Direct verbal suggestibility: Measurement and significance

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2020.103036Get rights and content

Abstract

Hypnotic suggestibility is part of the wider psychological trait of direct verbal suggestibility (DVS). Historically, DVS in hypnosis has informed theories of consciousness and of conversion disorder. More recently it has served as a research tool in cognitive science and in cognitive neuroscience in particular. Here we consider DVS as a general trait, its relation to other psychological characteristics and abilities, and to the origin and treatment of clinical conditions. We then outline the distribution of DVS in the population, its measurement, relationship to other forms of suggestibility, placebo responsiveness, personal characteristics, gender, neurological processes and other factors, such as expectancy. There is currently no scale specifically designed to measure DVS outside a hypnotic context. The most commonly used and well-researched of the hypnosis-based scales, the Harvard Group Scale, is described and identified as a basis for a more broadly based measure of DVS for use in psychological research.

Section snippets

Introduction.

Though suggestibility is a broady distributed human propensity, suggestion has received relatively little recent attention in the psychological literature (Halligan & Oakley, 2014). This has not always been the case and many early writers and researchers on psychology such as Binet, 1900, Sidis, 1898, and more recently, McDougall, 1908, Hull, 1929, Hull, 1933, Sherif, 1935, Asch, 1956 placed great emphasis on the importance of ‘normal’ suggestion and suggestibility (see Lundh, 1998 for an

Measuring hypnotic suggestibility using the Harvard Group scale

Presentation of the HGSHS:A testing procedure is traditionally in the form of a pre-recorded audio-file. It commences with a general introduction, establishing rapport, offering reassurance and introducing the participants to the nature of ‘hypnosis’ and ‘suggestion’. This is followed by the hypnosis induction procedure and scripted, standardized test suggestions before hypnosis is terminated.

There are 12 test items in the HGSHS:A, consisting of four ‘Motor’ suggestions (where an involuntary

Suggestibilty and its measurement more generally

A defining feature of the many scales of suggestibility, typically used in hypnotic contexts, is that test items (suggestions) are measures of DVS in that they are directly and verbally presented after a hypnotic induction procedure and are capable of producing a range of behavioural and psychological effects ideally experienced by the participants as involuntary. In relation to the latter, Weitzenhoffer (1974) introduced the term “classical suggestion effect” to identify the non-voluntary

Relevance of DVS for cognitive and clinical psychology

DVS is relevant to views of consciousness in that it exerts a powerful effect on how we as individuals experience the world and how that experience can be influenced by verbal interaction. The phenomena produced by direct verbal suggestion, particularly in the context of hypnosis, have a long history of informing psychological theories of ‘consciousness’ and in particular the role of ‘non-conscious’ processes (Cardeña, 2000, Cardeña, 2014, Ellenberger, 1994, Hebb, 1980, Hilgard, 1977, James,

Direct verbal suggestibility a trait

As noted above, individuals vary in their ability to respond to direct verbal suggestions and DVS as a trait is also a relevant factor in responsiveness to illusions of bodily awareness such as the rubber hand illusion (Walsh, Guilmette, et al., 2015), creativity (Shames & Bowers, 1992), outcomes in somatic medicine (Kekecs & Varga, 2013) and empathy (Wickramasekera & Szlyk, 2003). Individuals scoring highly on scales of direct verbal suggestibility are also more prone to confusing imagined

Characteristics of DVS

Direct verbal suggestibility (DVS) seems to be a distinct capacity and there is good evidence, primarily from the hypnosis literature, that it is not correlated with other forms of suggestibility such as placebo suggestibility and interrogative suggestibility (Kihlstrom, 2008, Tasso and Pérez, 2008, Tasso et al., 2020). Despite almost a century of experimental effort, however, DVS has not been shown to correlate with any of the major personality dimensions, such as extraversion/introversion (

Neuroscientific accounts of DVS

There is an active and growing body of research exploring the role of frontal areas of the brain in direct verbal suggestibility, particularly in hypnotic contexts (Parris, 2017). A number of theories implicate reduced frontal cortical executive function (or ‘hypofrontality’ - Dietrich, 2003), especially in the left hemisphere, in the propensity for suggestibility in general and hypnotic suggestibility in particular (Gruzelier, 1998, Gruzelier, 2006, Kihlstrom, 2013, Woody and Bowers, 1994).

Contextual influences on DVS

Though DVS with and without the addition of a hypnosis induction procedure are considered here as situationally-defined variants of the same trait, it is important to note that context, such as that supplied by a hypnotic induction, may influence the degree of responsiveness shown to direct verbal suggestions. In particular, expectation of outcome is a well-researched situational variable that affects responsiveness to direct verbal suggestions (Silva & Kirsch, 1992) and contributes to the

Measuring direct verbal suggestibility with the HGSHS:A and other scales

Barnier and McConkey (2004) in their review identify 13 ‘hypnotizability scales’ devised since the 1950′s that share the common feature of measuring direct verbal suggestibility. These scales involve between 1 and 12 test items (verbal suggestions) that are preceded in ten of them by a hypnotic induction procedure as standard. The induction procedure is optional in one of the other three scales and not used in the remaining two. Induction typically consists of a set of verbal instructions

Varieties of DVS and their relevance for cognitive science

As noted above, the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility: Form A (HGSHS:A) is the most commonly used DVS scale and is commonly considered as the ‘gold standard’(Benham et al., 2002, Council, 1999). It is formed of three subscales, which measure respectively responsiveness to ideomotor, motor challenge and cognitive suggestions (see below and Section 2 above). Aside from practical research considerations these three subscales effectively each measures a different variety of DVS that is

National norms and the structure of the Harvard Group scale

Normative data have been collected for the HGSHS:A since the early 1960s from 18 national, large-sample studies (13 from European countries, and one each from UK, America, Australia, Canada and China). The data overall show that the scores are normally distributed with low numbers of participants falling into the lowest and highest levels of responsiveness and consistency in the relative difficulty of each of the 12 test items. [See Oakley et al. (2020), for an overview and discussion of all 18

Imagination, instruction, suggestion and the ‘classic suggestion effect’

As noted earlier one problem with the HGSHS:A is the potential for confounding ‘imagination’ and the effects of direct suggestion that exists within the traditional wording of suggestions embedded in induction and testing procedures. We are proposing that the experiences and responses produced by DVS are not the same as those that result simply from the instruction to imagine or the spontaneous engagement in imaginative processes. Supporting this view is evidence that reported changes in

Measuring DVS in cognitive science for ‘instrumental’ research

As indicated above, a reliable measure of suggestibility is relevant to cognitive science and cognitive neuroscience research as a means of identifying highly suggestible participants for studies where suggestion can be used as a tool to create phenomena of interest, particularly where the study involves neuroimaging techniques (For overviews of this type of research see Halligan and Oakley, 2013, Oakley and Halligan, 2009, Oakley and Halligan, 2013). A pragmatic first step in this direction

Measuring DVS in cognitive science for ‘intrinsic’ research

One benefit of an increased interest in DVS within cognitive science generally – would be to raise awareness of the extensive relevant hypnosis literature and the various well-established hypnosis journals (e.g. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis. Australian Journal of Clinical Hypnosis; Contemporary Hypnosis, Contemporary Hypnosis and Integrative Therapy, and the International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis). ‘Normalising’ hypnotic suggestibility itself within the DVS

Discussion

Suggestion following a hypnotic induction procedure has begun to make a significant contribution to cognitive science generally, and cognitive neuroscience in particular. It is also the case that an individual’s score on scales involving a hypnotic induction procedure followed by direct verbal suggestions correlates well with their score on the same scales when the hypnotic induction is omitted. On this basis, we have proposed the term Direct Verbal Suggestibilty (DVS) for the trait that is

References (202)

  • A. Dietrich

    Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness. The transient hypofrontality hypothesis

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2003)
  • T. Egner et al.

    Hypnosis decouples cognitive control from conflict monitoring processes of the frontal lobe

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • B. Gandhi et al.

    Does ‘hypnosis’ by any other name smell as sweet? The efficacy of ‘hypnotic’ inductions depends on the label ‘hypnosis’

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2005)
  • L.H. Goldstein et al.

    Dissociation, hypnotizability, coping styles and health locus of control: Characteristics of pseudoseizure patients

    Seizure

    (2000)
  • G.H. Gudjonsson et al.

    The relationship between confabulation and intellectual ability, memory, interrogative suggestibility and acquiescence

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (1995)
  • P. Haggard et al.

    Anomalous control: When ‘free will’ is not conscious

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2004)
  • P.W. Halligan et al.

    Imaging hypnotic paralysis: Implications for conversion hysteria

    The Lancet

    (2000)
  • P.W. Halligan et al.

    Hypnosis and cognitive neuroscience: Bridging the gap

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • E.A. Holmes et al.

    Are there two qualitatively distinct forms of dissociation? A review and some clinical implications

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2005)
  • C.S. Humpston et al.

    The relationship between different types of dissociation and psychosis-like experiences in a non-clinical sample

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2016)
  • C. Iani et al.

    Attention control and susceptibility to hypnosis

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2009)
  • J.F. Kihlstrom

    Neuro-hypnotism: Prospects for hypnosis and neuroscience

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • J.F. Kihlstrom et al.

    Hypnosis in the right hemisphere

    Cortex

    (2013)
  • H. Anlló et al.

    French norms for the Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (2017)
  • M.B. Arnold

    On the mechanism of suggestion and hypnosis

    Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology

    (1946)
  • S.E. Asch

    Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of one against a unanimous majority

    Psychological Monographs

    (1956)
  • S.L. Baker et al.

    Hypnotic and placebo analgesia: Order effects and the placebo label

    Contemporary Hypnosis

    (1993)
  • E.L. Baker et al.

    Psychoanalytic approaches to clinical hypnosis

  • C.G. Balthazard et al.

    The ‘stuff’ of hypnotic performance: A review of psychometric approaches

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1985)
  • T.X. Barber et al.

    Toward a theory of hypnotic behaviour. Effects on suggestibility defining the situation as hypnosis and defining response to suggestions as easy

    Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology

    (1964)
  • T.X. Barber et al.

    Empirical evidence for a theory of ‘‘hypnotic behaviour: Effects on suggestibility of five variables typically included in hypnotic induction procedures

    Journal of Consulting Psychology

    (1965)
  • A.J. Barnier et al.

    Autobiographical remembering and forgetting: What can hypnosis tell us?

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (1999)
  • Barnier, A. J. & McConkey, K. M. (2004) Defining and identifying the highly hypnotizable person. In M. Heap, R. J....
  • Baudouin, C. (1920) Suggestion and autosuggestion: A psychological and pedagogical study based upon the investigations...
  • V. Bell et al.

    Dissociation in hysteria and hypnosis: Evidence from cognitive neuroscience

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • G. Benham et al.

    Hypnotic susceptibility scales: Are the mean scores increasing?

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (2002)
  • A. Binet

    La Suggestibilité

    (1900)
  • K.S. Bowers

    The Waterloo-Stanford Group C (WSGC) scale of hypnotic susceptibility: Normative and comparative data

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (1993)
  • P.G. Bowers

    Understanding reports of non-volition

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1986)
  • P. Bowers et al.

    The experience of hypnotic suggestions

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis

    (1988)
  • W. Braffman et al.

    Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability: An empirical analysis

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • R.A. Bryant et al.

    Eliciting autobiographical pseudomemories: The relevance of hypnosis, hypnotizability and attributions

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.

    (1999)
  • R.A. Bryant et al.

    Functional blindness: A construction of cognitive and social influences

    Cognitive Neuropsychiatry

    (1999)
  • E. Cardeña

    Hypnosis in the treatment of trauma. A promising, but not fully supported, efficacious treatment

    International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis.

    (2000)
  • E. Cardeña

    Hypnos and psyche: How hypnosis has contributed to the study of consciousness

    Psychology of Consciousness

    (2014)
  • E. Chevreul

    Lettre à M. Ampère, sur une classe particulière de mouvements musculaires

    Revue des Deux Mondes

    (1833)
  • R. Cohen Kadosh et al.

    Induced cross-modal synaesthetic experience without abnormal neuronal connections

    Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • H.M. Connors et al.

    Hypnotic analogues of delusions: The role of delusion proneness and schizotypy

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2014)
  • J.R. Council

    Measures of hypnotic responding

  • J.R. Council et al.

    Expectancy versus absorption in the prediction of hypnotic responding

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • Cited by (25)

    • Absorption and dissociation mediate the relationship between direct verbal suggestibility and impulsivity/compulsivity

      2022, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      Direct verbal suggestibility refers to a unique capacity of individuals to experience pronounced changes in cognition, behaviour, and perception in response to verbal descriptions (suggestions; Oakley et al., 2021).

    • Increased persuadability and credulity in people with corpus callosum dysgenesis

      2022, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is consistent with parallel work in field of hypnosis research, regarding the trait of ‘suggestibility’, a phenomenon consistent with gullibility, especially ‘secondary suggestibility’ (Eysenck & Furneaux, 1945). Suggestibility has been found to share little to no relationship with canonical personality dimensions defined in Big 5 or OCEAN, such as agreeableness and neuroticism (Gudjonsson, 1983; Liebman et al., 2002; Norris, 1973; Pires et al., 2013), but is considered an independent cognitive facet (Pires et al., 2013), and has demonstrated a high degree of stability, persisting over a period of 25 years in the same group of individuals (Piccione et al., 1989) Participant responsiveness to direct verbal suggestions (i.e., ‘Direct Verbal Suggestibility’, or DVS; Oakley et al., 2020; Oakley et al., 2021; Oakley & Halligan, 2017), with and without hypnosis, was outside the scope of this work and so it remains unclear whether exaggerated persuadability and insensitivity to social trickery in our CCD samples will translate into DVS. Rigorously exploring the possibility that CCD relates to both gullibility (second order suggestibility) and DVS in the future works may highlight an important role of the corpus callosum in this separable social trait, and work toward a clearer phenotype of the diagnosis.

    • Hypnotic suggestibility in dissociative and related disorders: A meta-analysis

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Hypnotic suggestibility, as assessed by standardized behavioural scales (Acunzo and Terhune, 2021; Woody and Barnier, 2008), refers to a stable capacity to experience involuntary responses to direct verbal suggestions in the context of hypnosis (e.g., analgesia: “you will not feel anything at all in your arm.” ; (Oakley et al., 2021; Polczyk, 2016)). Responses to suggestions among highly suggestible individuals are characterized by a pronounced reduction in the experience of authorship over one’s actions and experience (Polito et al., 2014; Weitzenhoffer, 1974) and bear close phenomenological resemblance to distortions in the sense of agency in clinical populations (Polito et al., 2015).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    Joint first authors.

    View full text