Skip to main content

An IERI – International Educational Research Institute Journal

School students’ beliefs about abilities and perspective-taking over time

Abstract

Background

Beliefs about one’s own abilities and perspective-taking are essential for appropriate behavior in professional and social life, cooperative learning, and situations were conflict is occurring. The social comparison theory and previous research suggested positive relations between school students’ beliefs about one’s own abilities and perspective-taking. The hypotheses were: (1) Beliefs about one’s own abilities and perspective-taking are positively related to each other after the transition to secondary school and (2) will fade thereafter.

Methods

The hypotheses were tested using data from two national longitudinal studies. In Study 1, analyses were based on a sample consisting of the same N = 4428 students in fifth, seventh, and ninth grades. Beliefs about one’s own abilities and perspective-taking were analyzed by one structural equation model considering relevant covariates, namely gender, school achievement, ethnic background, school type, and socioeconomic status. In Study 2, the sample consisted of N = 2105 students who were assessed at the beginning, during, and at the end of fifth grade. Beliefs about one’s own abilities and perspective-taking were analyzed by the same structural equation model as in Study 1 including the same covariates and additional cognitive abilities. Results: Relations only existed from the first to the second measurement point in both studies.

Conclusions

A possible explanation for the changed relationship over time is that the students adjusted to their peers, and likely learned to deal with diverse conflicting situations and to differentiate between academic, social or other abilities over the course of the school year. Perspective-taking items measure coordination between oneself and other people in social interactions and conflicting situations. In the future, such relations could be analyzed using domain-specific perspective-taking items.

Introduction

Students are not alone at school; they interact with each other and adapt to their peers (e.g., Levesque 1993), relatives (e.g., Lillard 1998), and teachers (e.g., Gehlbach et al. 2016). After transitioning to secondary school, students share a class with other students who are more or less previously unknown. “Social means” of comparison will tend to be more available than “non-social means” (Festinger 1954, p. 118ff.) such as school grades, and students are likely to take into account what other students say or do. In this case, their beliefs about their own abilities would be in part a function of social comparisons.

School students compare themselves to other students socially and academically. Academic self-concept refers to one’s beliefs about one’s own academic abilities or the cognitive representation of one’s own academic abilities (e.g., Marsh et al. 2014). Strong evidence exists concerning general and domain-specific academic self-concepts (mathematics versus language, e.g., Marsh 1986). For example, findings indicate that mathematical and verbal academic self-concepts are strong predictors of later school achievement (Möller et al. 2009).

However, whether academic self-concept supports or hinders social learning and socially appropriate behavior at school is understudied. One foundation for social learning and socially appropriate behavior is the cognitive capacity to engage in perspective-taking (e.g., Piaget 1972). Since Piaget proposed his theory, researchers from different schools of thought have proposed several theoretical and methodological approaches to examining perspective-taking concepts (e.g., Bosacki 2000) and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking (e.g., Davis 1980, 1983). Two of these approaches are outlined below.

Perspective-taking was conceptualized within the cognitive-developmental framework theory of mind (e.g., Baron-Cohen et al. 1985; Bosacki 2000; Wimmer and Perner 1983). Having a theory of mind involves the ability to distinguish between oneself and another individual’s self. Without this basic ability, one would expect that “what I think or believe, other people think or believe as well”. With a theory of mind, one expects differences between one’s own subjective thoughts or beliefs (e.g., intentions) and another person’s thoughts or beliefs. Thus, having a theory of mind is an essential feature of perspective-taking as a source of beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking.

The theory of mind is often assessed with false-belief tasks (e.g., Wimmer and Perner 1983) or social perspective-taking tasks (e.g., Bosacki 2000). Findings indicate that children without special needs have already developed a theory of mind at 4 years of age (e.g., Hughes and Ensor 2007). Thus, school students without special needs are able to distinguish between themselves and others’ self. Bloom and German (2000) summarized theory of mind research using false-belief tasks as an approach that focusses on specific situations instead of the tendency to engage in perspective-taking or beliefs about one’s perspective-taking. Theory-of-mind researchers are mainly interested in the development of conceptual perspective-taking skills (situation-specific attempts to anticipate others’ intentions, e.g., Baron-Cohen et al. 1985). Moreover, researchers have created “overlaps with cognitive empathy and the terms are used interchangeably” (Lawrence et al. 2004, p. 911).

Empathy is defined as a multidimensional concept with two basic dimensions: The emotional dimension of empathy reflects the tendency to engage in emotional reactions and compassionate responses (e.g., Davis 1980). The cognitive dimension of empathy refers to one’s cognitive capacity, including the ability of perspective-taking towards a human (-like) target (e.g., Devoldre et al. 2010). Individuals who demonstrate high levels of beliefs about their own social perspective-taking (measured by a task-related questionnaire) engaged more in understanding a dissimilar target in a scenario-based task than those demonstrating low levels of such beliefs (Chambers and Davis 2012).

Beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking also explained variance in situation-specific perspective-taking in other studies, suggesting that the two concepts are related to each other (e.g., Nelson et al. 2017). Researchers in these studies (e.g., Nelson et al. 2017) used a “perspective-taking” subscale adapted from Davis (1980). Davis’ (1983, p. 113) subscale assesses the “tendency to spontaneously adopt the psychological point of view of others”. It is a self-report measure that assesses one’s beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking. Such beliefs represent a disposition to mentalize or simulate different points of view in social interactions and conflict situations. This conceptualization considers perspective-taking as a simulation theory of someone else’s self (e.g., Chambers and Davis 2012). Davis’ perspective-taking subscale has been used in other studies to investigate relations to other constructs (e.g., empathy-quotient validation studies regarding theory of mind, Lawrence et al. 2004; executive functions and mindfulness, Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015).

The relationship between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking is rarely examined, although one might argue that an overarching self-evaluation of one’s abilities shapes one’s self-evaluation of both one’s academic abilities and one’s social abilities. The self-evaluation of academic abilities involves academic self-concept and the self-evaluation of social abilities involves beliefs about one’s perspective-taking in social interactions and conflict situations. Students with an intention to invest in perspective-taking might hold a more valid, realistic academic self-concept that in many cases might be lower than a self-concept generated without such an intention.

Conversely, academic self-concept, as one aspect of understanding oneself, might contribute to subsequent beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking or understanding others in social interactions and conflict situations. The link between self-evaluation and self-understanding or self-reflection is well established, with findings indicating that understanding one’s own self is related to understanding others’ self (see Dimaggio et al. 2008, for a review). In line with this assumption and research, the results of a transversal study (Bosacki 2000) based on theory of mind indicated positive relations between children’s self-concept at school and their social perspective-taking. Bosacki (2000) assessed self-concept at school using Harter’s (1985) self-perception inventory for children. A longitudinal study (Bosacki 2014, p. 217) including assessments of self-perceptions and “theory of mind understanding (social ambiguous stories)” found positive relationships between self-perceptions and theory of mind understanding in students 8 years of age that had faded 2 years later.

Longitudinal relations between academic self-concept and perspective-taking over 4 years of school would extend existing evidence on theory of mind research with other measures (e.g., Bosacki 2000) and simulation theory or cognitive empathy research (e.g., Chambers and Davis 2012; Davis 1980, 1983). Longitudinal data including beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking are available (see Fischer et al. 2017; and Quellenberg 2009, for scale documentation). The aim of the current studies was therefore to describe longitudinal relations between students’ academic self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking following the transition to secondary school.

In accordance with the findings presented by Bosacki (2000), the interim hypothesis was: academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own social perspective-taking are positively related to each other in children and adolescents. In the next section, links between beliefs about one’s own abilities and conditions for social comparisons are outlined.

Beliefs about oneself in social comparison theory

Festinger (1954) described nine hypotheses about an individual’s abilities and opinions in private or social conditions within his social comparison theory. Later findings supported the social comparison hypothesis (e.g., Marsh et al. 2014; see Wrzus et al. 2013, for a meta-analysis). Research on academic self-concept usually refers to social comparison theory as the internal–external frames of reference model (e.g., Marsh 1986). This model has frequently been tested with regard to internal frames of reference and math versus language abilities (e.g., see Möller et al. 2009, for a meta-analytic path analysis) or external frames of reference regarding different school types (e.g., Marsh 1987; Marsh et al. 2014). The big-fish-little-pond effect is based in part on social comparison theory and related theoretical perspectives positing that students compare their own academic accomplishments to those of others, and that this serves as one basis for the formation of their academic self-concept (e.g., Marsh et al. 2014). This is particularly relevant in Germany, where students are stratified by ability at an early age (about nine or 10 years of age). Indeed, children at school not only compare their own abilities to others, they compare clothes, bags, and electronic devices. The more children perceive similarities when they compare themselves with other children, the more likely they will find it easy to engage in self-simulation to others (e.g., Chambers and Davis 2012).

Festinger (1954, p. 118) assumed in his Hypothesis II that if “non-social means are not available, people evaluate their opinions and abilities by comparison respectively with the opinions and abilities of others.” Thus, each student might compare his/her statements or behavior with others’ statements or behavior, which might provide cues about others’ abilities as an external frame of reference. The students might evaluate their abilities by comparing them to the abilities of others during in-class activities: the better one subjectively understands others’ academic statements or questions, the higher one’s academic-self-concept level. Such social comparisons require considering other students, understanding their opinions, and comprehending their statements or the quality of their responses (e.g., correct or incorrect answers). Considering and understanding others even in conflict situations is related to beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking assessed by Davis’ (1980) perspective-taking subscale (e.g., Nelson et al. 2017). The better one subjectively understands other students in different (conflict) situations, the higher one’s level of belief about one’s own social perspective-taking. Appropriate assumptions about others’ opinions and abilities linked to self-evaluation affect children’s self-concepts (e.g., Bosacki 2000; Sebastian et al. 2008). Thus, self-concept might not only contribute to beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking, such beliefs might predict later self-concept as well. Accordingly, the interim hypothesis mentioned above has been changed to the assumption of a reciprocal relationship between these two constructs over time.

Changes over time have been found for students’ academic self-concept (e.g., Möller and Köller 2001) and beliefs about their own social perspective-taking (e.g., Van der Graaff et al. 2014). Previous research has suggested further relevant factors related to academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking in children and adolescents. Any analysis of the relations between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own social perspective-taking should take this previous research as a starting point.

Relevant factors for beliefs about oneself

Previous research has indicated that individual differences in academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own social perspective-taking can be explained by gender, cognitive abilities (e.g., school achievement, fluid reasoning, receptive vocabulary, or reading fluency), ethnic background, type of school, and the socioeconomic status of the student’s family. Gender differences among students with regard to academic self-concept are related to roles and contexts at school (e.g., Wigfield and Eccles 2000). For example, girls reported higher academic self-concept levels in reading than boys (e.g., Jacobs et al. 2002). The difference between girls’ and boys’ academic self-concept levels in reading has been shown to increase over time (e.g., Jacobs et al. 2002), even though overall academic self-concept levels in reading decreased from 1st to 12th grade in both girls and boys (Archambault et al. 2010). Van der Graaff et al. (2014) found that gender correlated with beliefs about one’s own social perspective-taking in both children and adolescents, and that girls’ levels were significantly higher than boys’ levels of such beliefs at six measurement points. The researchers (Van der Graaff et al. 2014) used Davis’ (1980) perspective-taking subscale.

A domain-specific self-concept such as academic self-concept in reading is related to students’ school achievement in that domain (e.g., Marsh 1986). Indicators of school achievement (e.g., Helmke and van Aken 1995; Karbach et al. 2013) are students’ school grades, e.g., in math class or literature class. Positive relations have been found between opportunities to practice mindfulness attention in combination with beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking and teacher-rated math grades (Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015). Fluid reasoning was significantly correlated with students’ self-concept in mathematics but not with their verbal self-concept (e.g., Mustafic et al. 2017). Reading fluency was positively related to students’ reading self-concept (e.g., Kasperski et al. 2016). Verbal abilities in the form of receptive vocabulary (e.g., Bosacki 2000) are correlated with self-concept as measured by Harter’s (1985) academic competence subscale. However, beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking ware barely correlated (females: r = .07, males: r = .15) with receptive vocabulary on the Wechsler adult intelligence scale (Davis 1983, p. 118).

Students’ ethnic backgrounds contribute to their self-concept via self-construal levels: While Western individuals with an individualistic mindset tend to overestimate their abilities, Asian individuals who see themselves as part of an interdependent network of peers and relatives demonstrate a collectivistic mindset and tend to estimate their abilities realistically (e.g., see Han and Humphreys 2016 for a review; Singelis 1994). Similarly to Davis (1980), Lillard (1998) highlighted variation in simulating someone else’s situation. Specifically, she proposed cultural sources of variation in perspective-taking. She argued that because “children grow into different cultures with different practices, it might make sense that they form different ideas about the mind that fit those practices” (p. 5). In line with this claim, Atkins et al. (2016) found inter-cultural differences in beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking using Davis’ (1980) items, that is, British participants reported higher levels than Asian university students. Ethnic background might also be relevant for adolescents’ perspective-taking at an earlier stage (Atkins et al. 2016).

According to the well-known big-fish-little-pond effect (e.g., Marsh 1987), academic self-concept levels are related to external frames of reference such as the type of school. Thus, moderate achievers have higher academic self-concept levels in schools with a vocational rather than an academic curriculum. Coming from a family with a low socioeconomic status negatively affected students’ academic self-concept, and academic self-concept, in turn, was positively related to peer-assisted learning (e.g., see Ginsburg-Block et al. 2006 for a meta-analytic review).

Taken together, academic self-concept (e.g., Marsh 1989) and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking (e.g., Davis 1980) are conceptualized as mental representations of an individual’s abilities (e.g., Bosacki 2000; O`Brien et al. 1992). Festinger’s Hypothesis II (1954, p. 118) assumes that “objective” and “non-social means” of comparing abilities are not available following the transition to secondary school, and that students evaluate their abilities vis-à-vis the perceived abilities of others and by understanding other students in conflict situations. Several further relevant factors should be borne in mind, namely gender, school achievement, cognitive ability scores, ethnic background, the type of school, and socioeconomic status. There is a distinct lack of longitudinal evidence on whether students’ academic self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking are related to each other over time at school. Using data from two national longitudinal studies, the hypothesis of reciprocal relations was tested with one latent autoregressive model with cross-lags in Study 1 and Study 2.

Overview of the present research and hypothesis

Festinger (1954, p. 118) assumed “To the extent that objective, non-social means are not available, people evaluate their opinions and abilities by comparison respectively with the opinions and abilities of others.“In accordance with previous research from other schools of thought (e.g., Bosacki 2000; Dimaggio et al. 2008), the aim of Study 1 was to analyze whether relations exist between students’ academic self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking over 4 years of school (controlling for gender, school grades in math class and literature class, ethnic background, type of school, and socioeconomic status).

Further findings (e.g., Wolgast and Barnes-Holmes 2018) suggest relations between self-concept in reading and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking assessed using Davis’ (1980) subscale. The cognitive representation of one’s reading abilities is self-concept in reading. The aim of Study 2 was therefore to determine whether reciprocal relations exist between students’ self-concept in reading and their beliefs about their own perspective-taking over 1 year of school, analogously to how Study 1 assessed relations over 4 years of school. Like Study 1, this aim was based on Festinger’s (1954) second hypothesis. In addition to the covariates considered in Study 1, available standardized assessments were included as covariates in Study 2: fluid reasoning (Weiß 2006), receptive vocabulary (Weiß 2006), and reading fluency (Auer et al. 2011).

The following hypotheses were tested in Studies 1 and 2: (1) The more students think about their academic abilities in comparison to the abilities of their classmates, the more they will believe that they engage in perspective taking, trying to understand their peers in conflict situations as well. (2) Reciprocal relationships between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking will exist after the transition to secondary school, and will fade thereafter.

Taken into account in all statistical analyses were the previously introduced relevant covariates, namely gender, school achievement, ethnic background, type of school, and socioeconomic status in both Study 1 and Study 2, and fluid reasoning, receptive vocabulary and reading fluency in Study 2. Fluid reasoning and reading ability scores were only available in the data used in Study 2. Gender differences have been found (e.g., Jacobs et al. 2002, Van der Graaff et al. 2014) in both types of beliefs about one’s own abilities, and this was taken into consideration by separately analyzing girls’ and boys’ data simultaneously in one structural equation model. As described below, in both studies, the measures were administered in schools over three measurement points (Times 1–3): in Study 1 over the course of 4 years, and in Study 2 over one school year. These longitudinal student samples (Study 1 and Study 2) stemmed from assessments administered in secondary schools as part of two large-scale studies with ethics approval (scientific use file; for a full sampling description, see Fischer and Klieme 2013; Study on the Development of All-day Schools—StEG 2013).

The first large-scale study aimed to provide an overview of all-day schools in Germany (broader scope). At Time 1, the sample was representative of German all-day schools. The second large-scale study aimed to provide insights into reading and social learning at all-day schools. The target schools were those with a focus on reading and social learning and were drawn from the first study’s representative school sample. Some school-level data from the first large-scale study were used in the analyses of the second study’s data and were not again asked. Therefore, Study 1 provides a broader view on academic self-concept and belief about one’s own perspective-taking over a longer period of time than Study 2.

Methods

Data

In Study 1, analyses were based on a sample consisting of N = 4428 students (n = 2100 female, n = 35 missing values for the variable gender; Mage = 10.90, SDage = .60, one non-response the variable age) from 392 classes at 211 schools. Students’ ethnic background was recorded as migration when at least one parent or the student him- or herself had been born outside Germany (this is in line with the criterion used in the Programme for International Student Assessment; Adams and Wu 2002). This was true of n = 1471 students (18 non-responses). Thus, the sample includes students with heterogeneous ethnic backgrounds.

After elementary school, students in Germany are assigned to different types of schools predominantly on the basis of their achievement in elementary school. In this sample, n = 1145 (26% of N = 4428) students attended an academically-oriented secondary school (Gymnasium), while the other n = 3283 (74%) students attended a secondary school with a more vocational curriculum.

Students answered questions about their family in order to assess socioeconomic status according to the Highest International Socio-Economic Index of Occupational Status (HISEI, Ganzeboom et al. 1992). Socioeconomic status was defined according to the highest status obtained by a member of the student’s immediate family, based on educational level and occupation. This international standard classification of occupations ranges from highly-ranked “physical scientists, technical and related workers” (e.g., “aircraft and ships officers” or medical professionals) to low-ranked “manual workers” (e.g., “street sweeper” or “road construction worker”; Ganzeboom et al. 1992, pp. 33–55). Students’ family socioeconomic status varied by 47 points (16 = minimum, 90 = maximum). Values up to 33 HISEI points made up the lowest quartile, and 23% of students fell within that range. Values over 55 HISEI points made up the highest quartile, and 24% of students fell within that range.

In Study 2, the sample consisted of N = 2105 fifth-grade students (n = 973 female, Mage = 10.37, SDage = .61, 24 non-responses for the variable age) from k = 127 classes at 66 schools. In this sample, n = 637 students attended a Gymnasium; the other n = 1468 students attended secondary schools with a more vocational curriculum. Information on students’ ethnic background (Adams and Wu 2002) and socioeconomic status (Ganzeboom et al. 1992) was gathered at the end of the study via a computerized questionnaire. An ethnic background other than German was reported by n = 762 (36%) students. Type of school and the socioeconomic status of a student’s family were measured using HISEI points as described above.

Procedures

Students participated in Study 1 or 2 with the consent of their parents, the students themselves, their school principals, and the education ministries of the federal states. In Study 1, students filled out a questionnaire including the same measures of academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking at school at the beginning of fifth grade (Time 1), seventh grade (Time 2) and ninth grade (Time 3).

In Study 2, students’ receptive vocabulary knowledge (Weiß 2006) and reading fluency (Auer et al. 2011) were tested using computer-based test versions at Time 1; afterwards, the students filled out a computer-based questionnaire. One laptop per student was provided to conduct the assessments; for example, 20 laptops were provided a class contained 20 students. All students completed the same measures at three measurement points: at the beginning (Time 1), middle (Time 2), and end (Time 3) of fifth grade. The summer break began after Time 3.

Measures

Academic self-concept (Study 1)

Students’ academic self-concept was measured with four items adapted from a standardized assessment (see Quellenberg 2009, for details). Each item was assessed on a 4-point scale (from 1 = not true at all to 4 = absolutely true). The items captured students’ general academic self-concept. The question was worded as follows:

Do the following statements apply to you? (a) I learn things quickly. (b) I usually know the answer to a question earlier than the others. (c) I get good grades without effort. (d) It is easy for me to solve difficult tasks.

In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was α = .75 at Time 1, α = .79 at Time 2, and α = .83 at Time 3, thus demonstrating acceptable internal consistency. Academic self-concept at Time 3 served as the outcome variable for testing the hypothesis of reciprocal relations between academic self-concept and perspective-taking over time.

Academic self-concept in reading (Study 2)

Four items were used to measure academic self-concept in reading (see Fischer et al. 2017, for details), with students’ responses recorded on scales ranging from 1 = not true at all to 4 = absolutely true. Students answered the following question:

What about reading in class? (a) I can comprehend difficult texts. (b) It is easy for me to comprehend new texts. (c) I can handle long texts. (d) If I want to understand a difficult text, I will succeed here too.

In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was α = .81 at Time 1, α = .78 at Time 2, and α = .83 at Time 3, thus demonstrating satisfactory internal consistency. The intraclass correlation was low, meaning that values for students in the same class did not tend to be more similar than those of students in different classes (intraclass correlation ICC = .04). Academic self-concept in reading at Time 3 served as the outcome variable for testing the hypothesis of relations between academic self-concept in reading and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking over time.

Beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking (Studies 1 and 2)

Davis (1980) proposed items to measure beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking as a disposition. Four items from Davis’ perspective-taking scale were available in this study (Davis 1980; Quellenberg 2009). Students responded to each of the four items using the same scenario—“Imagine yourself in your school”—to encourage them to think about how they usually act in social situations at school. Students’ beliefs about their own social perspective-taking were assessed using their responses to the following question:

What do you do? (a) I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective; (b) I believe that there are two sides to every question and try to look at them both; (c) Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine what I would think if I were in their place; or (d) I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement before I make a decision.

Students ranked their responses on a 4-point scale (from 1 = not true at all to 4 = absolutely true). Thus, scoring high on the beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking-abilities measure indicates that a person tends “to anticipate the behavior and reaction of others” (Davis 1983, p. 115). The items seem “to reflect an ability or proclivity to shift perspectives—to step ‘outside the self’—when dealing with other people” (Davis 1980, p. 11f.) with regard to friends (Item 1), perspective shifting (Item 2) and in conflict situations (Items 3 and 4). The construct validity and reliability of these items have been tested in several studies (e.g., Davis 1980, 1983; Davis et al. 1996). Furthermore, this measure has been used in other studies that also found support for the construct validity and reliability (e.g., Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015; Study on the Development of All-day Schools—StEG 2013; Van der Graaff et al. 2014). Cronbach’s alpha was in Study 1: α = .74 at Time 1, α = .78 at Time 2, α = .78 at Time 3, and in Study 2: α = .84 at Time 1, α = .81 at Time 2, and α = .86 at Time 3. Thus, the scale demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency in both studies.

Beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking at Time 3 served as the outcome variable for testing relations between academic self-concept and this belief over time in Studies 1 and 2. Overall, in Study 1, missing values for academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking across the three measurement points ranged from 0 to 7% (see Quellenberg 2009, for means, standard deviations, and number of missing values by variable). In Study 2, missing values for academic self-concept in reading and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking across the three measurement points ranged from 0 to 10%. (see Fischer et al. 2017, for means, standard deviations, and number of missing values by variable).

Covariates in studies 1 and 2

Students’ school achievement assessed in math class and literature class ranged from high achievement, coded as 1, to low achievement, coded as 6, at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. Students whose ethnic background was not German were coded as 1; ethnically German students were coded as 0. Type of school and the socioeconomic status of a student’s family were measured using HISEI points as described above.

Additional covariates in Study 2

Students’ school achievement assessed in literature class ranged from high achievement, coded as 1, to low achievement, coded as 6, at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. Previous research suggests correlations between beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking and general cognitive abilities (e.g., Schonert-Reichl et al. 2015) as well as verbal abilities (e.g., Gabay et al. 2016). In the current study, general cognitive abilities were assessed via processing capacity tasks (fluid reasoning via matrices; Weiß 2006) and reading fluency via the Salzburger Lese-Screening 5–8 (Auer et al. 2011). Students read syntactically and grammatically simple sentences as quickly as possible and responded to questions pertaining to the sentences (Auer et al. 2011). Further standardized tasks assessed comprehension of general and advanced German vocabulary (Weiß 2006). The test scores obtained from each of the used tests were scaled using Rasch modeling in accordance with item-response theory, with one difficulty parameter assigned to each test item within a test (one-parameter logistic test model constructed with the ltm package, Rizopoulos 2006). Table 1 contains product-moment correlation coefficients at Time 1, which demonstrate a correlation between vocabulary knowledge and reading fluency.

Table 1 Correlations between academic self-concept, perspective-taking, school achievement, cognitive abilities, and socio-economic status at time 1

Statistical analyses

In Studies 1 and 2, structural equation modeling (SEM) was constructed with the R package lavaan and weighted least squares with mean and variance adjustment estimation (WLSMV; Rosseel 2012). A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) included the latent factor academic self-concept measured by the respective four items and their residuals at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3, and the latent factor perspective-taking measured by the respective four items and their residuals over the same three measurement points (2 × 4 × 3 items; see Additional file 1 for the statistical model; Pornprasertmanit et al. 2014). Measurement invariance across time, gender, and type of school was tested using this two-factor CFA model. In Study 1, this CFA model including constrained factor loadings suggested configural invariance (\(\Delta\) CFI = .011; \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .001; see Additional file 2 for further fit indices and an overview of tested models) across 4 years according to recommended cutoffs for larger group sizes (Rutkowski and Svetina 2014). Thus, the factor structure found in the data from the students in fifth grade was equivalent to the factor structure found in the data from the same students 4 years later in ninth grade.

The two-factor CFA model including constrained factor loadings and intercepts for multi-group analyses suggested scalar invariance across gender (\(\Delta\) CFI = .005, \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .001, see Additional file 2 for further fit indices of the tested models). Students’ gender was coded as 0 = girls and 1 = boys. The factor structure and intercepts found for the girls’ data were equivalent to the factor structure and intercepts found for the boys’ data at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3.

The CFA model was additionally tested across types of school. Constrained factor loadings and intercepts at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3 suggested scalar invariance across types of school (\(\Delta\) CFI = .005, \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .001, see Additional file 2 for tested models). School types with a more vocational curriculum were coded as 0; Gymnasium was coded as 1. The factor structure and intercepts found in the data from schools with a vocational curriculum were equivalent to the factor structure and intercepts found in the data from schools with an academic-oriented curriculum. This CFA resulted in χ2 (237) = 762.276, fit indices: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .027, CI [.025, .029], comparative fit index (CFI) = .983, standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) = .031. These indices indicated that the postulated structure fit the data.

Statistical relations were specified in one structural equation model including latent academic self-concept measured by four items as indicators with four residuals each, as well as latent perspective-taking measured by four items as indicators with four residuals each at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. The model is depicted in Fig. 1. Students reported their school grades from elementary school as indicators of their school achievement (e.g., Karbach et al. 2013) at Time 1. Therefore, school grades in math class and in literature class at Time 2 were included as covariates of academic self-concept and perspective-taking only at Time 2 and Time 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study 1: Model scheme of the structure equation model with cross-lags outlined over the measurement points T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2 and T3 = Time 3. Girls’ (before the slash)/boys’ (after the slash). ASC = academic self-concept (Quellenberg 2009) and BPT = belief about one’s own perspective-taking (Davis 1980) were specified as latent variables (indicators and residuals are not depicted, see the Additional file 1 for the statistical model). Covariates were included as manifest variables, i.e., migration = ethnic background (out of Germany coded as 1, German background coded as 0), SES = socio-economic status, type of school, school grades in literature class, and school grades in math class. See Table 2 for the path coefficients and standard errors. *p < .05

Particularly in tests of reciprocal effects, it is critical that all variables are represented as latent variables with appropriate controls for typically complex measurement error. This is particularly important in cases where internal consistence is not high, when it is not reasonable to assume that the constructs are measured without error. The predictor variables ethnic background, type of school, and family socioeconomic status were therefore specified on both latent factors at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3 (i.e., all measurement points as described by Pekrun et al. 2017).

Auto-regressive paths were specified from latent academic self-concept at Time 1 to Time 2, and from Time 2, in turn, to latent academic self-concept at Time 3, as well as from latent perspective-taking at Time 1 to Time 2, and in turn, from Time 2 to latent perspective-taking at Time 3. Cross-lagged regression paths were specified between these two latent factors from Time 1 to Time 2 and from Time 2 to Time 3 (see Figs. 1 and 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Study 2: Model scheme of the structure equation model with cross-lags outlined over the measurement points T1 = Time 1, T2 = Time 2 and T3 = Time 3. Girls’ (before the slash)/boys’ (after the slash) ASC = academic self-concept (Fischer et al. 2017 and BPT = belief about one’s own perspective-taking (Davis 1980) were specified as latent variables (indicators and residuals are not depicted). Relevant factors were included as manifest variables, i.e., fluid reasoning and receptive vocabulary (Weiß 2006), reading fluency (Auer et al. 2011), migration = ethnic background (out of Germany coded as 1, German background coded as 0), SES = socio-economic status, type of school, and school grades in literature class. *p < .05

Table 2 Study 1 and Study 2: relations between academic self-concept and belief about one’s own perspective-taking over time

Grand mean centering was applied and random intercepts were allowed. The current study’s research question analyzes the relevance of an individual’s academic self-concept for that individual’s beliefs about his or her own perspective-taking. Therefore, the nested data structure of classes at schools was considered at the individual level via corrections for nested data (similar to TYPE = COMPLEX in Mplus; see Oberski (2014), for details). The model specification considered girls’ and boys’ data separately, i.e., girls’ data were analyzed without boys’ data and vice versa. Outcome variables were latent academic self-concept and latent perspective-taking at Time 3.

Study 2

Analogously to Study 1, measurement invariance was tested using the data from Study 2. The CFA suggested (factor loadings constrained, \(\Delta\) CFI = .008, \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .004) configural invariance over time, (factor loadings and intercepts constrained, \(\Delta\) CFI = .002, \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .002) scalar invariance across gender, and (factor loadings and intercepts constrained, \(\Delta\) CFI = .002, \(\Delta\) RMSEA = .001) scalar invariance across type of school. The CFA indicated that the postulated structure fit the data: χ2 (237) = 801.870, fit indices: RMSEA = .042, CI [.038, .045], CFI = .959, SRMR = .028.

Relations were specified in a manner analogously to Study 1: The structural equation model used in Study 1 was adapted to include academic self-concept in reading and perspective-taking at Time 1, Time 2 and Time 3 in Study 2. The model is depicted in Fig. 2. Seven predictor variables were included in the structural equation modeling: school grades in literature class, fluid reasoning measured by matrices, receptive vocabulary (Weiß 2006), and reading fluency (Auer et al. 2011), ethnic background, type of school, and socioeconomic status. The model was constructed with the R package lavaan using WLSMV estimation (Rosseel 2012, 2016). The nested data structure of classes at schools was again considered at the individual level (Oberski 2014).

Results

Results of the product-moment correlations are depicted in Table 1. In Study 1, the variables academic self-concept and perspective-taking were correlated significantly with each other at Time 1 (see Table 1). This correlation replicates previous findings from transversal studies using different methods (Bosacki 2000). The results from the structural equation modeling suggested a good fit between the assumed and real data structure: χ2 (660) = 1916.681, CFI = .958, RMSEA = .037, CI [.036, .039], SRMR = .042. Figure 1 shows the model including the covariates (math grades, literature grades, ethnic background, type of school, socioeconomic status) and the latent factors (without indicators and residuals for reasons of space) and all paths between two factors. Path coefficients and standard errors of the structural equations are depicted in Table 1.

The relations over 4 years of school, controlling for the covariates, were as follows: High academic self-concept levels among boys and girls at Time 1 in fifth grade significantly predicted high levels of perspective-taking in seventh grade (Time 2) but not from Time 2 to ninth grade (Time 3). High academic self-concept levels among boys in seventh grade (Time 2) did not predict high perspective-taking levels in ninth grade (Time 3; see Fig. 1).

High perspective-taking levels among girls in fifth grade (Time 1) did not significantly predict high academic self-concept levels in seventh grade (Time 2), nor was an association found from seventh to ninth grade (Time 2 to Time 3). High perspective-taking levels among boys in fifth grade (Time 1) significantly predicted high academic self-concept levels in seventh grade (Time 2), but not from seventh to ninth grade (Time 3; see Fig. 1). This structural equation model explained 51% of the variance in girls’ latent academic self-concept, 37% of the variance in boys’ latent academic self-concept, 20% of the variance in girls’ latent perspective-taking and 17% of the variance in boys’ latent perspective-taking in ninth grade (Time 3). The covariates contributed to the outcome variables academic self-concept and perspective-taking at Time 3 as follows: Girls with high socioeconomic backgrounds in fifth grade tended to exhibit higher academic-self-concept levels 4 years later than those with low socioeconomic backgrounds. Girls with high socioeconomic backgrounds and girls at a Gymnasium in fifth grade (Time 1) reported that they were more likely to consider others’ social perspectives 4 years later (perspective-taking at Time 3) than those with low socioeconomic backgrounds and those who attended a vocational-oriented school type (see Table 2).

In Study 2, the variables academic self-concept in reading and perspective-taking were significantly correlated with one another at Time 1 (see Table 1). This correlation replicated the correlations found in the study by Bosacki (2000) and in the present Study 1. The results of the structural equation modeling indicated a good fit between the assumed and real data structure, χ2 (726) = 1604.434; CFI = .955, RMSEA = .034, CI [.032, .036], SRMR = .038. Figure 2 shows the model including the manifestly measured covariates (school grades in literature class, fluid reasoning, receptive vocabulary, reading fluency, ethnic background, type of school, socioeconomic status), the measured latent factors (depicted without indicators and residuals for reasons of space), and all paths between two factors. Path coefficients and standard errors of the structural equations are given in Table 2.

The significantly relations over 1 year of schooling, controlling for the covariates, were as follows: Girls’ and boys’ self-concept in reading at the beginning of fifth grade (Time 1) was positively related to perspective-taking half a year later at Time 2 (see Table 2 and Fig. 2).

Social perspective-taking levels among girls and boys at the beginning of fifth grade (Time 1) were positively related to self-concept in reading half a year later at Time 2. This association could be only found among girls from Time 2 to the end of fifth grade half a year later (Time 3). No relations were found between boys’ self-concept in reading and perspective-taking from Time 2 to the end of fifth grade (Time 3; see Table 2 and Fig. 2). This model explained 35% of the variance in girls’ self-concept in reading, 50% of the variance in boys’ self-concept in reading, 33% of the variance in girls’ perspective-taking and 31% of the variance in boys’ perspective-taking at Time 3.

The covariates contributed to the outcome variables academic self-concept and perspective-taking at Time 3 as follows: Male low achievers in literature class (at Time 2) self-reported that they were more likely to consider others’ perspectives half a year later at the end of fifth grade (perspective-taking at Time 3) than high achievers in literature class. Receptive vocabulary at the beginning of fifth grade (Time 1) contributed to self-concept in reading at the end of fifth grade (Time 3) in both girls and boys (see Table 2). Girls and boys with high receptive vocabulary scores at the beginning of fifth grade (Time 1) demonstrated high academic-self-concept levels at the end of fifth grade (Time 2) even when controlling for reading fluency and school grades in literature class. Boys with good school grades in literature class at Time 2 reported that they were more likely to consider others’ social perspectives 5 months later at the end of fifth grade (perspective-taking at Time 3) than those with worse school grades in literature class (see Table 2).

Discussion

The aim of the present two studies was to analyze longitudinal relations between self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking among girls and boys following the transition to secondary school. In secondary school, for the first time, “social means” of comparing students’ abilities rather than “objective means” (Festinger 1954, p. 118ff.) are available as an external frame of reference.

Previous research from other schools of thought suggested positive (e.g., Bosacki 2000) and reciprocal (e.g., Dimaggio et al. 2008) relations between children’s cognitive representations of themselves and their cognitive representations of others. The first hypothesis was (1) the more students think about their academic abilities in comparison to the abilities of their classmates, the more they will believe that they engage in perspective taking, trying to understand their peers in conflict situations as well. The positive relations between students’ self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking support Hypothesis 1 and are in line with previous results, even though Bosacki (2000) used other measures (the self-perception scale by Harter 1985, and a storytelling task).

The results of the current Studies 1 and 2 suggest a substantial degree of rank order instability, particularly in students’ beliefs about their own perspective-taking. The auto-regressive parameters for this belief contradict the hypothesis of a stable disposition (or trait) over time. As expected in Hypothesis (2), reciprocal relationships between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking exist after the transition to secondary school, but fade thereafter, with the relations fading over the course of time. Bosacki (2014) found a similar fade-out in relations between self-perceptions and understanding others’ minds in children 8–10 years of age in a longitudinal study with two measurement points.

Two possible explanations can be identified for changes in the relationship between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking over time. First, the faded relations over two (Bosacki 2014) or four (current Study 1) years of school imply social adjustments, as Festinger (1954) described in his social comparison theory. When students have “objective means” (Festinger 1954, p. 118ff.) of comparing their own abilities and others’ abilities, and they agree with or reject the opinions of other students in class, their academic self-concept will be related to their school achievement. Beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking might be less necessary in this case. As Festinger (1954, p. 120) postulated, “The tendency to compare oneself with some other specific person decreases as the difference between his[/her] opinion or ability and one’s own increases.”

In accordance with Hypothesis II of Festinger’s social comparison theory, students’ academic self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking were linked immediately following the transition to secondary school, but not later. Thus, the fifth-grade students in the two present studies might have used perspective-taking to make adjustments after transitioning to a new school context. This adjustment might be reflected in part in the reciprocal relations between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking from Time 1 to Time 2 found in boys (Study 1) and girls (Study 2).

A second explanatory approach is that an increasing number of tasks and time pressure at school might constrain perspective-taking in a similar manner as in experimental studies (e.g., Chambers and Davis 2012). If this is the case, the relationship between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking would change in line with the current results from Study 1 and Study 2.

In addition, students experience physiological changes from late childhood in fifth grade to adolescence from seventh to ninth grade (e.g., Sebastian et al. 2008). Adolescence is known as a time of focusing on oneself rather than considering others (e.g., Sebastian et al. 2008), and might represent a possible underlying developmental process behind the changed relationship between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking. The reciprocal relations between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking over time found in these studies extend previous research (e.g., Bosacki 2000).

Study 2 allowed us to take a more specific look at the relations between girls’ and boys’ academic self-concept in reading and beliefs about their own perspective-taking across the fifth grade. The results of Study 2 replicated the transversal relationship between academic self-concept and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking at Time 1 following the transition to secondary school described in Study 1. Reciprocal relations between self-concept in reading and beliefs about one’s own perspective-taking were only found in girls over the first half year of fifth grade (Time 1 to Time 2). Self-concept in reading contributed to boys’ beliefs about their own social perspective-taking (Time 1 to Time 2) but not vice versa. The results are in line with Festinger’s second hypothesis, and suggest that girls and boys adjusted to their peers, and likely learned to deal with diverse conflict situations and to differentiate between academic abilities and social or other abilities over the course of the school year. Social comparisons were also less likely in Study 2 as soon as objective means (such as peers’ school grades) were available as an external frame of reference.

Limitations and practical implications

One limitation of Study 1 is that it measured general academic self-concept instead of a domain-specific academic self-concept. This limitation as well as the fact that academic achievement was measured using school grades resulted from the use of already-available scientific data in Study 1. Academic self-concept is a multidimensional construct (e.g., Davis 1980). It would probably exhibit the highest relationships to the corresponding achievement domain, as the results of Study 2 indicated. Similarly, domain-specific beliefs about one’s own social perspective-taking related to peer-assisted learning in literature class might also exhibit higher relationships with self-concept in reading than were found in Study 2.

In Study 1, school achievement based on school grades was not measured on a common metric. Due to the grading-on-a-curve effect, a given grade at Gymnasium is representative of a higher level of achievement than the same grade at another type of school. In contrast, in Study 2, both school grades (which lacked a common metric) and test scores (representing a common metric) were included in the analyses (see Fig. 2) because both school grades and test scores are indicators of cognitive abilities, but have different implications. School grades represent success at school, which reflects both cognitive abilities and further characteristics of students (e.g., gender, Voyer and Voyer 2014; or obesity MacCann and Roberts 2013), whereas test scores represent students’ cognitive abilities at one point of time.

A confounding variable might be social desirability, the tendency of students to answer in a manner that will be viewed favorably by their teachers. Social desirability was a concern for the self-report measures used in Study 1 and Study 2, and would be reflected in a similar correlation pattern over Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. However, the correlation pattern changed, as boys’ academic self-concept barely correlated with their beliefs about their own perspective-taking between Time 2 and Time 3.

The results of these studies have practical relevance for schools, for example, with respect to peer-assisted learning and dealing with conflict situations. Whether or not children solve tasks jointly with peers, and whether or not students help or criticize one another, children need perspective-taking to understand each other when working on tasks at school. Students interact with each other and adapt to their peers, relatives and teachers. Understanding others’ reactions or opinions by assuming that these persons have their own reasons resulting from their past experiences is a social resource. Attempts to understand another person’s social situation are related with low levels of stereotyping or prejudice (Davis et al. 1996). Students engage in mental representations (mentalizing) with both academic self-concept and perspective-taking (e.g., O`Brien and Albrecht 1992). Frequently deploying mental models in social situations might not only contribute to knowledge about oneself, consideration of others and dealing with conflict situations. Such mental models might also help students understand historical events (e.g., Hartmann and Hasselhorn 2008). Furthermore, it would be worthwhile to investigate whether students apply perspective-taking during in-class activities such as peer learning because children and adolescents need interpersonal understanding to adopt task-related and other thoughts about each other.

Conclusion for future research

The main findings of the two studies presented here indicate changes in the relationship between students’ academic self-concept and beliefs about their own perspective-taking over time. The fragile relationship between these two constructs suggests that students understand social and academic concerns and demands in different ways following the transition to secondary school. According to the internal–external frame-of-reference model (e.g., Möller et al. 2009), internal dimensional comparisons between beliefs about one’s own academic vs. social abilities (e.g., academic self-concept vs. social perspective-taking) could be assessed using adapted measures.

Perspective-taking items measure coordination between oneself and other people in social interactions and conflict situations. In the future, such relations could be analyzed using domain-specific social perspective-taking items (e.g. fictional characters described in mathematics word problems or historical tasks, e.g., Hartmann and Hasselhorn 2008). Perspective-taking is essential for effective communication, dealing with conflict situations and fostering social adjustments (e.g., Nelson et al. 2017). The current two studies contribute to our understanding of longitudinal relations between academic and social abilities at school following the transition to secondary school.

References

Download references

Authors’ contributions

The author read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author declares no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

The data are available as scientific use files on the website of The Institute for Educational Quality Improvement (https://www.iqb.hu-berlin.de/fdz/studies/StEG).

Funding

The longitudinal data used in the current studies were collected for two national studies (StEG; StEG-S: 01GTS0311A and B) funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anett Wolgast.

Additional files

Additional file 1: Figure S1.

Statistical model used in confirmatory analyses in Study 1 and Study 2 (s1, s2, and s3 = academic self-concept at Time 1, Time 2, and Time3; p1, p2, and p3 = perspective-taking at Time 1, Time 2, and Time3).

Additional file 2: Table S1.

Results of invariance tests in Study 1 and Study 2.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wolgast, A. School students’ beliefs about abilities and perspective-taking over time. Large-scale Assess Educ 6, 12 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40536-018-0065-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40536-018-0065-y

Keywords