Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T13:22:00.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonetic discrimination, phonological awareness, and pre-literacy skills in Spanish–English dual language preschoolers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 February 2021

Sara A. SMITH*
Affiliation:
University of South Florida, USA
Sibylla LEON GUERRERO
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education, USA
Sarah SURRAIN
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education, USA
Gigi LUK
Affiliation:
McGill University, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Sara A. Smith University of South Florida, College of Education. E-mail:Sarasmith3@usf.edu

Abstract

The current study explores variation in phonemic representation among Spanish–English dual language learners (DLLs, n = 60) who were dominant in English or in Spanish. Children were given a phonetic discrimination task with speech sounds that: 1) occur in English and Spanish, 2) are exclusive to English, and 3) are exclusive to Russian, during Fall (age m = 57 months) and Spring (age m = 62 months, n = 42). In Fall, English-dominant DLLs discriminated more accurately than Spanish-dominant DLLs between English-Spanish phones and English-exclusive phones. Both groups discriminated Russian phones at or close to chance. In Spring, however, groups no longer differed in discriminating English-exclusive phones and both groups discriminated Russian phones above chance. Additionally, joint English-Spanish and English-exclusive phonetic discrimination predicted children's phonological awareness in both groups. Results demonstrate plasticity in early childhood through diverse language exposure and suggest that phonemic representation begins to emerge driven by lexical restructuring.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, S., Welbourne, S., & Hesketh, A. (2016). Lexical restructuring in preliterate children: Evidence from novel measures of phonological representation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(4), 9971023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainsworth, S., Welbourne, S., Woollams, A., & Hesketh, A. (2019). Contrasting explicit with implicit measures of children's representations: The case of segmental phonology. Language Learning, 69(2), 323–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambrose, S. E., Fey, M. E., & Eisenberg, L. S. (2012). Phonological awareness and print knowledge of preschool children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(3), 811–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, J. L., Morgan, J. L., & White, K. S. (2003). A statistical basis for speech sound discrimination. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 155–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anthony, J. L., Solari, E. J., Williams, J. M., Schoger, K. D., Zhang, Z., Branum-Martin, L., & Francis, D. J. (2009). Development of bilingual phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: The roles of vocabulary, letter knowledge, and prior phonological awareness. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(6), 535564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., & Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 699714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingualism and executive function. Bilingual Cognition and Language: The state of the science across its subfields, 54, 283306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., Peets, K. F., & Yang, S. (2010). Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(4), 525–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birdsong, D. (2018). Plasticity, variability and age in second language acquisition and bilingualism. Frontiers in psychology, 9, 81, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2003). Simultaneous bilingualism and the perception of a language-specific vowel contrast in the first year of life. Language and speech, 46(2–3), 217–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branum-Martin, L., Tao, S., Garnaat, S., Bunta, F., & Francis, D. J. (2012). Meta-analysis of bilingual phonological awareness: Language, age, and psycholinguistic grain size. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, T. C., Yoshida, K. A., Hill, K., & Werker, J. F. (2007). The development of phonetic representation in bilingual and monolingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 455–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bus, A. G., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. Journal of educational psychology, 91(3), 403–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bus, A. G., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., & Fennell, C. T. (2014). Perceptual narrowing in the context of increased variation: Insights from bilingual infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(2), 274–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, B. J. (2014). The foundation of literacy: The child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle (second edition). Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M. (2004). Letter knowledge precipitates phoneme segmentation, but not phoneme invariance. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(3), 212225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Language and phonological skills in children at high risk of reading difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 631–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curenton, J. M., & Justice, L. M. (2008). Children's preliteracy skills: Influence of mothers' education and beliefs about shared reading interactions. Early Education and Development, 19, 261283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, S., & Zamuner, T. S. (2014). Understanding the developing sound system: Interactions between sounds and words. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(5), 589602.Google ScholarPubMed
