Skip to main content
Log in

Representationalism and cognitive culturalism: riders on elephants on turtles all the way down

  • Original Article
  • Published:
American Journal of Cultural Sociology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article examines the influx of neurocognitive concepts in cultural sociology and this development’s consequences for representationalism. In the first part, I examine representationalism in two research programs that have shaped cultural sociology from the cultural turn to the present: Jeffrey Alexander’s “strong program” and Ann Swidler’s “tool kit” theory. I also briefly discuss the mixed and contradictory findings presented in one of sociology’s most-cited cognitive works, Paul DiMaggio’s (Annu Rev Sociol 23:263–87, 1997) programmatic statement on cognitive psychology’s potential contributions to sociology, which catalyzed the discipline’s cognitive turn. In part two, I demonstrate how in working with and against these three pillars in cultural sociology, figures such as Omar Lizardo, Steven Vaisey, and John Levi Martin have drawn on the cognitive neurosciences to re-conceptualize culture in ways that may have profound consequences for representationalism as it is practiced in the field. I conclude by arguing that representationalism is present but suppressed in cognitive cultural theory and its empirical investigations; that representationalism finds support in the neurocognitive sources that cognitive culturalists cite; and by asserting that future general theories of action will be predicated on a more interactive relationship between automatic and deliberative cognitive domains than the cognitive culturalists currently allow.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I do not exhaust the cognitive or neuroscientific works these authors reference. Instead, I focus on some of the central figures as they are presented in the articles, many of which—Bellah, D’Andrade, Gallesse, Lakoff, Miller, Piaget—have been influential beyond the domains and concerns presented here.

  2. See Brekhus and Ignatow (2019, pp. 12–13) for a brief discussion of recent efforts to revise and refine the dual-process model of cognition.

  3. See Alexander (2006, pp. 569–570, note 4) for an indication of how the author interprets the cognitive linguistics of George Lakoff in a manner that substantiates the culture-structural dimensions of Alexander’s research program.

  4. While Haidt discusses morality broadly, he remains focused on decisions and actions that explicitly have a moral component, and for the most part he sticks to using terms such as “moral intuitions.” Vaisey uses the terms moral-schemas and moral dispositions, but his study purports to speak to the broader concept of culture in action, as both the role Swidler’s theory of cultural in action plays in his study, and the title of his article, suggest. In this article, I treat the main subject of Vaisey’s study as the broader of these two domains, or as culture in action.

  5. Currently, this drug classification system is in flux. Tobacco, deeply constituted by narratives of health, class, and freedom [to versus freedom from], for instance, is becoming increasingly polluted. The scent of “marijuana” on an urban, suburban, or almost any other street in the USA, on the other hand, would no longer signify the “dirt,” or social categories of peoples deemed dangerous or “matter out place,” that it would have even a decade ago. The signifier “marijuana” itself is also being interpreted as polluted due to the role the sign played last century in the domination of racial and ethnic minorities, and is being replaced by the term “cannabis.” To return to our main point: If we were to issue the same questionnaire today and interpret its results faithfully through Vaisey’s theoretical representation of cognitive schemas, then to the extent that the questionnaire detects shifts in dispositions, we would be led to conclude that the changes were the product of an accumulation of experiences minus, or unperturbed by, any linguistic or representationalist elements.

  6. Vaisey is accurate when he asserts that D’Andrade, in his presentation of the connectionist networks model, emphasizes that experience is a constituent element of culture and downplays the role of signification. Yet it is also clear that D’Andrade emphasized experience because he believed that the state of theory in anthropology at the time was in danger of succumbing to “extreme idealism” (p. 149) and of embracing “epistemological relativism” (p. 148; italics in original), a development he believed would make “objectivity and science impossible” (p. 148).

    To explain his concern, D’Andrade (1995, p. 148) shares a quote from Marshall Sahlins (who was quoting Leslie White) in which Sahlins advocated in too strong of terms for what D’Andrade interprets as an absolutist form of representationalism:

    Thus [with symbols] man built a new world in which to live…. Between man and nature hung the veil of culture, and he could see nothing save through this medium … permeating everything was the essence of words.

    The veil argument, D’Andrade explains, “comes from the idea that language and other symbol systems determine what we experience” (p. 148–149; emphasis in original). While he clearly chafes at what he interprets as epistemological over-determination, he nonetheless makes it explicit that rejecting the importance of language and signification altogether is not a tenable stance, either. Instead of simply dismissing representations, and in order to establish a causal hierarchy, D’Andrade performs a nifty theoretical maneuver that subsumes language to experience, arguing that

    [in] the connectionist model ‘words’ do not ‘encode’ experience. Rather, words signify schemas, which means that the units activated by a particular speech sound also activate a larger pattern of connections which are the active schema for a particular experience. The sounds of words are like ‘pointers’ to patterns of experience—indices to internal mental structures, not ‘veils’ between reality and experience (p. 149).

