Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 15 January 2021
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.609467

The Brain Emotional Systems in Addictions: From Attachment to Dominance/Submission Systems

  • 1Department of Human Neuroscience, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy

Human development has become particularly complex during the evolution. In this complexity, adolescence is an extremely important developmental stage. Adolescence is characterized by biological and social changes that create the prerequisites to psychopathological problems, including both substance and non-substance addictive behaviors. Central to the dynamics of the biological changes during adolescence are the synergy between sexual and neurophysiological development, which activates the motivational/emotional systems of Dominance/Submission. The latter are characterized by the interaction between the sexual hormones, the dopaminergic system and the stress axis (HPA). The maturation of these motivational/emotional systems requires the integration with the phylogenetically more recent Attachment/CARE Systems, which primarily have governed the subject’s relationships until puberty. The integration of these systems is particularly complex in the human species, due to the evolution of the process of competition related to sexual selection: from a simple fight between two individuals (of the same genus and species) to a struggle for the acquisition of a position in rank and the competition between groups. The latter is an important evolutionary acquisition and believed to be the variable that has most contributed to enhancing the capacity for cooperation in the human species. The interaction between competition and cooperation, and between competition and attachment, characterizes the entire human relational and emotional structure and the unending work of integration to which the BrainMind is involved. The beginning of the integration of the aforementioned motivational/emotional systems is currently identified in the prepubertal period, during the juvenile stage, with the development of the Adrenarche—the so-called Adrenal Puberty. This latter stage is characterized by a low rate of release of androgens, the hormones released by the adrenal cortex, which activate the same behaviors as those observed in the PLAY system. The Adrenarche and the PLAY system are biological and functional prerequisites of adolescence, a period devoted to learning the difficult task of integrating the phylogenetically ancient Dominance/Submission Systems with the newer Attachment/CARE Systems. These systems accompany very different adaptive goals which can easily give rise to mutual conflict and can in turn make the balance of the BrainMind precarious and vulnerable to mental suffering.

Introduction

Researches on human development (Boughner and Rolian, 2016) have progressively highlighted how the latter has become particularly complex during the evolution, compared to the development of other animal species. In this complexity, adolescence is an extremely important developmental stage, because of all changes occurring in the human BrainMind (Panksepp, 2011). Adolescence in humankind is characterized by multiple aspects—as will be discussed through this article—the most impressive of which is a significant restructuring of the brain (Arain et al., 2013; Walker et al., 2017) within a new hormonal context (Wierenga et al., 2018).

Studies of brain restructuring during adolescence have shown the emerging of the dual systems model (Pfeifer and Allen, 2012; Gladwin and Figner, 2014; Shulman et al., 2016), in which the limbic system—the seat of emotional functioning and reward—develops before the prefrontal cortex, involved in cognitive and regulatory processes (Padmanabhan et al., 2011; Casey et al., 2016). This model that traced back to adolescence the etiology of risk-seeking behaviors, the vulnerability to social frustrations and therefore the development of psychopathology, partially highlights the problem (Crone and Dahl, 2012). Highlighting the poorer regulation of cortical structures on the subcortical ones during adolescence, the dual systems model has contributed to paying less attention to the subcortical component. The latter has been relatively little studied as a set of motivational/emotional systems, biologically predetermined and expressing peculiar, endogenous intentionality. Affective Neuroscience (Panksepp, 1998) have contributed in the last two decades to reduce this imbalance, significantly bringing attention back to instinctual systems. These systems with peculiar neuronal and hormonal characteristics organize the adaptive behavior of mammals, including the human species, towards the material and relational environment.

What are these instinctual/emotional systems at the basis of mental and relational life? In Affective Neuroscience, Panksepp (1998), highlights the presence in mammals of several motivational/emotional systems responsible for regulating the interactions between conspecifics. Those are the SEEKING1 system that corresponds largely to the mesolimbic dopaminergic (ML DA) system, as will be discussed below; the LUST system that regulates sexuality, the PANIC/GRIEF system that regulates the interaction between caregiver and offspring and works together with the CARE system. In this article, we are going to use Attachment/CARE (Bowlby, 1969) to indicate the complex PANIC/GRIEF and CARE systems of Panksepp. Then there is the PLAY system that regulates the social interactions both in puppies and children and facilitates learning related to competitive interactions. Competitive interactions between sexually mature conspecifics are regulated by the inter-male aggression2 (Panksepp and Zellner, 2004; Kroes et al., 2006), also referred to as the urge for social dominance (Panksepp, 1998; Panksepp and Biven, 2012), Dominance system (van der Westhuizen and Solms, 2015) or agonistic behavior (Scott and Fredericson, 1951). The urge for social dominance is considered a “complex” secondary system made up of three primary emotional systems: “Contributing factors (to social dominance n.d.r.) include SEEKING and RAGE, as well as FEAR, and surely early experience with the rough-and-tumble PLAY system are involved as well.” (Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 169). Even if the RAGE system can be activated during the inter-male fighting, it is believed that the urge for social dominance has to be distinguished from that system (Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 169). The Dominance system “neurogeographically” includes the high density of testosterone receptors running from the medial amygdala, through the preoptic, anterior hypothalamic area, and down to the brainstem PAG (periaqueductal gray; Panksepp, 1998; Panksepp and Biven, 2012).

The urge for social dominance is in complementary interaction with the primary emotional system of FEAR, which activates submissive behaviors aimed at inhibiting aggression in the rival. It is, therefore, more appropriate to speak of a complex motivational/emotional system of Dominance/Submission (Giacolini and Sabatello, 2019); an exhaustive description of its functioning will be given later. The Dominance/Submission system—found in all vertebrates starting from fishes and reptiles up to mammals—motivates the acquisition of territorial, food and sexual resources (Lorenz, 1963; MacLean, 1990) through which Sexual selection takes place (Darwin, 1871).

The SEEKING system, the LUST system, the FEAR system and the RAGE system—present in all vertebrates—are the archaic motivational/emotional systems, while the PANIC/GRIEF system, the CARE system and the PLAY system—present only among mammals and some species of birds—are phylogenetically the most recent ones.

These motivational/emotional systems can be considered from the Life history theory’s perspective (Del Giudice et al., 2015; Knowles et al., 2019), significant articulations of the appearance of the developmental stage in various species. Research on adolescence, within the Life history theory and Affective Neuroscience point of view, can provide useful indications for the understanding and treatment of psychopathology, which makes its appearance in an elective way in this period of life and therefore also of the addicted behavior. It is precisely the emergence of psychopathology on the human development scene that can find an explanatory and treatment model, considering it as an effect of the peculiar features of adolescence—a human-specific evolutionary stage (Bogin, 1994, 1997, 1999; Bogin and Smith, 1996; Robson and Wood, 2008; Hochberg, 2009; Hochberg and Belsky, 2013)—characterized by the restructuring of interactions between the subcortical primary motivational/emotional systems. As we shall see, adolescence constitutes a switch from the phylogenetically more recent motivational/emotional systems of Attachment and CARE (ontogenetically the first ones activated) to the phylogenetically more archaic ones of sexuality (LUST) and Dominance/Submission, which introduce the subject into the dynamics of Sexual selection (Darwin, 1871). Until puberty, the interactions between the subject in developmental age and adults are primarily regulated by the motivational/emotional systems of Attachment/CARE, although behaviors of Dominance/Submission are observed among puppies and children (Strayer and Strayer, 1978; Hawley, 1999, 2002; Hawley and Little, 1999; Hawley et al., 2002; Pellegrini and Long, 2002; Pellegrini et al., 2007). With sexual maturation, priority goes not only to the motivational/emotional system of sexuality (LUST) but simultaneously to that of Dominance/Submission. The human BrainMind (Panksepp and Biven, 2012) is thus subjected to the need to integrate motivational/emotional systems, each characterized by specific neurophysiological and hormonal characteristics such as to determine intentional and relational objectives that can enter into mutual conflict. Adolescence is a developmental stage in which the work of integrating these motivational/emotional systems becomes a priority. This work can expose the subject to the difficulty of integrating these systems, due to the transfer of attachment bonds from caregivers to sexual partners (Cassidy and Shaver, 2016). In adolescence, these attachment bonds no longer derive from the biological predisposition—active at birth—within the temporal windows of imprinting (Lorenz, 1937), but they are the result of both Sexual selection and the Dominance/Submission system. This new dynamic among primary and secondary emotional systems can create a predisposition to psychopathology, including the most age-specific types of addiction, as will be described in this article.

