Skip to main content
Log in

Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children show sex differences in early speech development, with girls producing a greater number and variety of words at an earlier age than boys (Berglund et al. in Scand J Psychol 46(6): 485–491, 2005)—a pattern that also becomes evident in gesture (Butterworth and Morisette in J Reprod Infant Psychol 14(3): 219–231, 1996). Importantly, parents show variability in how they produce speech when interacting with their singleton sons vs. daughters (i.e., Cherry and Lewis in Dev Psychol 12: 278–282, 1976; Leaper et al. in Dev Psychol 34: 3–27, 1998). However, it is unknown whether the variability in speech input extends to different twin dyads or becomes evident in gesture input. In this study, we examined parental gesture and speech input to 35 singleton (19 boys, 16 girls) and 62 twin (10 boy–boy, 9 girl–girl, and 12 girl–boy dyads) Turkish children (age range = 0;10–3;4) in parent–child interactions. We asked whether there is evidence of sex (girls vs. boys) or group (singletons vs. twins) differences in parents’ speech and gesture production, and whether these differences also become evident in different twin dyads (girl–girl, boy–boy, girl–boy). Our results, based on parent-child interactions, largely showed no evidence of sex or dyad-composition difference in either parent speech or gesture, but evidence of a group difference in gesture, with the parents of singletons providing a greater amount, diversity, and complexity of gestures than parents of twins in their interactions. These results suggest that differences in parent input to singletons vs. twins might become evident initially in gesture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the recordings where both mother and father were present, they took turns while interacting with their children; we therefore included input across the two parents. The co-presence of mother and father during the interaction was equally distributed across all 5 groups, with n = 1 per group.

  2. The age of the grandmother was not recorded.

  3. The mean length of interaction varied by group, Msingletons = 10.06 (SD = 4.31) vs. Mtwins = 14.32 (SD = 4.65), F(1, 64) = 14.95, p < .001, but not by sex, Mgirls = 12.40 (SD = 5.65) vs. Mboys = 10.97 (SD = 4.31), F(1, 52) = 1.12, p = .29, or by twin dyad type, Mtwin boy–boy = 13.40 (SD = 3.41), Mtwin girl–girl = 15.78 (SD = 6.16), Mtwin boy- girl = 14.00 (SD = 4.39), F(2, 28) = 0.65, p = .53.

  4. For the 5 parent-child interactions where both parents were present, we tabulated speech production across the two parents.

  5. For the 5 parent-child interactions where both parents were present, we tabulated gesture production across the two parents.

  6. We conducted all analyses first with the whole sample, and then with a reduced sample by excluding the 5 parent-child interactions (n = 1/group) in which both parents were present. The pattern of results remained identical; we therefore only reported the results based on the whole sample.

References

  • Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development. Human Development, 28, 40–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1989). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aldrich, N. J., Brooks, P. J., Yuksel-Sokmen, P. O., Ragir, S., Flory, M. J., Lennon, E. M., Karmel, B. Z., & Gardner, J. M. (2015). Infant twins’ social interactions with caregivers and same-age siblings. Infant Behavior and Development, 41, 127–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 239–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2013). Spanish parents’ emotion talk and their children’s understanding of emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 670.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1986). Infants’ conventionalized acts: Gestures and words with mothers and peers. Infant Behavior and Development, 9(2), 215–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976). Language and context. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). Cognition and communication from nine to thirteen months: Correlation findings. The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in iIfancy, 33–68.

  • Baumann, S., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2019). Do early school-aged children’s gestures reflect parental gesture input in autism and typical development? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 67.

  • Bekken, K. (1989). Is there motherese in gesture? [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago.