Eilers, R. E., Wilson, W. R., & Moore, J. M. (1977). Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 20(4), 766780.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enriquez, B. E. (2015). Head Start Early Learning Outcomes Framework: Ages Birth to Five. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Head Start. https://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/pdf/elof-ohs-framework.pdfGoogle Scholar
Fennell, C. T., Tsui, A. S. M., & Hudon, T. M. (2016). Speech perception in simultaneously bilingual infants. Bilingualism across the lifespan: Factors moderating language proficiency. Washington, USA: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech discrimination in 11-month-old bilingual and monolingual infants: a magnetoencephalography study. Developmental Science, 20(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foster, M. E., & Anthony, J. L. (2019). English and Spanish profiles of oral language growth trajectories of dual language learners and monolingual English speakers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Pacific Coast Research Conference. San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Fowlert, A. E., Swainson, B., & Scarborough, H. (2004). Relationships of naming skills to reading, memory, and receptive vocabulary: Evidence for imprecise phonological representations of words by poor readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(2), 247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia-Sierra, A., Ramírez-Esparza, N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2016). Relationships between quantity of language input and brain responses in bilingual and monolingual infants. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 110, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genesee, F. (1989). Early bilingual development: One language or two?. Journal of child language, 16(1), 161179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gildersleeve, C., Davis, B., & Stubbe, E. (1996, November). When monolingual rules don't apply: Speech development in a bilingual environment. In American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Gildersleeve-Neumann, C., & Davis, B. (1998). Learning English in a bilingual preschool environment: Change over time. In Annual Convention of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Goldstein, B., & Washington, P. S. (2001). An initial investigation of phonological patterns in typically developing 4-year-old Spanish–English bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32(3), 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzales, K., Byers-Heinlein, K., & Lotto, A. J. (2019). How bilinguals perceive speech depends on which language they think they're hearing. Cognition, 182, 318330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottardo, A., Gu, Y., Mueller, J., Baciu, I., & Pauchulo, A.L. (2011). Factors affecting the relative relationships between first- and second-language phonological awareness and second language reading. In Durgunoglu, A. Y. (Ed.), Language and literacy development in bilingual settings (pp. 141167). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hammer, C. S., Hoff, E., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D. C., & Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 715733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, C. S., Jia, G., & Uchikoshi, Y. (2011). Language and literacy development of dual language learners growing up in the United States: A call for research. Child Development Perspectives, 5(1), 49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammer, C. S., & Miccio, A. W. (2006). Early language and reading development of bilingual preschoolers from low-income families. Topics in Language Disorders, 26(4), 322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartanto, A., Toh, W. X., & Yang, H. (2018). Bilingualism Narrows Socioeconomic Disparities in Executive Functions and Self-Regulatory Behaviors During Early Childhood: Evidence From the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Child development, 90(4), 1215–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes-Harb, R. (2007). Lexical and statistical evidence in the acquisition of second language phonemes. Second Language Research, 23(1), 6594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2018). Bilingual development in children of immigrant families. Child development perspectives, 12(2), 8086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jiménez González, J. E., & García, C. R. H. (1995). Effects of word linguistic properties on phonological awareness in Spanish children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87(2), 193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. (2004). KBIT-2: Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. 2nd. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Kim, D. H., Lambert, R. G., & Burts, D. C. (2018). Are young dual language learners homogeneous? Identifying subgroups using latent class analysis. The Journal of Educational Research, 111(1), 4357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 9791000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., Deguchi, T., Kiritani, S., & Iverson, P. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental science, 9(2), F13-F21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krizman, J., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2016). Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Developmental Science, 19(6), 881891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leon Guerrero, S., Smith, S., & Luk, G. (2016). Home language usage and executive function in bilingual preschoolers. Cognitive Control and Consequences of Multilingualism. John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2, 351.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In Meeting of the International Academy for Research on Learning Disabilities, Oct, 1986, Northwestern U, Evanston, IL, US. The University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Lonigan, C. J., Schatschneider, C., Westberg, L., & others. (2008). Identification of children's skills and abilities linked to later outcomes in reading, writing, and spelling. Developing Early Literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel, 55106.Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattys, S. L., Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Morgan, J. L. (1999). Phonotactic and prosodic effects on word segmentation in infants. Cognitive psychology, 38(4), 465494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82(3), B101-B111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restrucuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Milburn, T. F., Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2019). Stability of Risk Status During Preschool. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 52(3), 209219. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219418789373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Office of Head Start (2018). Office of Head Start services snapshot: National all programs (2017–2018). Office of Head Start: Washington, DC, USA.Google Scholar
Pace, A., Luo, R., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golinkoff, R. M. (2017). Identifying pathways between socioeconomic status and language development. Annual Review of Linguistics, 3, 285308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Nicoladis, E., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2011). Bilingual children's acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 38(3), 554578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, M., Zong, J., & Batalova, J. (2018). Growing superdiversity among young US dual language learners and its implications. Washington, DC: Migration Policy Institute.Google Scholar
Peirce, J. W. (2009). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in neuroinformatics, 2, 10.Google ScholarPubMed
Pierce, L. J., Genesee, F., Delcenserie, A., & Morgan, G. (2017). Variations in phonological working memory: Linking early language experiences and language learning outcomes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 38(6), 12651300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polka, L., Colantonio, C., & Sundara, M. (2001). A cross-language comparison of/d/–/ð/perception: evidence for a new developmental pattern. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(5), 21902201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quay, S., & Montanari, S. (2016). Early bilingualism: From differentiation to the impact of family language practices. Bilingualism across the lifespan, 23–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing; 2015.Google Scholar
Rigby, R. A., & Stasinopoulos, D. M. (2005). Generalized additive models for location, scale and shape,(with discussion). Applied Statistics, 54, 507554.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., Denmark, N., Harden, B. J., & Stapleton, L. M. (2016). The role of parent education and parenting knowledge in children's language and literacy skills among White, Black, and Latino families. Infant and Child Development, 25(2), 198220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rvachew, S., Ohberg, A., Grawburg, M., & Heyding, J. (2003). Phonological awareness and phonemic perception in 4-year-old children with delayed expressive phonology skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12(4), 463471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeghi, A., & Everatt, J. (2017). Phonological Awareness Development in Speakers of Languages Other Than English. In Gillon, G. (Ed.) Phonological Awareness, Second Edition: From Research to Practice. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Santillán, J., & Khurana, A. (2017). Developmental associations between bilingual experience and inhibitory control trajectories in Head Start children. Developmental science.Google Scholar
Schnitzer, M.L. (1997). Spanish–English Contrastive Phonology. San Juan, PR: Piedras Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Shi, R., Werker, J. F., & Morgan, J. L. (1999). Newborn infants’ sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72(2), B11B21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skoe, E., Burakiewicz, E., Figueiredo, M., & Hardin, M. (2017). Basic neural processing of sound in adults is influenced by bilingual experience. Neuroscience, 349, 278290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(1), 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soto, X., Olszweski, A., & Goldstein, H. (2019). Systematic review of phonological awareness interventions for Latino children in early and primary grades. Journal of Early Intervention.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Storch, S.A., & Whitehurst, G.J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2017). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 115.Google Scholar
Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition, 76(2), 147166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J.K. (2004). Avoiding the devastating downward spiral: The evidence that early intervention prevents reading failure. American Educator, 28(3), 619.Google Scholar
Ventura, P., Kolinsky, R., Fernandes, S., Querido, L., & Morais, J. (2007). Lexical restructuring in the absence of literacy. Cognition, 105, 334361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language learning and development, 1(2), 197234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F., Pons, F., Dietrich, C., Kajikawa, S., Fais, L., & Amano, S. (2007). Infant-directed speech supports phonetic category learning in English and Japanese. Cognition, 103(1), 147162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant behavior and development, 7(1), 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. M. (2004). CELF preschool 2: clinical evaluation of language fundamentals preschool. Pearson/PsychCorp.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2016). Early child L2 acquisition: Age or input effects? Neither, or both?. Journal of child language, 43(3), 608634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed