An inverted U-shaped relationship between cortisol awakening response and same-day error monitoring function in healthy males

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108052Get rights and content

Highlights

  • CAR is thought to provide an energetic “boost” for the coming day.

  • No association was observed between CAR and post-error behavior.

  • A significant quadratic relationship was observed between CAR and same-day ERN amplitude.

  • Moderate CAR was associated with best error monitoring function in healthy males.

  • Cortisol awakening response could be a biological predictor of cognitive function in the same day.

Abstract

The cortisol awakening response (CAR) is thought to provide an energetic “boost” for the coming day and has been shown to be associated with prefrontal dependent function. The aim of the current study was to examine the relationship between the CAR and same-day neural activity following an error response task. Forty-six healthy males (22.25 years ± 1.98) collected saliva samples at 0, 30 and 45 min post-awakening to measure the CAR, and on the same afternoon event-related potential activity during error processing was measured. Although no association was observed between CAR and post-error behavior, a significant quadratic relationship was observed between CAR and Error-related Negativity (ERN) amplitude, and this association remained while controlling for confounding factors. This finding suggested the existence of an inverted U-shaped relationship between CAR and the same-day error-monitoring function in healthy males.

Introduction

Given the established link between cortisol and cognition (de Kloet, Oitzl, & Joëls, 1999; Domes, Rothfischer, Reichwald, & Hautzinger, 2005; Joels, 2006; Lupien et al., 2005; Lupien, Juster, Raymond, & Marin, 2018; Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar, & Heim, 2009), efforts have focused on the cortisol awakening response (CAR), a rapid rise in cortisol levels that peaks between 30- and 45-min following morning awakening (Pruessner et al., 1997). Historically, studies treated the CAR as a trait measure for HPA-axis status (Wust et al., 2000). Its associations with both cognition and affective disorder such as depression have been reported (Almela, van der Meij, Hidalgo, Villada, & Salvador, 2012; Kofman et al., 2019; Kuehner, Holzhauer, & Huffziger, 2007; Quevedo, Doty, Roos, & Anker, 2017), though there is some inconsistency in the direction of relationship. For example, some research show greater CAR is associated with poorer cognition (Butler, Klaus, Edwards, & Pennington, 2017; Franz et al., 2011), but more recent research with accurate monitoring of sampling in the CAR period show greater CAR is associated with better cognitive functioning (Baumler et al., 2014; Evans, Hucklebridge, Loveday, & Clow, 2012).

At the same time, further studies argued that the CAR was prone to significant state variation which is greater than trait variation (Stalder, Evans, Hucklebridge, & Clow, 2010; Stalder, Hucklebridge, Evans, & Clow, 2009). For example, Hellhammer et al. (2007) reported that the CAR of a single day was determined to a great extent by state factors (61 %–82 %). It is proposed that state-related increases in the magnitude of the CAR may play a homeostatic role in ‘boosting’ cognitive performance upon awakening (Adam, Hawkley, Kudielka, & Cacioppo, 2006; Clow, Hucklebridge, Stalder, Evans, & Thorn, 2010). Recent studies found that CAR could predict same-day brain function, especially the prefrontal cortex (PFC) mediated executive function (Law, Hucklebridge, Thorn, Evans, & Clow, 2013). Law, Evans, Thorn, Hucklebridge, and Clow (2015) measured CAR and cognitive function over 50 days in one healthy male and found that on days in which CAR was higher executive function (measured by attention-switching task) in the morning of the same day was better. In an intensive 4-day protocol of 9 males, CAR and brain plasticity later on in the day was measured one week apart, on days in which CAR was greater this was associated with better same-day neuroplasticity measured in motor cortex (Clow et al., 2014). In addition, research from our laboratory also supported a similar positive relationship between the CAR and same-day brain function in prefrontal cortex; higher CAR was predictive of better function of response inhibition (Shi et al., 2018) and stronger positive medial prefrontal cortex connectivity (Wu et al., 2015). However, in another study, Moriarty et al. (2014) showed a more complex U-shaped relationship between CAR magnitude and same-day spatial working memory performance.

Error processing, another critical prefrontal cortex-related high-level ability, plays an important role in adapting to the environment (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004; Notebaert et al., 2009). Electrophysiological studies indicate that error processing includes different stages and two electrophysiological indices are of interest: the Error-related Negativity (ERN) and the Error Positivity (Pe) (Bediou, Koban, Rosset, Pourtois, & Sander, 2012; Luijten et al., 2014). The ERN is a negativity elicited approximately 50 ms after erroneous responses and it is thought to reflect the automatic error monitoring mechanism of the individual’s responses to his/her own errors (Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, 2018; Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, 1993). The Pe, a positivity following ERN, possibly reflects the conscious awareness of errors or the motivational significance attributed to the error (Helenius, Laasonen, Hokkanen, Paetau, & Niemivirta, 2010; Lievaart et al., 2016).

Prior studies have addressed the relationship between cortisol and error processing. For example, less cortisol change was correlated with the larger ERN amplitude, indicating that participants with larger ERNs were associated with successful stress regulation (Compton, Hofheimer, & Kazinka, 2013). On the contrary, Tops, Boksem, Wester, Lorist, and Meijman (2006) reported that ERN amplitudes were positively related with cortisol mobilization during a task session. With regards to the relationship between CAR and error processing, to our knowledge, there is only one study that showed a higher CAR was associated with slowed latency of the ERN (Zhang et al., 2015). However, one limitation of this work is that CAR was collected on two consecutive days after the ERP measurement. Since the CAR is characterized by substantial state variation, it is unclear whether CAR is associated with error processing of the upcoming day (i.e. measurement of CAR prior to the measurement of error processing on the same day).

In the present study, we set out to further elucidate whether the CAR was associated with same-day error processing using event-related potentials. To avoid possible confound eff ;ects of sex we examined an all-male, healthy young population (Almeida, Piazza, & Stawski, 2009). We hypothesized that the greater CAR was associated with better error processing (indicated by ERN/Pe). However, we did not exclude quadratic relationship because results from early studies investigating the relationship of the CAR and PFC function have been mixed.

Section snippets

Participants

The sample and the experimental procedure in this study are the same as in the paper of Shi et al. (2018) which addressed the relationship between CAR and response inhibition. We demonstrated that the CAR was predictive of the function of response inhibition in both the earlier cognitive step (i.e. conflict monitoring) and the behavioural performance of response inhibition on the same day (Shi et al., 2018). A total of 49 right-handed, healthy male university students were recruited from the

The preliminary analysis

Raw values for cortisol concentration on the testing day (i.e. day 2) ranged from 6.18 to 38.30 nmol/L. As expected, on average the peak in cortisol was found at 30-min post-awakening, followed by a decline until 45-min post awakening. Of the total 46 samples, 69.57 % of the participants showed a positive CAR (i.e. CAR > 0) (n = 32) and 30.43 % of the participants showed a negative CAR (n = 14). A two-tailed independent-samples t-test shows no significant differences in age, educational level,

Discussion

The current study examined the relationship between CAR and same-day error processing. Results showed that the CAR was not associated with the post-error behavioral adjustment. Although the hypothesis was not fully supported, we found an inverted U-shape relationship between CAR on the testing day and ERN amplitude in No/NoGo task, whereas no significant association was observed between the CAR on another day/trait CAR and the ERN amplitude. The quadratic effect explained 26.1 % of the variance

Contributors

Author S.X. collected, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. Author W.J.H. designed the study and revised the manuscript. Author S. N. participated in the interpretation of the data and the revision of the manuscript. All the authors approved the final manuscript.

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31900787, 31771246, 31530031) and Guangdong Innovative and Entrepreneurial Research Team Program.

References (71)

  • P. Evans et al.

    The cortisol awakening response is related to executive function in older age

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2012)
  • C.E. Franz et al.

    Cross-sectional and 35-year longitudinal assessment of salivary cortisol and cognitive functioning: The Vietnam Era twin study of aging

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2011)
  • E. Fries et al.

    The cortisol awakening response (CAR): Facts and future directions

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2009)
  • P. Helenius et al.

    Neural correlates of late positivities associated with infrequent visual events and response errors

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • J. Hellhammer et al.

    Several daily measurements are necessary to reliably assess the cortisol rise after awakening: State- and trait components

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2007)
  • V. Hidalgo et al.

    Memory performance is related to the cortisol awakening response in older people, but not to the diurnal cortisol slope

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2016)
  • M. Joels

    Corticosteroid effects in the brain: U-shape it

    Trends in Pharmacological Sciences

    (2006)
  • Y.B. Kofman et al.

    Cortisol reactivity and depressive symptoms in pregnancy: The moderating role of perceived social support and neuroticism

    Biological Psychology

    (2019)
  • B.M. Kudielka et al.

    Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: A review

    Biological Psychology

    (2005)
  • B.M. Kudielka et al.

    Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2003)
  • C. Kuehner et al.

    Decreased cortisol response to awakening is associated with cognitive vulnerability to depression in a nonclinical sample of young adults

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2007)
  • N. Lasikiewicz et al.

    Exploration of basal diurnal salivary cortisol profiles in middle-aged adults: Associations with sleep quality and metabolic parameters

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2008)
  • M. Lievaart et al.

    Trait anger in relation to neural and behavioral correlates of response inhibition and error-processing

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2016)
  • S.J. Lupien et al.

    The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: Integration of animal and human model studies

    Brain Research Reviews

    (1997)
  • S.J. Lupien et al.

    Stress hormones and human memory function across the lifespan

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2005)
  • S.J. Lupien et al.

    The effects of chronic stress on the human brain: From neurotoxicity, to vulnerability, to opportunity

    Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology

    (2018)
  • J.M. Mateo

    Inverted-u shape relationship between cortisol and learning in ground squirrels

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2008)
  • W.H. Miltner et al.

    Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior cingulate cortex: A source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the error-related negativity

    Biological Psychology

    (2003)
  • W. Notebaert et al.

    Post-error slowing: An orienting account

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • J.C. Pruessner et al.

    Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2003)
  • J.C. Pruessner et al.

    Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity

    Life Sciences

    (1997)
  • K. Quevedo et al.

    The cortisol awakening response and anterior cingulate cortex function in maltreated depressed versus non-maltreated depressed youth

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2017)
  • K.M. Ross et al.

    How stable are diurnal cortisol activity indices in healthy individuals? Evidence from three multi-wave studies

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • T.M. Schilling et al.

    For whom the bell (curve) tolls: Cortisol rapidly affects memory retrieval by an inverted U-shaped dose-response relationship

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2013)
  • X. Shi et al.

    The cortisol awakening response predicts response inhibition in the afternoon of the same day

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2018)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text