The urge for social dominance is present in both male and female children, as evidenced by the studies on Adrenarche (Maninger et al., 2009), the transspecies studies on the PLAY system (Panksepp et al., 1984, 1985) and the researches on developmental psychology (Hawley et al., 2002). In males, the Dominance system is characterized by a major coercive social dominance strategy while in females by a major prosocial dominance strategy (Hawley et al., 2002). With sexual maturity, the Dominance/Submission system is first of all characterized by the production of gonadal testosterone, produced in an extremely greater quantity in males than in females, in which the hormonal system is significantly characterized by estrogens and menstrual dynamics. In adolescence, the Dominance/Submission system acquires centrality in the regulation of social relationships, in line with the theory of Sexual selection (Darwin, 1871), exposing individuals to the consequences of the social rank hierarchy dynamics (Price et al., 2007), which can be conceptualized as a continuum between a richer relational world for dominant subjects and a poorer one for subordinate subjects (Nader and Czoty, 2005; Nader et al., 2012b; also see the interesting article on The Matthew Effect by Sloman and Dunham, 2004). With adolescence, the creation of relational bonds is now largely mediated by the secondary emotional system of Dominance/Submission, such as the conquest of a partner, of friends, of adults consideration, even if each relational bond will be simultaneously regulated by emotions of the Attachment/CARE systems. As we shall see, social stress from both losses in social encounters named Social defeat and from social loss, such as the loss of a loved one both share some same neurohormonal mechanisms (Panksepp et al., 2002, p. 111). Social defeat stress, which is activated when the subject is defeated during social interaction and involves the decrease or loss of social bonds, determines the release of CFR (corticotropin-releasing factor) which activates both the FEAR/Submission system and the PANIC/GRIEF systems (Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 340). The intra-sexual competition and the Social defeat stress exposes the subject to neurophysiological effects that can predispose not only to depression but also to addictions (Zellner et al., 2011). In this article, the interaction between the sexual hormones, the stress axis (HPA) and the dopaminergic system is considered above all in relation to males, both for a lower complexity of the neurohormonal systems involved than for females and for a lack of transspecies studies that consider the interactions between intra-sexual competition, Social defeat stress and addiction in female individuals (Hawley et al., 2008; Nader et al., 2012a).

ML DA-Seeking System in Motivated (Addicted) Behaviors

Investigations on the neurobiological processes involved in the establishment of addictive behaviors and habits indicate a primary role of the mesolimbic dopaminergic (ML DA) system and associated areas and circuits (Wise and Bozarth, 1987; Di Chiara and Imperato, 1988; White, 1996; Alcaro et al., 2007). Indeed, the activation of this system provides the effect of reward and influences the reinforcement-learning processes, conditioning the incentive value attributed to the stimuli and the behaviors in both classical and operant conditioning (Robinson and Berridge, 2001, 2003, 2008; Salamone and Correa, 2002; Everitt and Robbins, 2005). The ML DA system thus plays a fundamental role in the acquisition of compulsive habits as well as in the continuous reactivation of such habits by conditioned stimuli.

However, from a neuro-ethological perspective which focuses on the instinctive and unconditioned factors that also influence learning (Lorenz, 1965; Panksepp, 1998), the ML DA system is considered to be the neurobiological substratum of the SEEKING system. This system is believed to have an intrinsic psycho-behavioral function that has evolved to motivate organisms to explore and to look for any kind of stimulus necessary for survival and reproduction (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Alcaro et al., 2007; Alcaro and Panksepp, 2011). Such emotional disposition constitutes the instinctive basis of all motivated behaviors during the appetitive phase of exploration and searches for distal stimuli and it is distinct from the second phase of consumption of a proximal stimulus (Wise and Bozarth, 1987; Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Salamone and Correa, 2012).

Expanding on the perspective of Affective Neuroscience (Panksepp, 1998; Alcaro and Panksepp, 2014), we proposed that addiction is generally characterized by a narrowing and tightening of the SEEKING disposition around certain compulsive memories that result in it being activated exclusively in specific contexts and channeled only through specific sequences of procedural activities. This may be due to some form of control exerted by stored memories in the superior cortical and limbic structures that interact with the activity of the subcortical centers and in particular of the ML DA-SEEKING system (Alcaro et al., 2007). Indeed, neurobiological research on animal dependency models has shown that the formation of compulsive habits is accompanied by a gradual functional reorganization of the brain from the molecular level to the system-circuit level (Nestler, 2002, 2014). At the neurochemical level, for example, a strengthening of glutamatergic synaptic connections has been observed in some nodal centers of cortico-striatal circuits (Pennartz et al., 2009), which would give rise to the well-known phenomenon of sensitization through which dependent memory complexes acquire a disproportionate power of activation of the ML DA-SEEKING system (see Steketee and Kalivas, 2011 for a summary on the subject).

In other words, there is a weakening of the ability of the ML DA-SEEKING system to express itself independently of the link with dynamic representations closed within the superior procephalic circuits and formed following repeated experiences of conditioning. Such loss of functional autonomy of the ML DA-SEEKING system constitutes, therefore, a common root for all forms of addiction or dependence, despite each having its basis in the action of specific memory complexes (Alcaro et al., 2007; Alcaro and Panksepp, 2011; Alcaro, 2019).

ML DA-Seeking System in Adolescence

Although the evidence suggests that the loss of functional autonomy of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system is promoted and enhanced by specific experiences that progressively induce addiction, human and animal studies of dependence models have identified some neurobiological factors that can predispose an individual to develop compulsive behaviors. One of the most prominent of these factors has proved to be an endogenous predisposition to the dysregulation of the functioning of the ML DA-SEEKING system (George et al., 1995; Grace, 2000; Marinelli and White, 2000; Chefer et al., 2003; Alcaro et al., 2007; Alcaro and Panksepp, 2011). Such dysregulation causes the system to be hyper-reactive to particular environmental conditions characterized by the presence of novelty or artificial reinforcements such as substances of abuse (Piazza et al., 1989; Pierre and Vezina, 1997). Individuals characterized by this hyper-reactivity have been classified as “novelty seekers” or “sensation seekers” since they show strong attraction towards novel environments, sensorial stimulation, and high-risk settings (Bardo et al., 1996).

Interestingly, adolescents also show heightened orientation towards rewards in the environment including preferences for novelty, increased interest in risky situations, and other forms of emotional and sensorial stimulation (Wahlstrom et al., 2010). Following these observations, fMRI studies have shown that some neural systems innervated by the ML DA-SEEKING system are significantly more active in adolescents than children or adults when receiving primary rewards (e.g., sweet liquid; Galván and McGlennen, 2013), secondary rewards (e.g., money; Ernst et al., 2005; Galván et al., 2006; Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010), or social rewards (Guyer et al., 2006; Chein et al., 2011), as well as in the presence of appetitive social cues (Somerville et al., 2011). Such spikes in activity are associated with compromised cognitive control (Somerville et al., 2011) and increased self-reported risk-taking (Galván, 2014).

Accordingly, it has been hypothesized that the ML DA-SEEKING system is at a functional ceiling during adolescence (Chambers et al., 2003), as evidenced by overall higher tonic DA levels, peaks in DA cell firing, greater DA innervation, and increased DA receptor densities (see Padmanabhan et al., 2011; Padmanabhan and Luna, 2014). Therefore, the ML DA-SEEKING system is thought to be in a state of “overdrive” during adolescence, which appears to have important functional significance for behavioral outcomes and increases susceptibility to the onset of addiction and many other psychiatric disorders.

However, although the prevailing view is that heightened activity of the ML DA-SEEKING system represents a liability that orients adolescents towards risky behaviors and results in compromised well-being (e.g., Chambers et al., 2003; Casey et al., 2008) It has also been proposed that increases in risk-taking behaviors may also be adaptive for promoting survival and skill acquisition (Spear, 2000; see below).

Indeed, the overdrive of the ML DA-SEEKING system can be linked to the developmental phase of sexual maturation (see below) and to the explosion of hormonal and libidinal energy resulting in the search for contexts and situations in which it can be adequately satisfied (for a neuro-psychoanalytic point of view see Solms and Turnbull, 2002). Sexual drive (LUST system) is one of the main biological motivators capable of promoting and facilitating the activation of the SEEKING system (Panksepp and Biven, 2012). Moreover, sexual maturation affected during adolescence leads to a transition from infancy to adulthood, from the familiar environment to a complex and multiform social world (as we will see in detail below). From an Affective Neuroscience perspective, such change is also characterized by the transition from an emotional base cantered on the secureness of attachment (CARE disposition, PANIC disposition) and the joy of PLAY towards an emotional base cantered on the lust for sex and the competitive activity towards conspecifics (DOMINANCE, RAGE)3, although the urge for social dominance is also present among the puppies and children (see above).

In such a context, the tendency to explore and take risks may provide an adaptive function that affords a unique opportunity for adolescents to attain new experiences at a time when they are primed to learn from their environments and leave the safety of their caregivers (Spear, 2011). Therefore, we believe that the development of sexuality and the correlated ML DA-SEEKING, along with the competitive drive within the sexual selection, overdrive constitute the main vectors that lead the adolescent to abandon the safe haven and bonds of parental attachment and to embark on the difficult waters of an open and complex sea made up of uncertain, problematic and dangerous relationships and new unpredictable and uncertain contexts and situations. This journey is the necessary prerequisite for reaching adulthood with the consequent relocation of one’s individual identity from the family matrix to that of the shared social world, in which adolescents can find positive opportunities by exploring environments more suited to their genotype than those offered to their family matrix (Scarr and McCartney, 1983). In the next paragraphs, we will see this journey both from a phylogenetic and ontogenetic point of view.

Although adolescence is generally characterized by an ML DA-SEEKING overdrive, individual vulnerability to develop an addiction may be more specifically related to an increased electric responsivity of ML DA neurons to glutamatergic and other excitatory input (Marinelli and White, 2000; Alcaro et al., 2007). Such hyper-reactivity of the SEEKING system may be also related to a deficit in the tonic DA transmission, that is in the levels of DA molecules that extend outside the synaptic space and change slowly because they are relatively independent of nerve impulses (Grace, 2000; Chefer et al., 2003). Indeed, as a result of inhibitory regulatory processes on type D2 autoreceptors, the tonic levels of DA inhibit the readiness for the electrical discharge of DA neurons (Grace, 2000; Schmitz et al., 2003) and then tend to limit the tendency to search for environmental conditions characterized by novelty or artificial reinforcements such as drugs of abuse (Piazza et al., 1989; Pierre and Vezina, 1997). Therefore, the increased risk in adolescence may be related not only to an unspecific DA overload but instead to an enhanced ratio between the electric reactivity of ML DA neurons and the levels of tonic DA and D2 autoreceptors (Alcaro et al., 2007; Alcaro and Panksepp, 2011).

Sexual Reproduction (Lust) and Competition

Sexual maturation determines in all vertebrate species, particularly mammals, the ability to reproduce and activates at the same time a process called Sexual selection by Darwin (1871).

Sexual reproduction also involves the drive to mate that is at the same time inextricably connected to the drive to compete with the individuals of the same species and gender (usually males) to gain access to the sexual reward.

This classic theory of motivational/emotional systems connected to the dynamics of Sexual selection has been expanded in recent years by studies that show that the urge for social dominance—is a strategy for Sexual selection, which is in itself a source of reward (Chester, 2017). Attacking a conspecific to impose one’s desire and dominance is an expression not only of the urge for social dominance (van der Westhuizen and Solms, 2015) but also a source of specific gratification (Chester, 2017).

Inter-male aggression in Agonistic behavior (Scott and Fredericson, 1951), highlights the complementary behavioral characteristics in combatants, such as agonistic engagement characterized by manifestations of aggressive threats and pacification, submission, avoidance behaviors in the defeated contender (see below). Unlike predatory aggression, intraspecific contests would be rarely fatal (see for a critic to this classic ethological observation Natarajan and Caramaschi, 2000), except in the human species where the evolution of the brain would have determined a specific use of aggression within the paradigm of Sexual selection (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1984). The hypothesis that aggression, within the paradigm of Sexual selection, may derive from predatory aggression—as we shall see below—seems to be borne out by the positive effect it evokes in the individual (Chester, 2017), in contrast to defensive aggression (see note 2) that generates a negative effect (Panksepp, 1998; Panksepp and Biven, 2012). Research by Affective Neuroscience also seems to indicate similar conclusions because: “[…] the predatory aggression is the manifestation of the SEEKING urge.” (Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 165) and: “[…] the inter-male aggression that leads to dominance hierarchies seems to be an expression of the SEEKING system than of the RAGE system.” (Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 168).

The positive effect associated with inter-male aggression is evident in the so-called PLAY system (Panksepp, 1998; Burgdorf et al., 2008; Graham and Burghardt, 2010; Panksepp and Biven, 2012), where both males and females playing tendencies are quite comparable (Panksepp, 1998, p. 230–281), even if some researchers highlighted that male infants tend to display more agonistic-like behaviors, while female infants display more social behaviors and behaviors reminiscent of mating (Graham and Burghardt, 2010).

The inter-male aggression related to positive affective experience has traditionally been linked to the pleasure of revenge, as retaliation for the violence suffered (Chester and DeWall, 2016). Neuroimaging studies have shown, during vengeful aggressive behavior, both the activation of areas of the ventral striatum—part of the ML DA system assigned to the reward—and the reduced activation of the prefrontal lateral cortex connected to its regulation (Chester and DeWall, 2016; Chester, 2017; Chester et al., 2019).

The activation of the dopaminergic system connected to aggression, however, is determined not only in aggression by retaliation but also in the absence of an avenging motivation such as proactive aggression or “appetitive aggression” (Carver and Harmon-Jones, 2009; Weierstall and Elbert, 2011; Hecker et al., 2012).

This aspect has been identified as being present in the so-called sensation-seeking personalities in which the pleasure determined by the elevation of the dopaminergic arousal is positively connected to experiencing a strong aggressive activation as an expression of predatory and inter-male aggression (Miller et al., 2012; Chester et al., 2016; Chester, 2017). The significant involvement of the ML DA-SEEKING system in the dynamics of inter-male aggression is evidenced by studies that have highlighted characteristics specific to addiction dynamics (Golden and Shaham, 2018). Animals that have experienced dominant aggression on a conspecific tend to prefer places where this experience has occurred and to repeat these behaviors on subordinate conspecifics (Legrand, 2013; Chester, 2017). Chester writes: “This suggests that aggression is intrinsically reinforcingChester (2017). It is necessary to add and underline an element which is not entirely evident in the studies cited, that this “appetitive aggression”: an expression of that particular aggressive motivational-emotional system which—as explained above—developed according to the Sexual selection (Lindenfors and Tullberg, 2011) and which is called inter-male Agonistic behavior (Scott and Fredericson, 1951), or Dominance emotional system (van der Westhuizen and Solms, 2015). To this latter denomination, we prefer the Dominance/Submission motivational/emotional system (Giacolini and Sabatello, 2019), because in our opinion it explicitly highlights the complex aspect of this inter-male aggressive system, which has the FEAR system as a complementary motivational system (Fish et al., 2005). In any species, the competitive contest does not involve the physical elimination of the contender (see above), as it happens in predatory aggression. This peculiarity was made possible by the function of the FEAR system within the competition, whose manifestations (of the FEAR system) in all species have the power to inhibit aggression in the victorious contender, preventing the elimination of the overwhelmed adversary (see van der Dennen, 2005 for review).

In social species, the FEAR system has given rise to the so-called Submission or Pacification behaviors, whose signaling is the basis for the formation of rank structures, allowing the coexistence of dominant and submissive individuals. These behaviors have also been called Yielding subroutines or Involuntary Defeat Strategy (Sloman and Gilbert, 2000; Sloman, 2002) to highlight the dimension of being forced or compelled present in the individual to maintain the rank structure. Such behaviors are considered analogous to those observed in humans manifesting depression (Gilbert, 1992, 2006).

On the other hand, dominant individuals manifest—in an equally compulsive way within the rank structure—Dominance subroutines or Involuntary Dominant Strategy (Sloman, 2002): the expression of the aggressive component of the motivational/emotional system of Dominance/Submission that has the function of reaffirming and verifying the supremacy of dominant individuals over subordinates. Dominance subroutines or Involuntary Dominant Strategy are considered analogous to the behaviors manifested along the manic spectrum (Sloman, 2002; Malatynska and Knapp, 2005; Johnson and Carver, 2012; Logan and McClung, 2016; Harrison et al., 2018) that are also characterized by the compulsive and unconscious dimension of these emotional states.

It can be assumed that these behaviors function to keep the hierarchies relatively stable and therefore the social structure of conspecific groups, thus favoring their synergy. If inter-male aggression—as expressed in Dominance subroutines—is a source of reward and potentially of addiction (Golden and Shaham, 2018), the FEAR system can likewise cause the activation of the dopaminergic system, opening the way to compulsive and addictive behaviors expressed as Yielding subroutines or Involuntary Defeat Strategy. It is now well established that the ML DA-SEEKING is activated not only in the presence of stimuli with a high reward content but also in the presence of adverse stimuli (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999; Alcaro et al., 2007). In the latter case, the ML DA-SEEKING appears to drive a search for “safety” (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999).

Consequently, the depletion of the dopaminergic system ML DA-SEEKING in the depressive state (Panksepp and Watt, 2011) occurs at the same time as its activation. This contradiction has recently been explained by discovering that the dopaminergic system is not homogeneous, but it is composed of various subsystems. These studies have highlighted how the ML DA system is formed of multiple subtypes of DA neurons with particular axonal projections and inputs, distinct anatomical, molecular and electrophysiological characteristics (Ikemoto, 2007; Lammel et al., 2012, 2014).

It appears that some DA subsystems are activated in the presence of reward stimuli, while others in the presence of stimuli with salience, whether positive or negative, but all subsystems transmit an alerting signal (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010).

The ML DA-SEEKING System thus appears to be a powerful mechanism that can push the individual towards rewarding behaviors, such as aggression for dominance, but can also keep active a depressive emotional dimension connected to a subjective “belief of defeat,” which activates automatic protective submission behaviors to maintain a state of “safety.”

It should be noted, that the depression considered here is not the one resulting from separation (see the next paragraph) and therefore from the activation of the attachment system or PANIC/GRIEF system and the diminished brain reward/ML DA-SEEKING (Panksepp and Watt, 2011), but rather the effect of the dynamics of the Dominance/Submission motivational/emotional system. However, as will be highlighted later, the neurohormonal dynamic typical of separation will be “secondarily” activated in the defeated subject of the social competition.

Depression, as an emotional manifestation caused by the activation of the FEAR/Submission system (Gilbert, 1992, 2006) and by Social defeat (see below), will provide a means to achieve “safety” from the threatening aggression of a dominant and therefore from the source of a negative effect (see about the activation of VTA DA neurons in response to reward or aversive stimuli: Tanimoto et al., 2004; Brischoux et al., 2009; Lammel et al., 2014).

From Vertebrates to Mammals, Towards Homo Sapiens

To understand the problem of addiction, a complex model that involves the interaction of the numerous biopsychosocial variables is required (Griffiths and Larkin, 2004; Lewis, 2005; Griffiths, 2008; Ratliff et al., 2016). Similarly, adolescence—as a topic of both research and mental care—involves considering the interaction between multiple factors of varying nature. During this developmental phase of a subject’s life, many factors are at work including the development of the soma, the maturation of various body parts, in particular the sexual and cerebral apparatus. Along with this biological development, the adolescence stage is characterized by the cultural dimension that is ritualized in multiform ways that lead to entry into adulthood (Kaplan and Garner, 2017; Kunnen et al., 2019).

The mind is the epiphenomenon of the functioning of the cerebral organ, which has evolved over millions of years in its effort to adapt to the environment, first natural and then relational (Alexander, 1990), constituting a complex BrainMind system (Cacioppo et al., 2014; Panksepp, 2014).

Adolescence is known to constitute a new phylogenetic developmental stage, added along with childhood, during the evolution of the genus Homo. This has allowed a better articulation of the stages already present in more evolved mammals, namely infancy, juvenile, and sexual maturity. One of the main causes of these stages of appearance, which have temporally extended the individual growth, is to be found in the massive increase of brain mass that characterizes the human species (Robson and Wood, 2008). The appearance of the adolescence growth stage, which extends the stage of puberty to a relatively long period, is assumed to be related to the further increase in the offspring survival rate. Thanks to the help given by young adolescents in raising siblings during childhood, the mother was allowed to engage in new pregnancies, contributing to the population growth (Bogin and Smith, 1996). Alongside this aspect connected to the aid in the rearing of offspring, elective for young adolescent females, there is another even more significant one: the learning function allowed by this new developmental stage, namely the learning of relational modalities in the social context, which were becoming more and more complex (Hochberg and Konner, 2020), and of the technological resources that were developing (Robson and Wood, 2008). Among the relational learning peculiar to the human species, a prominent place concerns the intra-gender competition that is inter-male aggression, elective in young adolescent males, and typical of Sexual selection. However, intra-sexual competition in the human species has been significantly complexed, implying not only the dimension of the conflict between individuals (as among other animal species), but also and above all the dimension concerning competition between groups (Van Vugt et al., 2007). The latter seems to be a peculiarity of the human species, partially shared by other evolved primates such as chimpanzees and orangutans (Wilson and Wrangham, 2003). Competition between groups indicates a complex regulation of the RAGE and FEAR motivational/emotional systems (Lebel, 2017), which has allowed the ability to tolerate proximity between conspecifics of the same sex, partially inhibiting the aggressive and distancing tendency. This has allowed cooperation to conquer the territorial resources, food, and sexual relations of another group of conspecifics. And precisely the ability to cooperate—specific to the human species (Puurtinen and Mappes, 2009)–would have found its origin and enhancement from intra-sexual group competition, as claimed by the Male Warriors Hypothesis (Van Vugt et al., 2007; McDonald et al., 2012). According to this theory, cooperation would be the effect of the male individual’s discovery that joining in intra-sexual competition determines a greater probability of victory, as well as survival both in obtaining food and in defending themselves from predators. The competition between groups would have selected a particular tendency to be afraid and attack those who do not belong to their group and to have instead positive effects towards members of their group—a propensity identified even among the highly evolved primates (Mahajan et al., 2011). Competition between groups (elective among males) would have contributed to determining the propensity of these males to remain within the group to which they belong at the period of mating, while the females, on the contrary, to migrate towards the husband’s group (Knipper et al., 2017). This greater propensity of males to feel part of a belonging group and to start a conflict with conspecifics of the outgroup is significantly correlated with testosterone production (Reimers and Diekhof, 2015; Muñoz-Reyes et al., 2020), which is extremely higher in males than in females. “[…] humans, particularly men, may possess psychological mechanisms enabling them to form coalitions capable of planning, initiating and executing acts of aggression on members of outgroups (with the ultimate goal of acquiring or protecting reproductive resources)” (McDonald et al., 2012, p. 671). Competition between groups, therefore, highlights the evolution of intra-sexual fighting that requires the individual to be able to combine the motivation for intra-sexual aggression and the fear of the opponent with the ability to create ties of proximity, to build functional alliances for struggling. This ability to compete through the group is an acquisition that seems to have been favored precisely by the onset of a long period of adolescence. The initiation rites (which took place during the period of adolescence; Alexander and Norbeck, 2011), sanctioned in the so-called primitive populations the transition from the indistinct identity of a child to that of an adult man. In particular, those related to the acquisition and learning of warrior skills, as well as tolerance and danger management. The rites of passage (Abeliovich, 2018) are, therefore, a window on the modeling of the primary and secondary motivational/emotional systems during adolescence, each characterized by a peculiar intentional organization that guides the behavior of single individuals (Montag and Panksepp, 2017). Cooperating to compete is an evolutionary acquisition of the great apes, whereby as in chimpanzees, cooperative behavior can be seen in group hunting, territorial defense, and alliances during male power struggles (Suchak et al., 2016; Massen et al., 2019). In adolescence, cooperating to compete in the new status makes the young adolescent feel inserted in a network of bonds with other peers and, at the same time, makes him perceive the ability to compete effectively. The emergence of psychopathology in adolescence, including substance use or addictive behaviors, highlights the adolescent’s dramatic belief that he is unable to combine the competitive drive with the cooperative drive and to transfer the attachment bonds into a new sexual relationship (Cassidy and Shaver, 2016). Cooperating to compete therefore implies the ability to tolerate the inevitable experience of Social defeat, implicit in the structuring of rank within the human group. The latter is an experience that makes the adolescent subject particularly vulnerable to mental pain, as the experience of Attachment-Care systems that had primarily organized his relational and mental life is still present in him.

Paraphrasing the zoologist Richard D. Alexander, the Environment of Evolutionary Adaptation—EEA (Bowlby, 1969) of Homo sapiens is no longer the material one but the relational one (Alexander, 1990). Being able to integrate motivational/emotional systems, characterized by purposes, intentionality, and therefore by extremely different BrainMind states, is the peculiar work of the adolescent period that makes the young subject vulnerable to psychopathology and addiction.

The Affective Neuroscience have (Panksepp, 1998) highlighted how the instinctual-emotional heritage is itself the primary form of consciousness through which human intentionality is expressed (Solms, 2019). Mental functioning cannot be understood, therefore, as a simple learning function, nor consciousness as a simple function of the corticalization of the human species. Based on research with animals (Panksepp, 1998) and humans (Panksepp, 2014; Solms, 2019), Affective Neuroscience has highlighted how the instinctual-emotional heritage is itself the primary form of awareness through which human intentionality is expressed.

This approach leads to questions about the phylogenetic heritage that led to the emergence of human psychic functioning and from this perspective the motivational/emotional systems are of particular importance. The latter are phylogenetically homologous in mammals and in the human species and they are wired into the subcortical structures between the brainstem and the limbic system, which are currently considered the roots from which the cortical, autonoetic and reflexive consciousness has evolved in a “bottom-up” and “top-down” mutual interaction (Panksepp and Solms, 2012; Solms, 2018).

Attachment, Infancy, and Mental Pain

In this article, attention will be paid to the interaction between the “two complexes” motivational/emotional systems crucial for understanding adolescence and the dynamics of addictions: the Attachment/CARE systems and the Dominance/Submission systems (Giacolini and Sabatello, 2019).

The well-documented Attachment system (Bowlby, 1969) forms the basis of the child’s or offspring’s well-being when close to a reference adult or caregiver and involves a mental state that is largely determined by the release of endogenous opioids on mu receptors when offspring and caregivers are in proximity (Insel and Young, 2001).

The removal of the caregiver causes a depletion of these opioids at the receptor level and the implementation of dynorphin production on k receptors (Shippenberg et al., 2007), both of which cause a sense of discomfort and alarm in the offspring (Panksepp, 1998). This malaise occurs not only in infancy but can manifest itself at other times in life following separation from a significantly important individual (Cassidy and Shaver, 2016).

Both Spitz and Wolf (1946) and Bowlby (1969) have been accredited for their important contributions to the study of the separation of children from their caregivers. Spitz and Wolf (1946) has identified analogies between the stages of the prolonged separation process in young children (under 1 year of age) called the Emotional Deprivation Syndrome, which can lead to Anaclitic depression and the pathogenic effects of chronic stress referred to as General Adaptation Syndrome (stress syndrome) by Selye (1936) a few years earlier (Selye, 1936, 1946, 1956; see for current discussion Del Giudice et al., 2018). Here we anticipate the observation that drug withdrawal can be considered a General Adaptation Syndrome (Chartoff and Carlezon, 2015).

Descriptions of children’s reactions to separation from caregivers are marked by specific behavioral and neurophysiological states summarized in three phases related to the progression of separation. Bowlby (1969) defined the first phase as “Protest,” characterized by active recalls to the missing caregiver. If the separation continues and the child does not succeed in obtaining proximity to the sought-after parent, the phases of “Despair” and then of “Detachment” occur. These are identified by a gradual and progressive inhibition of the recall and search of the caregiver (Bowlby, 1969). These descriptions of the separation process identified by Spitz and Bowlby have been supported by the identification of a specific neuronal circuit that regulates the separation process, called the PANIC/GRIEF System (Panksepp, 1998). This neuronal pathway is composed of the Anterior Cingulate, the Dorsomedial Thalamus, the Periaqueductal Gray, and some regions of the cerebellum. The activity of other areas has been identified only in animals, but being so small, PET in humans hardly highlights them: the ventral zone of the septum—Dorsal Preoptic Area—Bed Nucleus of Stria Terminalis—BN (Panksepp, 1998).