    So, while he rejects the absolutist-representational assertion suggested by the veil metaphor, D’Andrade nonetheless maintains that symbols remain an important dimension in cognitive anthropology’s future theories: “By changing the model of human cognition and meaning from a system containing only symbolic serial processing to a system containing both symbolic serial processing and connectionist parallel distributed processing, a number of things about human culture look different” (ibid., emphasis in original).

  7. Given the theoretical and methodological stakes of Martin’s argument, cultural sociologists will be motivated to familiarize themselves with the kinds of methods and findings on which he is basing his assertions. Miller’s argument is based on a meta-analysis of experiments in information processing. The following is a sample of some of the experiments Miller drew on to arrive at his conclusions.

    Miller describes Irwin Pollack’s (1952) uni-dimensional experiment this way: “Pollack… asked listeners to identify tones by assigning numerals to them. The tones were different with respect to frequency, and covered the range from 100 to 8000 cps in equal logarithmic steps. A tone was sounded and the listener responded by giving a numeral. After the listener had made his response, he was told the correct identification of the tone” (Miller 344).

    Miller describes an auditory study done by Pollack and Picks (1954). “They managed to get six different acoustic variables that they could change: frequency, intensity, rate of interruption, on-time fraction, total duration, and spatial location. Each one of these six variables could assume any one of five different values, so altogether there were 56, or 15,625 different tones that they could present. The listeners made a separate rating for each one of these six dimensions. Under these conditions the transmitted information was 7.2 bits, which corresponds to about 150 different categories that could be absolutely identified without error. Now we are beginning to get up into the range that ordinary experience would lead us to expect” (Miller p. 347).

    Miller describes Hayes’s (1952) multi-dimensional stimuli experiment this way. Hayes tried out “five different kinds of test materials: binary digits, decimal digits, letters of the alphabet, letters plus decimal digits, and with 1000 monosyllabic words. The lists were read aloud at the rate of one item per second and the subjects had as much time as they needed to give their responses. A procedure described by [R. S.] Woodworth was used to score the responses” [Miller 1994 (1955), p. 349].

  8. See Smith (2005, pp. 44–45), for a discussion of the steps representationalist cultural sociology should take in order to move beyond Geertz’s “webs of significance” metaphor.

References

  • Alexander, J.C. 1987. The Centrality of the Classics. In Social Theory Today, ed. A. Giddens and J. Turner, 11–57. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, J.C. (1988 [1987]). Action and Its Environments. In Action and Its Environments: Toward a New Synthesis, ed. Alexander, J.C., 301–333. New York: Columbia University Press.

  • Alexander, J.C. 2006. The Civil Sphere. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, J.C., and P. Smith. 1993. The Discourse of American Civil Society: A New Proposal for Cultural Studies. Theory and Society 22 (2): 151–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellah, R. 1970. Between Religion and Science. In Beyond Belief, ed. R. Bellah, 237–257. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellah, R., et al. 1985. Habits of the Heart. Berkeley, CA: UC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berezin, M. 2019. On the Construction Sites of History: Where did Donald Trump Come From? In Politics of Meaning/Meaning of Politics, ed. J.L. Mast and J.C. Alexander. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1977. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1990a. In Other Words: Essays Towards a Reflexive Sociology. Cambridge, MA: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. 1990b. The Logic of Practice. Cambridge, MA: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brekhus, W., and G. Ignatow. 2019. The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Sociology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. 1998. The Sociology of Philosophies: A Global Theory of Intellectual Change. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. 2000. The Magical Number 4 in Short-Term Memory: A Reconsideration of Mental Storage Capacity. Behavior and Brain Sciences 24: 87–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. 1995. The Development of Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, Roy. 2002. Cultural Darwinism and Language. American Anthropologist 104 (1): 223–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. 1967. Of Grammatology. London: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, Paul. 1997. Culture and Cognition. Annual Review of Sociology 23: 263–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., and G. Lakoff. 2005. The Brain’s Concepts: The Role of the Sensory-Motor System in Conceptual Knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22: 455–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2003. The Manifold Nature of Interpersonal Relations: The Quest for a Common Mechanism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 358: 517–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 1966. Religion as a Cultural System. In The Religious Situation, ed. D. Cutler, 639–688. Boston: Beacon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 1973a. Ideology as a Cultural System. In The Interpretation of Cultures, ed. C. Geertz, 193–233. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 1973b. Thick Description: Toward an Interpretive Theory of Culture. In The Interpretation of Cultures, ed. C. Geertz, 3–30. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 1973c. Deep Play: Notes on the Balinese Cockfight. In The Interpretation of Cultures, ed. C. Geertz, 412–453. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorski, P. 2019. Why Evangelicals Voted for Trump: A Critical Cultural Sociology. In Politics of Meaning/Meaning of Politics, ed. J.L. Mast and J.C. Alexander, 164–184. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, K., and J.E. Keeney. 2015a. Impure, or Just Weird? Scenarios Sampling Bias Raises Questions About the Foundation of Morality. Social Psychology and Personality Science 6: 859–868.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, K., and J. Keeney. 2015b. Disconfirming Moral Foundations Theory on its Own Terms: Reply to Graham. Social Psychological and Personality Science 6 (8): 874–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2001. The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review 108 (4): 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2005. The Happiness Hypothesis. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. 2012. The Righteous Mind. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J.R.M. 1952. Memory Span for Several Vocabularies as a Function of Vocabulary Size, in Quarterly Progress Report. Cambridge, MA: Acoustics Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

  • Intraub, H., C. Gottesman, E. Willey, and I. Zuk. 1996. Boundary Extension for Briefly Glimpsed Photographs: Do common perceptual processes result in unexpected memory distortions? Journal of Memory and Language 35: 118–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennett, J., and C. Fine. 2009. Will the Real Moral Judgment Please Stand Up? The Implications of Social Intuitionist Models of Cognition for Meta-Ethics and Moral Philosophy, Ethical Theory and Moral Practice 12 (1): 77–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., and M. Johnson. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, O. 2004. The Cognitive Origins of Bourdieu’s Habitus. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 34: 375–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, O. 2007. ‘Mirror Neurons’, Collective Objects and the Problem of Transmission: Reconsidering Stephen Turner’s Critique of Practice Theory. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 37 (3): 319–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, O. 2014. Beyond the Comtean Schema: The Sociology of Culture and Cognition versus Cognitive Social Science. Sociological Forum 24 (4): 983–989.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, Omar, and M. Strand. 2010. Skills, Tool Kits, Contexts and institutions: Clarifying the Relationship Between Different Approaches to Cognition in Cultural Sociology. Poetics 38 (2): 205–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, J.L. 2010. Life’s a Beach But You’re an Ant, and Other Unwelcome News for the Sociology of Culture. Poetics (Special Issue on: Brain, Mind and Cultural Sociology) 38 (2): 229–244.

  • Mast, J.L. 2019. Introduction: Fragments, Ruptures, and Resurgent Structures: The Civil Sphere and the Fate of ”Civilship” in the Era of Trumpism. In Politics of Meaning/Meaning of Politics, ed. J.L. Mast and J.C. Alexander, 1–16. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, R. 2017. Fast or Slow: Sociological Implications of Measuring Dual-Process Cognition. Sociological Science 4: 196–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A. (1994 [1955]). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review 101 (2): 343–352.

  • Patterson, O. 2014. Making Sense of Culture. Annual Review of Sociology 40: 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, I. 1952. The Information of Elementary Auditory Displays. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 24: 745–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, I., and L. Picks. 1954. Information of elementary multi-dimensional auditory displays. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 26: 155–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugh, A.J. 2013. What Good are Interviews for Thinking About Culture? Demystifying Interpretive Analysis. American Journal of Cultural Sociology 1 (1): 42–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, H. 2011. The Cultural Context of Cognition: What the Implicit Association Test Tells US about How Culture Works. Sociological Forum 26 (1): 121–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. 2007. The Resolute Irresolution of Clifford Geertz. Common Knowledge 13 (2–3): 191–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. 2005. Why War?. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Strand, M., and O. Lizardo. 2015. Beyond World Images. Sociological Theory 33 (1): 44–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strand, M., and O. Lizardo. 2017. The Hysteresis Effect: Theorizing Mismatch in Action. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 47 (2): 164–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swidler, A. 1986. Culture in Action: Symbols and Strategies. American Sociological Review 51 (2): 273–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swidler, A. 2001. Talk of Love: How Culture Matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tavory, I., and A. Swidler. 2009. Condom Semiotics: Meaning and Condom Use in Rural Malawi. American Sociological Review 74 (2): 171–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaisey, S. 2009. Motivation and Justification: A Dual-Process Model of Culture in Action. American Journal of Sociology 114 (6): 1675–1715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The reviewers’ comments and those of the editor helped to sharpen this article’s arguments considerably. The author thanks them for their significant feedback. The author also thanks the participants of the Yale Center for Cultural Sociology Workshop, Alexis Galán and Agustin Casagrande at Excellence Cluster Normative Orders, and Erik Ringmar for their insightful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jason L. Mast.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mast, J.L. Representationalism and cognitive culturalism: riders on elephants on turtles all the way down. Am J Cult Sociol 8, 90–123 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41290-019-00087-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/s41290-019-00087-6

Keywords

Navigation