, D., Matsuki, K., & Kuperman, V. (2017). Surviving blind decomposition: a distributional analysis of the time-course of complex word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition.

  • Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (1997). How complex simplex words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 118–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaoul, C., Westbury, C. F., & Baayen, R. H. (2013). The subjective frequency of word n-grams. Psihologija, 46(4), 497–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaoul, C., Baayen, R. H., & Westbury, C. F. (2014). N-gram probability effects in a cloze task. The Mental Lexicon, 9(3), 437–472.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soveri, A., Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M. (2007). Word frequency and morphological processing in Finnish revisited. The Mental Lexicon, 2(3), 359–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger, S., & van Rijn, H. (2013). It’s time to do the math: computation and retrieval in phrase production. The Mental Lexicon, 8(1), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stump, G. (2001). Inflectional morphology: a theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, C. C. (2016). Lexical processing in simplified Chinese: An investigation using a new large-scale lexical database. PhD thesis, Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen.

  • Tabak, W., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2005). Lexical statistics and lexical processing: semantic density, information complexity, sex, and irregularity in Dutch. In S. Kepser & M. Reis (Eds.), Linguistic Evidence—Empirical, Theoretical, and Computational Perspectives (pp. 529–555). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tabak, W., Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. H. (2010). Producing inflected verbs: a picture naming study. The Mental Lexicon, 5(1), 22–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M. (2004). Morphological decomposition and the reverse base frequency effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 745–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., & Forster, K. I. (1976). Lexical storage and retrieval of polymorphemic and polysyllabic words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 607–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomaschek, F., & Baayen, R. H. (2017, in preparation). The consequences of lexical proficiency for articulation. Manuscript.

  • Tomaschek, F., Wieling, M., Arnold, D., & Baayen, R. H. (2013). Word frequency, vowel length and vowel quality in speech production: an EMA study of the importance of experience. In INTERSPEECH (pp. 1302–1306).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomaschek, F., Tucker, B. V., Wieling, M., & Baayen, R. H. (2014). Vowel articulation affected by word frequency. In Proceedings of 10th ISSP, Cologne (pp. 429–432).

    Google Scholar 

  • Traficante, D., & Burani, C. (2003). Visual processing of Italian verbs and adjectives: the role of the inflectional family size. In R. H. Baayen & R. Schreuder (Eds.), Morphological structure in language processing (pp. 45–64). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, A., & Tucker, B. V. (2011). The effects of n-gram probabilistic measures on the recognition and production of four-word sequences. The Mental Lexicon, 6(2), 302–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, A., Derwing, B., Libben, G., & Westbury, C. (2011). Processing advantages of lexical bundles: evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks. Language Learning, 61(2), 569–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vare, S. (2012). Eesti keele sõnapered. Tallinn: Eesti Keele Sihtasutus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N. (2006). Generalized additive models. New York: Chapman & Hall/CRC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N. (2011). Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, Statistical Methodology, 73(1), 3–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, S. N., Goude, Y., & Shaw, S. (2015). Generalized additive models for large data sets. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series C. Applied Statistics, 64(1), 139–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, C. (2010). Three factors in language variation. Lingua, 120(5), 1160–1177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, C. (2016). The price of linguistic productivity. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Pärtel Lippus and Einar Meister for their assistance with the data collection as well as the editor-in-chief Olivier Bonami and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful reviews on the earlier version of this paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kaidi Lõo.

Additional information

This research was supported by the national scholarship program Kristjan Jaak, funded and managed by Archimedes Foundation in collaboration with the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research and by an Alexander von Humboldt research award (grant number 1141527) to the first and last author, respectively.

Appendix A

Appendix A

Table 7 Pairwise correlations between the key predictors in Experiment 1
Table 8 Pairwise correlations between the key predictors in Experiment 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lõo, K., Järvikivi, J., Tomaschek, F. et al. Production of Estonian case-inflected nouns shows whole-word frequency and paradigmatic effects. Morphology 28, 71–97 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-017-9318-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-017-9318-7

Keywords

Navigation