  • Berglund, E. V. A., Eriksson, M., & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status in 18-month‐old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46(6), 485–491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 207–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 235–258). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, S., McMahon, C., & Ungerer, J. A. (2003). Maternal speech style with prelinguistic twin infants. Infant and Child Development, 12(2), 129–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Morisette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14(3), 219–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, L., & Lewis, M. (1976). Mothers and two-year-olds: A study of sex differentiated aspects of verbal interaction. Developmental Psychology, 12, 278–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, P. M., & Dickman, Z. (1984). Features of interaction of infant twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 33, 165–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 10(4), 352–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, D., Lytton, H., & Pysh, F. (1980). Twin—singleton language differences. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 12, 264–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P., Abraham, J., Berkman, S., & Staska, M. (1997). The development of infants’ preference for motherese. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 477–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, E. A. (1937). The development of linguistic skills in twins, singletons with siblings, and only children from age five to ten years. University of Minnesota Child Welfare Monograph Series, 14, 174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, E. (1932). The development of language in twins. Child Development, 3, 298–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S. (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 66–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dimitrova, N., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Adamson, L. B. (2017). Do verbal children with autism comprehend gesture as readily as typically developing children? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 3267–3280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, M., & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative development. First Language, 19, 55–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, M., Marschik, P. B., Tulviste, T., Almgren, M., Pérez Pereira, M., Wehberg, S., & Gallego, C. (2012). Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: Evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(2), 326–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short form versions of the MacArthur communicative development inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(1), 95–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furrow, D., Nelson, K., & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language, 6, 423–442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galsworthy, M. J., Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development. Developmental Science, 3(2), 206–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garitte, C., Almodovar, J. P., Benjamin, E., & Canhao, C. (2002). Speech in same-and different- sex twins 4 and 5 years old. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 5(6), 538–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 43–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P., & Smith, J. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gücüyener, K., Arhan, E., Soysal, A. S., Ergenekon, E., Turan, O., Onal, E., Koç, Turkyılmaz, C., & Atalay, Y. (2011). Language assessment of non-handicapped twins at 5 years of age. Pediatrics International, 53(6), 944–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay, D., Prior, M., Collett, S., & Williams, M. (1987). Speech and language development in pre- school twins. Acta Geneticae Medicae Gemellologiae, 36, 239–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, L., & Özçalışkan, Ş, & Williamson, R. (2018). Type of iconicity influences children’s comprehension of gesture. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 327–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language Input and Child Syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45(3), 337–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1062–1083.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 343–365.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 368–371.

  • Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14(1), 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J., Önen, F., Demir, N., Uzlukaya, A., & Uludağ, P. (1974). A Turkish Peabody picture- vocabulary test. Hacettepe Bulletin of Social Sciences & Humanities, 6(1–2), 129–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketrez, F. N. (2014). İkizlerde dil edinimi. Unpublished research report submitted to TUBITAK.

  • Ketrez, F. N. (2016). Different paces (but not different paths) in language acquisition. In M. Güven, D. Akar, B. Öztürk & M. Kelepir (eds). Exploring the Turkish linguistic landscape: Essays in honor of Eser Erguvanlı-Taylan (pp. 183–202). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ketrez, F. N. (2017). Sibling influence on morphological development? In Ketrez, F. Nihan, Aylin Küntay, Şeyda Özçalışkan, & Aslı Özyürek (Eds.) Social environment and cognition in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç. Trends in Language Acquisition Research 21, 99–110. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

  • Ketrez, F. N., & Aksu-Koç, A. (2020). Turkish lexicon for morphosyntactic coding. Manuscript in preparation.

  • Kimura, D. (1998). Sex and cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., & Malcuit, G. (2002). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers’ involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free-play with their infants at 9 and 15 months. Sex Roles, 47, 507–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., Anderson, K. J., & Sanders, P. (1998). Moderators of gender effects on parents’ talk to their children: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 34, 3–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. S. (1993). Pretend play skills and the child’s theory of mind. Child Development, 64(2), 348–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, H. M. (2014). Lexical and acoustic features of maternal utterances addressing preverbal infants in picture book reading link to 5-year-old children’s language development. Early Education and Development, 25, 1103–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lock, A. E. (Ed.). (1978). Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lytton, H., Conway, D., & Suave, R. (1977). The impact of twinship on parent-child interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 97–107.

  • Maccoby, E. (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (3rd ed). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Mascaro, J. S., Rentscher, K. E., Hackett, P. D., Mehl, M. R., & Rilling, J. K. (2017). Child gender influences paternal behavior, language, and brain function. Behavioral Neuroscience, 131(3), 262–273.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 12, 93–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Namy, L. L., Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (2000). Verbal labels and gestural routines in parental communication with young children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 63–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Namy, L., Vallas, R., & Knight-Schwarz, J. (2008). Linking parent input and child receptivity to symbolic gestures. Gesture, 8, 302–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1983). A pragmatic approach to early language acquisition. Paper presented at: The Study Group on Crosscultural and Crosslinguistic Aspects of Native Language Acquisition. Jerusalem, Israel: Institute for Advanced Studies, Hebrew University.