The PANIC/GRIEF system is also characterized by specific neurochemistry that has a central point in the endogenous opioid system. The separation that gives rise to protest and discomfort is correlated with the depletion of endogenous opioids. If the separation continues, the effect of the continuing state of stress causes the prolonged alteration of the HPA axis (Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis) and contributes to a hyperproduction of the opioid dynorphin (Bruchas et al., 2010).

This in turn, through the innumerable kappa receptors (Shippenberg et al., 2007) found in the ML DA system, inhibits both dopamine production in the ventral tegmental area VTA and its release in the nucleus accumbens (Watt and Panksepp, 2009).

The subsequent dopamine depletion is expressed as a depotentiation of the ML DA SEEKING system that governs the appetitive behavior (Panksepp, 1998; Watt and Panksepp, 2009; Zellner et al., 2011).

The joint effect of these two depletions characterizes not only the dynamics of separation from a caregiver but also those of depression in adulthood and the abstinence both from substance and non-substance addictive behaviors.

Why this particular interest in the Attachment (PANIC/GRIEF system) and in particular for the dynamics connected to separation? Because it is probably the biggest cause of our mental pain (Panksepp and Biven, 2012). The recognition of the caregiver consolidated during infancy, brings with it the stabilization of the need for his/her presence, whose absence will activate the stress and depletion processes mentioned above, responsible for mental pain and analogous to a real craving (uncontrollable compulsive desire; Zellner et al., 2011).

From this point of view, the attachment bond can be considered as the prototype of the “first addiction” (Zellner et al., 2011). In this regard, as pointed out by Jaak Panksepp (Yovell et al., 2016), opiates are the first identified antidepressant drugs, bearing in mind all the inevitable problems they entail.

Opiates use is known to reduce and replace the need for social relationships. However, soon the substance that creates this social well-being, heroin for example, inevitably leads to the learning of a reinforcement that activates the appetitive system to seek it out in a paroxysmal way.

Thus attention shifts to the ML DA SEEKING system. Any addictive substance, alcohol, tobacco, cocaine, et cetera, activate the increase of extracellular dopamine concentration in the dopaminergic system sites and especially in the nucleus accumbens (Volkow and Li, 2004). Quickly, the ML DA SEEKING system becomes the driving center of addictions, both for substance and non-substance addictive behaviors (like gambling, internet, et cetera) that have the same power to subvert the system (Koob and Volkow, 2016; Uhl et al., 2019).

From Infancy to Juvenile Age

In most mammals, there are only two stages of development: infancy and sexual maturity. The transition from one to the other is quite sudden and puberty occurs when growth rates are decreasing (Bogin and Smith, 1996). In more social species—such as wolves, lions, elephants, and primates—a third stage has been added after infancy (namely after weaning): the juvenile stage (Bogin and Smith, 1996), which has postponed the advent of puberty. Infancy indicates the period of maximum dependence of the puppy from the caregiver, represented electively by breastfeeding. In the human species—as mentioned above—it is replaced by childhood around the age of 3. Childhood (3–6 years old) is a period in which the immaturity of the dentition and digestive tract requires food prepared by the caregiver, easily digestible, and very energetic to meet the nutritional requirements for the body growth, especially the brain. After childhood, the juvenile age takes over from around 6–7 years up to about 11–12 years. The juvenile stage is characterized by individuals sufficiently autonomous from their caregivers and with motor and cognitive resources such as to be able to provide in part to their sustenance and protection from predators (Bogin and Smith, 1996), even if they still maintain an interaction of dependence with the adult of reference. One of the characteristics of the juvenile stage, in some primates and in the human species, is the phenomenon of Adrenarche (Maninger et al., 2009). Around the age of 6–8, the maturation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA) is considered the beginning of the pubertal maturation process (Mundy et al., 2015). The maturation of the adrenal cortex, the zona reticularis, determines the production of androgens such as dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulfate DHEA-S along with androstenedione (A4) converted into a certain amount of testosterone (Antoniou-Tsigkos et al., 2019). The latter two, with the maturation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis (HPG), will be produced in greater quantities by the gonads, characterizing the actual sexual development of puberty. Adrenarche is therefore considered to be the beginning of puberty, or as it is defined as “adrenal puberty” (Del Giudice et al., 2009) before the visible signs highlight gonadal puberty. Adrenal androgens are neurosteroids active in brain regions involved in emotional and behavioral regulation (Maninger et al., 2009; Mundy et al., 2015). “Adrenal puberty” determines the appearance of physical signs such as pubic hair, the change in the composition of sweat that produces body odor, but the most significant changes are the behavioral ones shaped by sexual selection and related to competition within gender identity (Del Giudice et al., 2009, p. 2). The effects attributed to the Adrenarche correspond to those of the PLAY system described by the Affective Neuroscience (Panksepp et al., 1984, 1985; Panksepp, 1998). This motivational/emotional system is activated during the juvenile stage of mammals and is characterized by an intra-sexual competitive-like interaction, with the characteristic of reversibility (Panksepp and Biven, 2012). In fact, in these interactions regulated by the PLAY system, the competition does not definitively sanction a winner and a loser, as in the competitive interactions that will follow sexual maturation. This reversibility allows the contenders to maintain a mutually friendly interaction and a source of positive effects (Knutson et al., 2002). One of the evolutionary function of the PLAY system seems to be, therefore, to train juvenile subjects to assume behaviors that will be characteristic of adolescence and then of intra-sexual competition in adulthood, even if there is still no clear evidence of a significant continuity from the rough-and-tumble Play to mature Inter-male aggression in the executive brain mechanisms (Panksepp, 1998, p. 286). The subsequent and massive production of gonadal testosterone in puberty will determine the reduction of the PLAY System manifestations because the androgen hormone promotes aggression, especially between sexual adult male, leading animals and humans to real fights (Panksepp, 1998, p. 286; Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 362).

It is thus possible to hypothesize that the PLAY system—functionally connected to the Adrenarche—constitutes a developmental stage through which two motivational systems start to be mutually conjugated: the Attachment/CARE system and Dominance/Submission system. Adrenarche, through the production of adrenal hormones with a low androgen content, highlights their strategic function in activating in a reduced way the competitive behavioral system of Dominance/Submission. It can be assumed that this allowed the beginning of integration between these latter emotional systems and the Attachment/CARE motivational/emotional systems, making possible friendly proximity between the offspring while they are struggling.

Social Competition in Human Development

While the motivational/emotional systems of Attachment/CARE have as a priority the maintenance of proximity between individuals (Cassidy and Shaver, 2016), the motivational/emotional systems of Dominance/Submission that regulate intra-sexual competition are predisposed to create distance between individuals to achieve an optimal dislocation of the population concerning environmental resources (Lorenz, 1963).

With puberty, the biological beginning of adolescence, the need to stay close to a caregiver is increasingly replaced by the need to join a peer group. Even before puberty, interaction with peers is fundamental for the proper functioning of mental processes (Pellis and Pellis, 2007; Vicedo, 2010), as considered above. With the beginning of adolescence the urge for social dominance, which already had its appearance with the Adrenarche (Del Giudice et al., 2009), now acquires particular importance (Weisfeld, 1999; Hawley, 2011), as a tool of sexual selection and to drive the adolescent away from the family environment (Weisfeldt and Woodward, 2004).

During adolescence, gonadal maturation increases the production of testosterone especially in males, with a ratio about ten times higher than in females (Braunstein, 2007), amplifying the urge for social dominance, which replaces Attachment as the main organizer of the relational mental life. The maturation of the sexual organs with the increasing production of testosterone, with related gender differences (Holder and Blaustein, 2013), stimulate the dopaminergic system functioning (Wahlstrom et al., 2010) connected to the production levels of the androgen hormone (Jardí et al., 2018). This may be one of the variables that determine greater competitiveness among males along with a stronger drive to seek out risky situations. It is believed to be also one of the causes of schizophrenic syndromes that are more prevalent in adolescent males (Trotman et al., 2013; Bratek et al., 2015), than females, while the latter display an increased incidence of depression (Sundquist et al., 2004; Sinclair et al., 2014).

Brain regions are not affected uniformly by the developmental effects of puberty. In particular, the subcortical areas of the ML DA-SEEKING system mature before the cortical ones (Wahlstrom et al., 2010; see above the Dual System Theory), predisposing the subject to both impulsiveness and addiction. In parallel, there is a proliferation of glucocorticoid receptors (GR) in the dopaminergic system, especially in prefrontal cortical areas (Sinclair et al., 2014). The latter is one of the main causes of vulnerability to social stress in adolescents.

Therefore, adolescents are more inclined to seek stimulation (Steinberg, 2007)—in particular social stimuli—compared to individuals in other stages of life, but at the same time, they are particularly vulnerable to social stress. This concept refers to the subject’s vulnerability to easily perceive himself as a loser compared to other conspecifics (Kroes et al., 2006) and therefore inadequate in the context of relationships. This results in negative self-evaluation and evokes a similar emotional state to the one generated by separation related to the Attachment system, because the Social defeat, the loss of social status and the social loss, the loss of a loved one, share some of the same neurohormonal mechanisms: “Both types of social losses share certain key physiological features, such as arousal of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and activation of non-specific arousals/attentional circuits like ascending norepinephrine and acetylcholine systems.” (Panksepp et al., 2002, p. 111). This connection between Social defeat and social loss has been well demonstrated by the resident-intruder paradigms, according to which after defeat, if the loser is reunited with other “friendly” animals it recovers from the stress of defeat. On the contrary, if the loser remains isolated it shows signs of weight loss, heart rate disruptions, body temperature regulation, and increased fear (Panksepp et al., 2002) that characterize depression in the human species. This neuro-hormonal dynamic is activated not only as a consequence of competitive interactions in progress but also in the anticipation that they may happen: “A psychosocial stressor is the anticipation, justified or not, that a challenge to homeostasis looms.” (Sapolsky, 2005, p. 648).

The experience of Social defeat can be related to acute and circumscribed social stress or to a lasting state that then becomes chronic stress (Luckett et al., 2012; Gray et al., 2015).

Experiences of continuous Social defeat, such as causing chronic stress, have a significant effect on the dopaminergic system, reducing functionality (McLaughlin et al., 2006; Selten and Cantor-Graae, 2007; Selten et al., 2013). In animal models, Social defeat stress predisposes the individual to exaggerate subsequent addiction-like behaviors for cocaine, methamphetamine, alcohol, and opioids (Shimamoto, 2018). Those behaviors are related to the intensity, duration, frequency, and stress in defeated animals (Shimamoto, 2018). Exposure to adverse stimuli such as Social defeat, as well as positive reinforcement stimuli, increases concentrations of extracellular dopamine in the terminal areas of the ML DA system, nucleus accumbens, striatum, and prefrontal cortex (Tidey and Miczek, 1996; Piazza and Le Moal, 1998; Sapolsky, 2017). In defeated animals, during the social threat, extracellular dopamine levels in the accumbens and prefrontal cortex increase by approximately 160% from baseline (Tidey and Miczek, 1996).

The Dominance system is influenced by two hormones, testosterone and arginine-vasopressin (AVP; or ADH antidiuretic hormone). Both are involved in sexual dynamics (Panksepp and Biven, 2012), and the latter also in fostering social ties (van der Westhuizen and Solms, 2015). One of the effects of androgenic hormones is to block the separation alarm (or inhibit the PANIC/GRIEF system) and the social withdrawal in adult males (Enter et al., 2014).

As mentioned above, the Dominance system is complementary to the FEAR/Submission system. The latter is marked by the activation of the stress axis through the release of corticotrophin-releasing factor (CFR) and the production of cortisol, the neuro-chemical signature that characterizes the experience of Social defeat (Panksepp, 1998; Sapolsky, 2004, 2005). Cortisol acts by inhibiting the production and functioning of testosterone that promote Dominance behaviors and in turn, it reduces the blocking of the social separation alarm and the withdrawal (Enter et al., 2014; see above also Panksepp et al., 2002). At the same time, cortisol activates the production of the opioid dynorphin (already considered for the effects of separation) that depresses the ML DA system (McLaughlin et al., 2006). It is now well established that the factor that differentiates individuals in the dynamics of Dominance/Submission is the ratio between the basal level of testosterone and that of cortisol (Terburg et al., 2009; van der Westhuizen and Solms, 2015; Barel et al., 2017). Therefore, consequently, it can be hypothesized that this is one of the causes of the variability in addiction vulnerability during adolescence.

The effects of cortisol are, in turn, significantly inhibited by the release of endogenous opioids on mu receptors, or artificially by the administration of morphine as widely studied in “playful” interactions between males of various animal species (Panksepp et al., 1985; Panksepp and Biven, 2012). The administration of opioids in animals makes individuals dominant in “playful” competitive situations (Panksepp et al., 1985). This neurochemical dynamic highlights the close analogy and connection between competition stress and separation stress (Giacolini, 2019).

There is therefore a close correlation between the stress of Social defeat with the FEAR defensive system (Panksepp, 1998) and with the more phylogenetically recent PANIC/GRIEF emotional system. Both of these primary emotional systems share overlapping neuroanatomy, most notably the PAG (periaqueductal gray), and chemicals such as the neurotransmitters GABA, norepinephrine, serotonin, and dopamine. Furthermore, both are activated by CRF (corticotropin-releasing factor; Panksepp, 1998, p. 268; Panksepp and Biven, 2012, p. 334–340).

The positive correlation observed between secure attachment and social dominance and between insecure attachment and tendency to anxiety and behaviors of Submission and Social defeat (Irons and Gilbert, 2005; Hawley et al., 2008) is completely in agreement with the arguments presented above.

How do adolescence, attachment/separation, Dominance/Submission, Social defeat, and the dynamics of addictions combine? As described above, chronic stress has a negative effect on the ML DA system, altering its functionality and rhythmicity, and resulting in a significant reduction of D2 receptors. This reduction contributes to creating a predisposition in the individual towards substance or behavioral addictions, so by stimulating the production of dopamine—as mentioned above—the emotional experience (feeling) of well-being is induced. This experience then drives the subject to search for the object that has caused this emotional state to maintain it at an optimal level (Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999).

In this regard, the study conducted by Morgan et al. (2002) is of particular importance because it is one of the few studies of animal models using primates (Macaca fascicularis), rather than rats or hamsters. In this study, adult male macaques subjected to a long period of isolation showed a depletion of D2 receptors in the basal ganglia, as observed in subjects with substance addiction or in victims of chronic stress. This depletion of D2 receptors can be closely linked to the vulnerability that subjects display after taking substances of abuse and towards which they subsequently become more easily addicted. The adult male macaques thus treated were in due course divided into groups and after about 3 months they could ingest cocaine. The substance was consumed by all the subjects but was subsequently sought compulsively by those animals belonging to the subordinate rank. The dominants were, on the contrary, resilient to addiction, because they had experienced the substance without developing an addiction. PET scans revealed that the addicted, subordinate animals continued to show depletion of D2 receptors in the region of the basal ganglia similar to the one following the period of isolation. The resilient and dominant animals showed instead a recovery to a level similar to the one they had before isolation. According to the authors, this experiment shows that in selected individuals with very similar phenotypic characteristics (basal levels of testosterone and cortisol analogs, et cetera), vulnerability to addiction may be determined by social position or rank that exposes submissive subjects to chronic stress and dominant subjects to the opposite effect (Sapolsky, 2004, 2005). Bearing in mind the questions that arise from extrapolating conclusions about human behavior based on animal models results, Morgan’s study is part of a broad line of research on the effects of Social Defeat, especially in adolescence, which indicates how it predisposes individuals to a vulnerability towards substance abuse and behavioral addictions (Sullivan et al., 2006; Tharp-Taylor et al., 2009; Topper et al., 2011; Shimamoto, 2018). It is worth mentioning that Björkqvist (2001), notes that the terms “dominant” and “subordinate” are used in animal studies, while the terms “bullies” and “victims” are used in human studies, in which the interaction between peers, students or colleagues are examined. Social defeat related to academic, work, and social status frustration has received less attention, although there are some important studies (Selten et al., 2013).

Comparative psychopathology has shown that subordinates are not only constantly in a state of stress, but they are also in a peripheral position concerning the group and more exposed to dangers (Sapolsky, 2004, 2005; Huhman, 2006).

Therefore, Social defeat exposes individuals to emotions (feelings) very similar to the experiences of separation. On the opposite, the achievement of a dominant place evokes feelings that can be considered analogous to that of reunification in the dynamic of separation from a caregiver (Kozorovitskiy and Gould, 2004).

Conclusion

We have described above how sexual maturation contributes to the enhancement of the ML DA system that, in combination with hormonal changes, creates a powerful drive for a wider exploration of the world and sexual competition in search of an expected reward (Ikemoto, 2007).

But simultaneously, the ML DA system in adolescence also creates an equally powerful drive to avoid distress and to seek “safety” from social pain (namely Social defeat; Ikemoto and Panksepp, 1999).

In both cases, adolescence appears to be a period in which the human BrainMind is physiologically in a state predisposed to seek greater arousal (Chambers et al., 2003; Crews et al., 2007) or to avoid sources of pain through fixing on easily accessible sources of pleasure that mitigate or nullify the feared sense of threat (García-Oliva and Piqueras, 2016).

In adolescence, the dynamics of the Dominance/Submission system and its interaction with the Attachment/CARE system acquires a particular intensity due to the asymmetry in the maturation of the subcortical areas compared to the cortical ones (see above the dual systems model) which makes the subjects more exposed to the neuro-hormonal and psychological dynamics of these emotional systems, exposing teenagers to be particularly vulnerable to mental suffering and addictions.

The Attachment motivation system has long been acquired as an instinctual system that regulates relations between children and caregivers (Cassidy and Shaver, 2016). More difficult is the recognition that interactions and relationships between individuals can be equally regulated by the urge for social dominance. If sexuality has always represented the persistence of an instinctual heritage in human mental functioning (Freud, 1905), the competitive interaction—which in every culture influences the dynamics of social relations—is still little studied within the human brain. Adolescence, in this sense, is a phase of development particularly suited to study these instinctual dynamics and the difficult and conflicting work of integrating very different phylogenetically motivational/emotional systems. It is precisely the work of integrating these instinctual systems that creates a state of potential vulnerability to mental pain and consequent addicted problems related to it, together with the dramatic observation of the close functional interaction between the cultural dimension and motivational systems and their regulation (Kunnen, 2012).

In traditional cultures, the difficult passage characteristic of the adolescence stage has been ritualized through social practices that often consisted of removing adolescents from the family context and making them live together. During this transition phase, especially adolescent males were often subjected to tests of courage to prove their worth and their ability to survive by themselves. Furthermore, the rituals of initiation into adulthood often included the use of drugs and other practices that caused altered states of consciousness to lead the individual towards new forms of representation of reality. Such practices have been maintained, albeit in a modified form even in contemporary society. Many of the risk-taking behaviors typical of today’s adolescents can be seen as similar initiatory rituals which, however, very often have lost their socio-cultural reference and meaning and which therefore threaten to trap the individual in a spiral of compulsive behaviors with no way out. Moreover, the consequences of risk-taking are likely to be context-dependent. In our modern society, the environments where adolescents take risks (e.g., driving cars) may result in maladaptive instead of adaptive outcomes (Spear, 2008).

Within the BrainMind (Panksepp and Biven, 2012), the regulation and the integration between motivational/emotional systems are a central aspect in the construction of both normal and pathological personality functioning (Montag and Panksepp, 2017). However, as contemporary authors pointed out, the heuristic potential connected to the study of motivational/emotional systems has still to be widely explored (Maze, 1993; Boag, 2014), with particular regard to the urge for social dominance (Hawley, 2002; Johnson et al., 2012; Panksepp and Biven, 2012).

A limit of this article is that the authors do not address the contrast between the primary process and the secondary and tertiary processes (Panksepp and Biven, 2012) because it is beyond the scope of this article. We intend to address this issue in a future article.

Author Contributions

The article was a collaborative, joint effort in which TG took the theoretical lead, AA introduced important neuroscientific contributions, DC played a more supportive and advisory role. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Child Neurology and Psychiatry, Department of Human Neuroscience, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy (Prof. Vincenzo Leuzzi); University project 2018, Grant No. RM11816433230C9C (DC).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ The capital letters used for motivational/emotional systems are a convention in Panksepp’s works both for labeling neurologically and for a need of a specialized terminology ones.
  2. ^Three distinct aggressive circuit hae been provisionally identified in the mammalia brain: predatory, intermale, and affective attack or RAGE circuits” (Panksepp, 1998, p. 188).
  3. ^ Although they are separate developmental phases, there are nevertheless interesting overlaps between the attachment processes and the processes of social competitiveness since some psychobiological indices of secure attachment are also common to individuals who show a propensity for dominance. As a single example, it is sufficient to note that the quantity of D2 receptors, which we can generally associate with the steady functioning of the ML DA-SEEKING system is a good indicator of secure attachment and social dominance. Conversely, lower concentrations of D2 receptors in striatal areas is an indicator of social submission and anxious insecurity that predisposes to the development and maintenance of compulsive behaviors (Morgan et al., 2002).

References

Abeliovich, R. (2018). Reconsidering arnold van gennep’s Les rites de passage from the perspective of ‘Performance Studies’. J. Class. Sociol. 18, 283–298. doi: 10.1177/1468795x18789013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcaro, A. (2019). Il soffio della consapevolezza ed il taglio della coscienza. Riv. Psicol. Analit. 48, 119–137.

Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., Huber, R., and Panksepp, J. (2007). Behavioral functions of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system: an affective neuroethological perspective. Brain Res. Rev. 56, 283–321. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., and Panksepp, J. (2011). The SEEKING mind: primal neuro-affective substrates for appetitive incentive states and their pathological dynamics in addictions and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1805–1820. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alcaro, A., and Panksepp, J. (2014). “Le radici affettive e immaginative del Sé. Un’indagine neuroetologica sulle origini della soggettività,” in La Plasticità del Sé. Una Prospettiva Neuropsicodinamica, eds G. Nortoff, M. Farinelli, R. Chattat, and F. Baldoni (Bologna: Il Mulino), 65–89.

Google Scholar

Alexander, R. D. (1990). How did humans evolve? Reflections on the uniquely unique species. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology Special Publication 1, 1–38.

Google Scholar

Alexander, B. C., and Norbeck, E. (2011). Rite of passage. Encyclopedia Britannica. Available online at: https://www.britannica.com/topic/rite-of-passage

Google Scholar

Antoniou-Tsigkos, A., Zapanti, E., Ghizzoni, L., and Mastorakos, G. (2019). “Adrenal androgens,” in Endotext [Internet], eds K. R. Feingold, B. Anawal, and A. Boyce (South Dartmouth, MA: MDtext.com), 1–23.

Google Scholar

Arain, M., Haque, M., Johal, L., Mathur, P., Nel, W., Rais, A., et al. (2013). Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 9, 449–461. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S39776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardo, M. T., Donohew, R. L., and Harrington, N. G. (1996). Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 77, 23–43. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00203-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barel, E., Shahrabani, S., and Tzischinsky, O. (2017). Sex hormone/cortisol ratios differentially modulate risk-taking in men and women. Evol. Psychol. 15:1474704917697333. doi: 10.1177/1474704917697333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res. Rev. 28, 309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björkqvist, K. (2001). Social defeat as a stressor in humans. Physiol. Behav. 73, 435–442. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00490-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boag, S. (2014). Ego, drives, and the dynamics of internal objects. Front. Psychol. 5:666. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogin, B. (1994). Adolescence in evolutionary perspective. Acta Paediatr. 406, 29–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13418.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogin, B. (1997). Evolutionary hypotheses for human childhood. Yearbook Phys. Anthropol. 40, 63–90. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(1997)25+<63::AID-AJPA3>3.0.CO;2-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogin, B. (1999). Patterns of Human Growth, 2nd edition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bogin, B., and Smith, B. H. (1996). Evolution of the human life cycle. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 8, 703–716. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6300(1996)8:6<703::AID-AJHB2>3.0.CO;2-U

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boughner, J. C., and Rolian, C. (2016). Developmental Approaches to Human Evolution. New Jersey, NJ: Wiley Publishing.

Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and Loss. London: The Hogarth Press.

Google Scholar

Bratek, A., Koźmin-Burzyñska, A., Krysta, K., Cierpka-Wiszniewska, K., and Krupka-Matuszczyk, I. (2015). Effects of hormones on cognition in schizophrenic male patients—preliminary results. Psychiatr. Danub. 27, S261–S265.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Braunstein, G. D. (2007). Safety of testosterone treatment in postmenopausal women. Fertil. Steril. 88, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2007.01.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brischoux, F., Chakraborty, S., Brierley, D. I., and Ungless, M. A. (2009). Phasic excitation of dopamine neurons in ventral VTA by noxious stimuli. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 4894–4899. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811507106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., and Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational control: rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron 68, 815–834. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruchas, M., Land, B., and Chavkin, C. (2010). The dynorphin/kappa opioid system as a modulator of stress-induced and pro-addictive behaviors. Brain Res. 1314, 44–55. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., Kroes, R. A., Moskal, J. R., Pfaus, J. G., Brudzynski, S. M., and Panksepp, J. (2008). Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play and aggression: behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback. J. Comp. Psychol. 122, 357–367. doi: 10.1037/a0012889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cacioppo, J., Cacioppo, S., Dulawa, S., and Palmer, A. (2014). Social neuroscience and its potential contribution to psychiatry. World Psychiatry 13, 131–139. doi: 10.1002/wps.20118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carver, C. S., and Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: evidence and implications. Psychol. Bull. 135, 183–204. doi: 10.1037/a0013965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Galván, A., and Somerville, L. H. (2016). Beyond simple models of adolescence to an integrated circuit-based account: a commentary. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 128–130. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., and Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1124, 111–126. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (2016). Handbook of Attachment. Theory, Research and Clinical Applications, 3rd edition. New York, NY: The Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Chambers, R. A., Taylor, J. R., and Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chartoff, E. H., and Carlezon, W. A. Jr. (2015). Drug withdrawal conceptualized as a stressor. Behav. Pharmacol. 25, 473–492. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chefer, V. I., Zakharova, I., and Shippenberg, T. S. (2003). Enhanced responsiveness to novelty and cocaine is associated with decreased basal dopamine uptake and release in the nucleus accumbens: quantitative microdialysis in rats under transient conditions. J. Neurosci. 23, 3076–3084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-03076.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., and Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Dev. Sci. 14, F1–F10. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01035.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chester, D. S. (2017). The role of positive affect in aggression. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 366–370. doi: 10.1177/0963721417700457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chester, D. S., Bell, S. B., DeWall, C. N., West, S. J., Romero-Lopez, M., and Craig, A. (2019). Neural correlates of intertemporal choice in aggressive behavior. Aggress. Behav. 45, 507–516. doi: 10.1002/ab.21838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chester, D. S., and DeWall, C. N. (2016). The pleasure of revenge: retaliatory aggression arises from a neural imbalance towards reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chester, D. S., DeWall, C. N., Derefinko, K. J., Estus, S., Lynam, D. R., Peters, J. R., et al. (2016). Looking for reward in all the wrong places: dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms indirectly affect aggression through sensation-seeking. Soc. Neurosci. 11, 487–494. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1119191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crews, F., He, J., and Hodge, C. (2007). Adolescent cortical development: a critical period of vulnerability for addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 86, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., and Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636–650. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.

Del Giudice, M., Angeleri, R., and Manera, V. (2009). The juvenile transition: a developmental switch point in human life history. Dev. Rev. 29, 1–31. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2008.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giudice, M., Buck, C. L., Chaby, L., Gormally, B. M., Taff, C. C., Thawley, C. J., et al. (2018). What is stress? A systems perspective. Integr. Comp. Biol. 58, 1019–1032. doi: 10.1093/icb/icy114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., and Kaplan, H. (2015). “Life history theory and evolutionary psychology,” in The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, 2nd Edn., D. M. Buss (New York, NY: Wiley), 88–114.

Google Scholar

Di Chiara, G., and Imperato, A. (1988). Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 85, 5274–5278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.85.14.5274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1984). Die Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens. Grundriß der Humanethologie. München: Piper. [English translation Human Ethology. New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter].

Google Scholar

Enter, D., Spinhoven, P., and Roelofs, K. (2014). Alleviating social avoidance: effects of single dose testosterone administration on approach-avoidance action. Horm. Behav. 65, 351–354. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage 25, 1279–1291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fish, E. W., DeBold, J. F., and Miczek, K. A. (2005). Escalated aggression as a reward: corticosterone and GABAA receptor positive modulators in mice. Psychopharmacology 182, 116–127. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0064-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freud, S. (1905). Three Essays on Sexuality. Strachey, J. (1953). S. E. V. VII(1901–1905). London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis.

Google Scholar

Galván, A. (2014). “Neural systems underlying reward and approach behaviors,” in The Neurobiology of Childhood, eds S. L. Andersen and D. S. Pine (Berlin: Springer), 167–188.

Google Scholar

Galván, A., Hare, T. A., Parra, C. E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G., et al. (2006). Earlier development of the accumbens relative to orbitofrontal cortex might underlie risk-taking behavior in adolescents. J. Neurosci. 26, 6885–6892. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1062-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galván, A., and McGlennen, K. (2013). Enhanced striatal sensitivity to aversive reinforcement in adolescents versus adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 284–296. doi: 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2020.129190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Oliva, C., and Piqueras, J. A. (2016). Experiential avoidance and technological addictions in adolescents. J. Behav. Addict. 5, 293–303. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, S. R., Fan, T., Ng, G. Y., Jung, S. Y., O‘Dowd, B. F., and Naranjo, C. A. (1995). Low endogenous dopamine function in brain predisposes to high alcohol preference and consumption: reversal by increasing synaptic dopamine. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 273, 373–379.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Giacolini, T. (2019). Social competition, psychopathology and the (Physiological) developmental trauma. A narrative review. EC Psychol. Psychiatry 8, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Giacolini, T., and Sabatello, U. (2019). Psychoanalysis and affective neuroscience. The motivational/emotional system of aggression in human relations. Front. Psychol. 9:2475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (1992). “Depression”: The Evolution of Powerlessness. Hove: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Gilbert, P. (2006). Evolution and depression: issues and implications. Psychol. Med. 36, 287–297. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gladwin, T. E., and Figner, B. (2014). ““Hot” cognition and dual systems: introduction, criticisms, and ways forward,” in Neuroeconomics, Judgment and Decision Making, eds E. A. Wilhelms and V. F. Reyna (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 157–180.

Google Scholar

Golden, S., and Shaham, Y. (2018). Aggression addiction and relapse: a new Frontier in Psychiatry. Neuropsychopharmacology 43, 224–225. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A. (2000). The tonic/phasic model of dopamine system regulation and its implications for understanding alcohol and psychostimulant craving. Addiction 95, 119–128. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, K. L., and Burghardt, G. M. (2010). Current perspectives on the biological study of play: signs of progress. Q. Rev. Biol. 85, 393–418. doi: 10.1086/656903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. L., Norvelle, A., Larkin, T., and Huhman, K. L. (2015). Dopamine in the nucleus accumbens modulates the memory of social defeat in Syrian hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus). Behav. Brain Res. 286, 22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.02.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. D. (2008). The biopsychosocial and “complex” systems approach as a unified framework for addiction. Behav. Brain Sci. 31, 446–447. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x08004822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, M. D., and Larkin, M. (2004). Editorial. Conceptualizing addiction: the case for a “complex systems” account. Addict. Res. Theor. 12, 99–102. doi: 10.1080/1606635042000193211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyer, A. E., Nelson, E. E., Pérez-Edgar, K., Hardin, M. G., Roberson-Nay, R., Monk, C. S., et al. (2006). Striatal functional alteration in adolescents characterized by early childhood behavioral inhibition. J. Neurosci. 26, 6399–6405. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0666-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, P. J., Geddes, J. R., and Tunbridge, E. M. (2018). Review the emerging neurobiology of bipolar disorder trends. Trend Neurosci. 41, 18–30. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: a strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Dev. Rev. 19, 97–132. doi: 10.1006/drev.1998.0470

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H. (2002). Social dominance and prosocial and coercive strategies of resource control in preschoolers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 167–176. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00073-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H. (2011). The evolution of adolescence and the adolescence of evolution: the coming of age of humans and the theory about the forces that made them. J. Res. Adolesc. 21, 307–316. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00732.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H., and Little, T. D. (1999). On winning some and losing some: a social relations approach to social dominance in toddlers. Merrill Palmer Q. 45, 185–214.

Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., and Card, N. A. (2008). The myth of the alpha male: a new look at dominance-related beliefs and behaviors among adolescent males and females. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 32, 76–88. doi: 10.1177/0165025407084054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawley, P. H., Little, T. D., and Pasupathi, M. (2002). Winning friends and influencing peers: strategies of peer influence in late childhood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 466–473. doi: 10.1080/01650250143000427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hecker, T., Hermenau, K., Maedl, A., Elbert, T., and Schauer, M. (2012). Appetitive aggression in former combatants—derived from the ongoing conflict in DR Congo. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 35, 244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochberg, Z. (2009). Evo-devo of child growth II: human life history and transition between its phases. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 160, 135–141. doi: 10.1530/EJE-08-0445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochberg, Z., and Belsky, J. (2013). Evo-devo of human adolescence: beyond disease models of early puberty. BMC Med. 11, 113–124. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hochberg, Z., and Konner, M. (2020). Emerging adulthood, a pre-adult life-history stage. Front. Endocrinol. 10:918. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2019.00918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holder, M. K., and Blaustein, J. D. (2013). Puberty and adolescence as a time of vulnerability to stressors that alter neurobehavioral processes. Front. Endocrinol. 35, 89–110. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2013.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huhman, K. L. (2006). Social conflict models: can they inform us about human psychopathology? Horm. Behav. 50, 640–646. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikemoto, S. (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens-olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Res. Rev. 56, 27–78. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikemoto, S., and Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: a unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Res. Rev. 31, 6–41. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00023-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R., and Young, L. J. (2001). The neurobiology of attachment. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Irons, C., and Gilbert, P. (2005). Evolved mechanisms in adolescent anxiety and depression symptoms: the role of the attachment and social rank systems. J. Adolesc. 28, 325–341. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jardí, F., Laurent, M. R., Dubois, V., Kim, N., Khalil, R., Decallonne, B., et al. (2018). Androgen and estrogen actions on male physical activity: a story beyond muscle. J. Endocrinol. 238, R31–R52. doi: 10.1530/JOE-18-0125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. L., and Carver, C. S. (2012). The dominance behavioral system and manic temperament: motivation for dominance, self-perceptions of power, and socially dominant behavior. J. Affect. Disord. 142, 275–282. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, S. L., Leedom, L., and Muhtadie, L. (2012). The dominance behavioral system and psychopathology: evidence from self-report, observational, and biological studies. Psychol. Bull. 138, 692–743. doi: 10.1037/a0027503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, A., and Garner, J. K. (2017). A complex dynamic systems perspective on identity and its development: the dynamic systems model of role identity. Dev. Psychol. J. 53, 2036–2051. doi: 10.1037/dev0000339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knipper, C., Mittnik, A., Massy, K., Kociumaka, C., Kucukkalipci, I., and Maus, M. (2017). Female exogamy and gene pool diversification at the transition from the Final Neolithic to the Early Bronze Age in central Europe. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 114, 10083–10088. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706355114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knowles, J., Capiron, R., Tredway, C., and Burke, D. (2019). “Life history model of psychopathology,” in Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science, eds T. Shackelford and V. Weekes-Shackelford (Berlin: Springer), 1–7. doi: 10.10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_702-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, B., Burgdorf, J., and Panksepp, J. (2002). Ultrasonic vocalizations as indices of affective states in rats. Psychol. Bull. 128, 961–977. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Volkow, N. D. (2016). Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 760–773. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozorovitskiy, Y., and Gould, E. (2004). Dominance hierarchy influences adult neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus. J. Neurosci. 24, 6755–6759. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0345-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroes, R. A., Panksepp, J., Burgdorf, J., Otto, N. J., and Moskal, J. R. (2006). Modeling depression: social dominance-submission gene expression patterns in rat neocortex. Neuroscience 137, 37–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunnen, E. S. (2012). A Dynamic Systems Approach to Adolescent Development. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Kunnen, E. S., De Ruiter, N. M. P., Jeronimus, B. F., and van der Gaag, M. A. (2019). Psychosocial Development in Adolescence: Insights from the Dynamic Systems Approach. New York, NY: Routledge Psychology.

Lammel, S., Lim, B. K., and Malenka, R. C. (2014). Reward and aversion in a heterogeneous midbrain dopamine system. Neuropharmacology 76, 351–359. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lammel, S., Lim, B. K., Ran, C., Huang, K. W., