Skip to main content
Log in

Non-magnitude sources of bias on duration judgements for blank intervals: conceptual relatedness of interval markers reduces subjective interval duration

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We report three experiments in which the events flanking a temporal interval were either related or unrelated, based on overlap in the letter identity of single letters (Experiment 1), in the conceptual congruency of color words and colored rectangles (Experiment 2), or in the conceptual congruency of sentence stems and their terminal words (Experiment 3). In all cases, we observed a bias for participants to judge the duration of temporal intervals as shorter when the flanking events were related. We draw an analogy between these temporal judgement distortions and those reported elsewhere (Alards-Tomalin et al. in J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 40(2):555–566, 2014) that revealed that the similarity in the relative magnitude of flanking events generate the same type of bias on duration judgements. The observation that non-magnitude dimensions of relatedness between flanking events can also bias duration judgements raise questions about the applicability of two influential theoretical frameworks for understanding the distorting effects that non-temporal stimulus dimensions can have on duration judgments, A Theory of Magnitude (Buetl and Walsh in Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 12:1831–1840, 2009, Walsh in Trends Cogn Sci 7:483–488, 2003) and the Conceptual Metaphor Theory (e.g., Lakoff and Johnson in Philosophy in the flesh: the embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. Basic Books, New York, 1999). In our general discussion, we consider a number of alternative frameworks that may account for these findings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data/material

The data set will be provided upon request.

Notes

  1. Similarly, it is conceivable that encountering only low constraint sentence stems in the practice phase may have had a biasing effect on our participants. Perhaps participants experienced an expectancy violation when they encountered high constraint stems when they encountered then during the experimental trials? If so, might that explain why participants were biased to judge intervals following High Constraint stems as long? We are not convinced that participants carried strong expectations about the type of sentence stems that they encountered during the practice phase into the experimental session. Participants knew they were in a warm-up phase and we expect that they were primarily focused on trying to get a sense of the difference between long and short intervals. Nevertheless, we would again point out that a low probability of marker relatedness produced a bias to judge intervals as shorter in Experiments 1 and 2. Suggesting that an expectancy violation was the source of participants’ bias to judge intervals as long on High Constraint trials in Experiment 3 is not compatible with a holistic consideration of the results of our three experiments.

  2. One of our reviewers pointed out that, although we equated the number of AX Repetition and XB Repetition trials in Experiments 1 and 2, the likelihood of encountering an AX letter repetition was a bit lower than the likelihood of encountering an XB letter repetition or a colored rectangle/color word sequence. Given any letter or color word as the A item in those experiments, the likelihood that the X item would match was .10 (40 AX Repetition trials/400 total trials) in Experiment 1 and .25 (48 AX Congruent experimental trials/192 total experimental trials) in Experiment 2. However, given the presentation of non-matching AX items, the likelihood that the B item would match the preceding X item was about .11 (40 XB Repetition trials/40 XB Repetition trials + 320 No Repetition trials) in Experiment 1 and about .33 (48 XB Congruent trials/48 XB Congruent trials + 96 No Repetition trials) in Experiment 2. As a result, although participants were less likely to encounter matching AX and XB items than non-matching items, matching XB items would have been somewhat more expected than matching AX items.

    Across both experiments, our results establish that participants’ likelihood of making a long-short judgement was lower on AX related trials than on XB related trials. Despite the possible difference in whether participants expected a relatedness between AX items vs. between XB items in Experiments 1 and 2, encountering a match in either case biased participants to judge the interval separating them as shorter. Consequently, we were curious as to whether the potential difference in participants’ expectations that they would encounter matching AX vs. XB markers generated a difference between their likelihood of judging AX intervals as short-long and XB intervals as long-short. In Experiment 1, participants judged AX Repetition intervals as short-long on .530 of trials and XB Repetition intervals as long-short on .518 of trials. In Experiment 2, participants judged AX Congruent intervals as short-long on .561 of trials and XB Congruent intervals as long-short on .555 of trials. Although, in both experiments, participants’ likelihood of judging intervals marked by related items was nominally higher when AX items were related than when XB items were related, these differences were not statistically significant. Thus, we are unable to make any strong inferences based on these differences. If differences in the participants’ expectations for encountering a related AX vs. XB items contributed to our results, the effect was quite subtle.

References

  • Abe, S. (1935). Experimental study on the correlation between time and space. Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 3, 53–68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alards-Tomalin, D., Leboe-McGowan, J. P., Shaw, J. D. M., & Leboe-McGowan, L. C. (2014). The effects of numerical magnitude, size, and color saturation on perceived interval duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 555–566.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and experience: Universal metaphors of time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, F. T. (1979). Credibility of repeated statements: Memory for trivia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 241–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., & Armour, V. (1991). Repetition and the ring of truth: Biasing comments. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 23(2), 195–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birngruber, T., Schroeter, H., & Ulrich, R. (2014). Duration perception of visual and auditory oddball stimuli: Does judgment task modulate the temporal oddball effect? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 76, 814–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A. (1982). Temporal judgments and contextual change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8(6), 530–544.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, R. A., George, E. J., & Reed, M. A. (1980). A watched pot sometimes boils: A study of duration experience. Acta Psychologica, 46(2), 81–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5, 341–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of times east: Absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community. Psychological Science, 21, 1635–1639.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. W. (1995). Time, change, and motion: The effects of stimulus movement on temporal perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 105–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representations of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 12, 1831–1840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cai, M. B., Eagleman, D. M., & Ma, W. J. (2015). Perceived duration is reduced by repetition but not by high-level expectation. Journal of Vision, 15(13), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cappelletti, M., Muggleton, N., & Walsh, V. (2009). Quantity without numbers and numbers without quantity in the parietal cortex. NeuroImage, 46, 522–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umitlà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102, 101–117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106(2), 579–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23, 643–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, K. M., & Yeh, S. L. (2009). Asymmetric cross-modal effects in time perception. Acta Psychologica (Amsterdam), 130, 225–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1953). A new phenomenon in time judgment. Nature, 172, 901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1954). Interdependence of temporal and auditory judgments. Nature, 174, 642–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., Hansel, C. E. M., & Sylvester, J. D. (1955). Interdependence of judgments of space, time and movement. Acta Psychologica, 11, 360–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Linden, D. E. J., Gever, W., Berger, A., & Henik, A. (2007a). The brain locus of interaction between number and size: A combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and event-related potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 957–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., Cohen Kadosh, K., Schuhmann, T., Kaas, A., Goebel, R., Henik, A., et al. (2007b). Virtual dyscalculia induced by parietal-lobe TMS impairs automatic magnitude processing. Current Biology, 17, 689–693.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collyer, C. E. (1977). Discrimination of spatial and temporal intervals defined by three light flashes: Effects of spacing on temporal judgments and of timing on spatial judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 357–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1964). Information, acoustic confusion and memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 4, 429–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooperrider, K., & Núňez, R. (2009). Across time, across the body: Transversal temporal gestures. Gesture, 9, 181–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G., & Neath, I. (1995). The influence of pitch on time perception in short melodies. Music Perception, 12(4), 379–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diliberto, K. A., Altarriba, J., & Neill, W. T. (2000). Novel popout and familiar popout in a brightness discrimination task. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(7), 1494–1500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Douglas, K. M., & Bilkey, D. K. (2007). Amusia is associated with deficits in spatial processing. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 915–921.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagleman, D. M., & Paridayath, V. (2009). Is subjective duration a signature of coding efficiency? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 1841–1851.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Edmonds, E. M., Cahoon, D., & Bridges, B. A. (1981). The estimation of time as a function of positive, neutral or negative expectancies. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 17, 259–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enns, J. T., Brehaut, J. C., & Shore, D. L. (1999). The duration of a brief event in the mind’s eye. Journal of General Psychology, 126, 355–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, V. (2004a). The structure of time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, V. (2004b). Language and time: A cognitive linguistics approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition: From single digits to arithmetic. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1461–1483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Reviews Psychology, 35, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geldreich, E. W. (1934). A lecture room demonstration of the Tau-effect. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs, R. W. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S., Ivry, R. B., Franz, E., Perreault, L., & Metthé, L. (1996). Markers’ influence on the duration discrimination of intermodal intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 424–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S., & Rousseau, R. (1991). Judging the relative duration of multimodal short empty time intervals. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Handel, S. (1988). Space is to time as vision is to audition: Seductive by misleading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 315–317.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 107–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haspelmath, M. (1997). From space to time: Temporal adverbials in the world’s languages. Munich: Lincom Europam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, H., & King, S. M. (1931). The Tau effect: An example of psychological relativity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 14, 202–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & Cognition, 10, 389–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, M. J., & McAuley, J. D. (2009). Evaluations of an imputed pitch velocity model of the auditory kappa effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 551.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, M. J., McAuley, J. D., & Zaleha, M. (2009). Evaluation of an imputed pitch velocity model of the auditory Tau effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1399–1413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, S. K., Javadi, A. H., van der Meer, E., & Busch, N. A. (2013). How long depends on how fast: Perceived flicker dilates subjective duration. PLoS ONE, 8, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, E. M., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Dehaene, S. (2005). Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 435–448.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., & Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time: Evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44, 454–461.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Allan, L., Collins, J. C., & Larwill, L. K. (1988). Memory influences subjective experience: Noise judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(2), 240–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 3, 306–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C., Brown, J., & Jasechko, J. (1989a). Becoming famous overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the past. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(3), 326–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1989b). Memory attributions. In H. L. Roediger & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving (pp. 391–422). New Jersey: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B., & Huang, Y. L. (1982). Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the Tau and Kappa effects. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 128–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Review, 83, 323–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. R., Maser, D. J., & Kidd, G. R. (1978). Rate and structure in memory for auditory patterns. Memory & Cognition, 6, 246–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., Paffen, C. L. E., Hogendoorn, H., & Verstraten, F. A. J. (2006). Time dilation in dynamic visual display. Journal of Vision, 6, 1421–1430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanai, R., & Watanabe, M. (2006). Visual onset expands subjective time. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 1113–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, E., & McAuley, J. D. (2013). Effects of pitch distance and likelihood on the perceived duration of deviant auditory events. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1547–1558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. San Diego: Harcourt Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kövecses, Z. (2002). Metaphor: A practical introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubovy, M. (1981). Concurrent-pitch segregation and the theory of indispensable attributes. In M. Kubovy & J. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization (pp. 55–99). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (2nd ed., pp. 203–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In R. W. Gibbs (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 17–38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núňez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lebensfeld, P., & Wapner, S. (1968). Configuration and space-time interdependence. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 106–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leboe, J. P., & Whittlesea, B. W. A. (2002). The inferential basis of familiarity and recall: Evidence for a common underlying process. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(4), 804–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leboe, L. C., & Mondor, T. A. (2007). Item-specific congruency effects in nonverbal auditory stroop. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 71(5), 568–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leboe, L. C., & Mondor, T. A. (2008). The role of a change heuristic in judgments of sound duration. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1122–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lhamon, W. T., & Goldstone, S. (1975). Movement and the judged duration of visual targets. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 5, 53–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKenzie, N. (2007). The kappa effect in pitch/time context (Doctoral dissertation, The Ohio State University).

  • Mackenzie, N., & Jones, M. R. (2005). The auditory kappa effect revisited. In 46th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

  • Matthews, W. J. (2015). Time perception: The surprising effects of surprising stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 172–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, W. J., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Temporal cognition: Connecting subjective time to perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 865–907.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Van Zandt, B. J. (1987). Nonspecific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(4), 646–648.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marghetis, T., & Núňez, R. (2013). The motion behind the symbols: A vital role for dynamism in the conceptualization of limits and continuity in expert mathematics. Topics in Cognitive Science, 5, 299–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masson, M. E. J., & Caldwell, J. I. (1998). Conceptually driven encoding episodes create perceptual misattributions. Acta Psychologica, 98(2–3), 183–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays, 22, 94–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2004). Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: Coincidence detection of oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Research, 21, 139–170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuda, F. (1974). Effects of space and velocity on time estimation in children and adults. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 37, 107–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Directed attention prolongs the perceived duration of a brief stimulus. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1305–1317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miall, C. (1989). The storage of time intervals using oscillating neurons. Neural Computation, 1, 359–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, C. V., & Lee, S. G. (1972). The effect of real and imputed distance on judgments of time: Some experiments on the Kappa effect. Psychonomic Science, 29, 207–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Núňez, R. (2009). Gesture, abstraction, and the embodied nature of mathematics. In W. M. Roth (Ed.), Mathematical representation at the interface of body and culture (pp. 309–328). Charlotte: IAP-Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Koch, G., & Caltagirone, C. (2009). Spatial-temporal interaction in the human brain. Experimental Brain Research, 195, 489–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Mario Vicario, C., Salerno, S., Koch, G., Turriziani, P., Mangano, R., et al. (2008). Perceiving numbers alters time perception. Neuroscience Letters, 438, 308–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ono, F., & Kawahara, J. (2008). The effect of false memory on temporal perception. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 72, 61–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ono, F., Yamada, K., Chujo, K., & Kawahara, J. (2007). Feature-based attention influences later temporal perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 69, 544–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pariyadath, V., & Eagleman, D. (2007). The effect of predictability on subjective duration. PLoS ONE, 2, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pariyadath, V., & Eagleman, D. (2012). Subjective duration distortions mirror neural repetition suppression. PLoS ONE, 7, 1–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinel, P., Piazza, M., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Distributed and overlapping cerebral representations of number, size, and luminance during comparative judgements. Neuron, 41, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poynter, W. D. (1983). Duration judgment and the segmentation of experience. Memory & Cognition, 11, 77–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price-Williams, D. R. (1954). The Kappa effect. Nature, 173(4399), 363–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, D., & Summers, J. (1995). Duration illusions in a train of visual stimuli. Perception, 24, 1177–1187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umiltà, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarrazin, J. C., Giraudo, M. D., Pailhous, J., & Bootsma, R. J. (2004). Tau and Kappa effects revisited: Dynamics of balancing space and time in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 411–430.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schindel, R., Rowlands, J., & Arnold, D. H. (2011). The oddball effect: Perceived duration and predictive coding. Journal of Vision, 11, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime (Version 2.0). [Computer software and manual]. Psychology Software Tools Inc.

  • Schwartz, M. (1982). Repetition and rated truth value of statements. The American Journal of Psychology, 95(3), 393–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Exogenous visual attention prolongs perceived duration. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 68–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 328–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shigeno, S. (1986). The auditory Tau and Kappa effects for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 9–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shigeno, S. (1993). The interdependence of pitch and temporal judgments by absolute pitch possessors. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 682–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ten Hoopen, G., Miyauchi, R., & Nakajima, Y. (2008). Time based illusions in the auditory mode. Psychology of Time, 86, 139–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Traugott, E. (1978). On the expression of spatio-temporal relations in language. In J. H. Greenberg, C. A. Ferguson, & E. A. Moravcsik (Eds.), Universal of human language (Vol. 3, pp. 369–400). New York: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, M. (2013). The information-processing model of timing (Treisman 1963): Its sources and further development. Timing & Time Perception, 1, 131–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, P. U., Intriligator, J., Rivest, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2004). Attention and the subjective expansion of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 1171–1189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7, 483–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H. (2004). Decision processes in models of timing. Acta Psychological, 64, 303–317.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., & Percival, A. (1998). Why “sounds are judged longer than lights”: Application of a model of internal clock in humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 97–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 161–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A. (1993). Illusions of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1235–1253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., Jacoby, L. L., & Girard, K. A. (1990). Illusions of immediate memory: Evidence of an attributional basis for feelings of familiarity and perceptual quality. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 716–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Leboe, J. P. (2000). The heuristic basis of remembering and classification: Fluency, generation, and resemblance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(1), 84–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Williams, L. D. (2001). The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: II. Expectation, uncertainty, surprise, and feelings of familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 14–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and metaphors: Explaining cognitive interactions between space, time, and number. Cortex, 64, 209–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, B., Perlman, M., & Matlock, T. (2014). Using space to talk and gesture about numbers: Evidence from the TV News archive. Gesture, 13, 377–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witherspoon, D., & Allen, L. G. (1985). The effects of prior presentation on temporal judgments in a perceptual identification task. Memory and Cognition, 13, 101–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yeshurun, Y., & Maron, G. (2008). Transient spatial attention and the perceived duration of brief visual events. Visual Cognition, 16, 826–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoblick, D. A., & Salvendy, G. (1970). Influence of frequency on the estimation of time for auditory, visual, and tactile modalities: The Kappa effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 157–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5, 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada to LLM. We would like to thank our two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada to Launa Leboe-McGowan.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Launa C. Leboe-McGowan.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no conflict of interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

Ethics approval

The University of Manitoba Psychology and Sociology Research Ethics Board approved this project (HS22323 P2018:135 Conceptual Associations and Perceptions of the Passage of Time), and therefore, the study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards identified in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

Sentence stem and terminal word combinations in Experiment 3, based on the stimulus set developed by Whittlesea and Williams (2001).

High constraint stems

Terminal words

Congruent

Incongruent

1. The policeman identified himself with his

Badge

Line

2. The sailor marked their position on the

Map

Coat

3. When the music started he asked her to

Dance

Laugh

4. Deep in the forest the hunter shot a

Deer

Lamp

5. She tiptoed across the room without making a

Sound

Plan

6. The electricity went off during the violent

Storm

Bite

7. He escaped from the plane crash without a

Scratch

Lip

8. They swam and played at the

Beach

Bus

9. After the accident he was covered in

Blood

Whisky

10. The hikers got lost when they left the

Trail

Planet

11. He had a bad headache and a very sore

Throat

Canary

12. She carried all her money in her

Purse

Forest

13. The cat waited patiently to catch the

Mouse

Tree

14. The prisoner screamed insults at the

Guard

Bread

15. They sell wine by the bottle or the

Glass

Wound

16. They sat in the garden on an old oak

Bench

Road

17. They ate everything down to the last

Crumb

Slope

18. They went into the tavern to get a

Beer

Sash

19. The new admiral took command of the

Ship

Robe

20. He loves apple juice and drinks it by the

Litre

Branch

21. The children played in the yard at the

School

Railing

22. Nothing smells worse than a dead

Skunk

Ability

23. He panted heavily as he ran up the

Hill

Gathering

24. He rode his bike up and down the

Street

Pond

25. When the car hit a bump he bit his

Tongue

Cloth

26. The poet got stuck writing the second

Verse

Wheeze

27. The conductor took their tickets on the

Train

Boutique

28. The grey horse is the fastest on the

Track

Stake

29. The cat eagerly licked the last drop of

Milk

Plantation

30. The difficult problem strained his

Mind

Gravy

31. She cleaned the kitchen floor with a

Broom

Toast

32. He secured the gate with a lock and

Chain

Jacket

33. The wrestler had a very hairy

Chest

Pint

34. The police were worried about the rate of

Crime

Schooner

35. The little boy got lost in the

Crowd

Lantern

36. The reeling drunk fell into the

Ditch

Pouch

37. At the fashion show she bought a red

Dress

Lawn

38. The dog chased the yellow cat over the

Fence

List

39. The pond was alive with ducks and

Swans

Grime

40. To explain the equation the teacher drew a

Graph

Carafe

41. The gardener spent the day trimming the

Hedge

Felony

42. They moved from the apartment to a

Farm

Shackle

43. She wore a red sweater and a matching

Skirt

Sparrow

44. The tailor sewed buttons on the

Shirt

Grin

45. When she winked at the baby he gave a

Smile

Mop

46. He had a cold that made him sniffle and

Cough

Rush

47. She took a shirt to the cleaners to remove the

Stain

Pole

48. He planted the little tree and supported it with a

Post

Coach

49. She settled for a job as a cashier in a

Shop

Path

50. She tied the parcel together with

String

Paint

51. The logger cut down the tree and sat on the

Log

Cord

52. The coal miners were covered with dust and

Filth

Meadow

53. She sewed the wedding dress with silver

Thread

Feet

54. For breakfast he likes honey and jam on his

Toast

Hand

55. Her hips are tiny and so is her

Waist

Earth

56. That summer the farmer harvested tons of

Corn

Ranch

57. The explorer set out on a voyage around the

World

Stump

58. They sailed across the Pacific in a small

Yacht

Bakery

59. In the darkness of the cellar she lit a

Match

Print

60. The farmer lost his crop to the

Frost

Pit

Low constraint stems

Congruent

Incongruent

1. They didn’t know what to do with the

Fence

Arm

2. Her husband thought it would be nice to have a

Tree

Church

3. What she admired most was his

Imagination

Flask

4. He spent a pleasant day watching the

Fish

Satchel

5. It’s easy to see the mark she made on the

Diagram

Sloop

6. The famous actress was best known for her

Giggle

Key

7. While cutting the lawn she ran the mower over a

Stick

Theft

8. After 4 years of study he was an expert on

Wheat

Legs

9. She couldn’t find a place to put the

Rag

Neck

10. The young singer worried about his

Face

Rodent

11. The old man couldn't find a buyer for his

Boat

Yogurt

12. She thought it was a good time to make fresh

Muffins

Dirt

13. White carpet is very elegant but you risk a

Stain

Cold

14. She thought it was a good idea to use a

Flashlight

Warden

15. They used all her money to buy a

House

Sonnet

16. She didn't have the money to buy a

Dress

Grain

17. He found a new way to use the old

Cable

Smudge

18. She had seen his name in a book about

Assault

Noise

19. There was a lot of noise in the

Crowd

Doe

20. It was his job to clean out the

Ditch

Geese

21. She asked the clerk to show her a nice

Blouse

Register

22. She held on tight to his woolen

Sleeve

Stool

23. While eating supper he suddenly had to

Sneeze

Waltz

24. She decided to invest her money in a big

Store

Candy

25. She had to quit working when she ran out of

Thread

Blood

26. She wrote him a letter telling him about the

World

Quart

27. He took precautions to protect his

Nose

Gale

28. She had saved up miles of red

Rope

Card

29. To finish the garden they had to remove the

Trunk

Lace

30. After waiting for so long he was covered with

Sweat

Carrier

31. Every elected official should carry a

Label

Rat

32. Later that day they went to the

Lake

Hurricane

33. She couldn't help liking the charming

Chair

Blotch

34. He went out to buy sausages and

Gravy

Hail

35. On the corner of the table there was a bit of

Glass

Seashore

36. The clerk spilled coffee all over the

Map

Fuss

37. You can't do very much with just a

Piece

Moose

38. She developed a severe headache at the

Party

Scarf

39. He ran by the spot without wanting a

Drink

Museum

40. He had always enjoyed watching the

Fleet

Yarn

41. In cases like this you don't expect

Hail

Tango

42. Next to the precious tureen was a

Jug

Frigate

43. He didn't really need to have another

Guard

Gradient

44. It was the first time she had ever seen a

Moose

Head

45. He went into the attic to try to see the

Mouse

Judge

46. In the accident she lost her new

Bag

Ear

47. She asked him to stop and pick up another

Gallon

Brain

48. The new building over there will be a

College

Shawl

49. He was always afraid he might meet a

Bear

Tag

50. She searched for him on the far side of the

Hill

Barley

51 She was afraid she was about to make a

Sound

Shaft

52. Her appointment was delayed because of the

Blizzard

Herons

53. He met the love of his life in the

Street

Rhyme

54. That kind of medicine is good for the

Throat

Seat

55. His research was all on the cells of the

Finger

Piece

56. He never thought about the state of the

Circuit

Groan

57. He helped the company build a new

Path

Drink

58. There are few things as relaxing as the

Rain

Bruise

59. Later that evening she told him the second

Verse

Wall

60. Many people go through life without a

Scratch

Graph

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Leboe-McGowan, L.C., Leboe-McGowan, J.P., Fortier, J. et al. Non-magnitude sources of bias on duration judgements for blank intervals: conceptual relatedness of interval markers reduces subjective interval duration. Psychological Research 86, 209–233 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01482-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01482-w

Navigation