  • Ninio, A. (1984). Functions of speech in mother–infant interaction. Israel: Final science report to the U.S.-Israel Binational Science Foundation. Jerusalem.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2017). Early gesture provides a helping hand to spoken vocabulary development for children with autism, Down syndrome, and typical development. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(3), 325–337.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., & Baumann, S. (2018). Do parents model gestures differently when children’s gestures differ? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 1492–1507.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Dimitrova, N. (2013). How gesture Input provides a helping hand to language development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 227–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, Gentner, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Do iconic gestures pave the way for children’s early verbs? Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 1143–1162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005a). Do parents lead their children by the hand? Journal of Child Language, 32(3), 481–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005b). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96, B101–B113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006a). ‘X is like Y’: The emergence of similarity mappings in children’s early speech and gesture. In G. Kristianssen, M Achard, R.Dirven & F. R. de Mendoza (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics: Foundations and fields of application (pp. 229–262). Mouton de Gruyter.

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006b). Role of gesture in children’s early constructions. In E. Clark & B. Kelly (Eds.), Constructions in acquisition (pp. 31–58). Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, Goldin-Meadow, S., Gentner, D., & Mylander, C. (2009). Does language about similarity foster similarity comparisons in children? Cognition, 112(2), 217–228.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture–speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 190–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Sex differences in language first appear in gesture. Developmental Science, 13(5), 752–760.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ozturk, S., Pınar, E., Ketrez, F. N., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2021). Effect of sex and dyad composition on speech and gesture development of singleton and twin children. Journal of Child Language.

  • Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development, 76, 763–782.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, J. R. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers’ speech to young children: Age and sex comparisons. Child Development, 44, 182–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramer, A. L. (1976). Syntactic styles in emerging language. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 49–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Zubrick, S. R., Taylor, C. L., Gayán, J., & Bontempo, D. E. (2014). Late language emergence in 24-month-old twins: Heritable and increased risk for late language emergence in twins. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(3), 917–928.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language, 7, 353–369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language, 20, 305–330.

  • Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş, & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., & Redshaw, J. (1991). Growing up as a twin: Twin–singleton differences in psychological development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 885–896.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Thorpe, K., Greenwood, R., Northstone, K., & Golding, J. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: I: design; twin–singleton differences in language, and obstetric risks. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 44(3), 326–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savić, S. (1980). How twins learn to talk: A study of the speech development of twins from 1 to 3. Academic Press.

  • Schaffer, H. R., & Crook, C. K. (1979). Maternal control techniques in a directed play situation.Child Development, 50, 989–996.

  • Shatz, M. (1982). On mechanisms of language acquisition: can features of the communicative environment account for development? In E. Wanner & E. L. Gleitman (Eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art (pp. 102–127). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43, 549–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: fine-tuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In P. M. Fletcher (Ed.), The handbook of child language (pp. 180–194). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., & Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Talking to children: Language input and acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stafford, L. (1987). Maternal input to twin and singleton children: Implications for language acquisition. Human Communication Research, 13(4), 429–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanfield, C., Williamson, R. A., & Özçalışkan, Ş (2014). How early do children understand gesture-speech combinations with iconic gestures? Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 462–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, K. J., Golding, J., MacGillivray, I., & Greenwood, R. (1991). Comparison of prevalence of depression in mothers of twins and mothers of singletons. British Medical Journal, 302, 875–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, K. J., Greenwood, R., & Rutter, M. (2003). Twins as a natural experiment to study the causes of mild language delay: II. Family interaction risk factors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(3), 342–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Mannie, S., & Barton, M. (1989). The development of communicative competence in twins. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 2(1), 49–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Mannle, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1986). Linguistic environment of 1-to 2-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 169–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S. K., Chan, C., Li, H., & Kwong, S. M. (2002). Sex differences in syntactic development: Evidence from Cantonese-speaking preschoolers in Hong Kong. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(6), 509–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts and vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 293–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the helpful comments provided by the reviewers and the editor, Dr. Boone, on an earlier version of the manuscript. We also thank Melis Çiftçi and Fatih Şahinkayası for their help with data transcription and coding, along with the families for their participation in our study. The data collection for the project was funded by a Grant to N.K. (Ketrez-Sözmen, Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey, 111K270).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ebru Pınar.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 4.

Table 4 Correlational matrix for parents’ speech and gesture production (significant correlations are in bold)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pınar, E., Ozturk, S., Ketrez, F.N. et al. Parental Speech and Gesture Input to Girls Versus Boys in Singletons and Twins. J Nonverbal Behav 45, 297–318 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-020-00356-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-020-00356-w

Keywords

Navigation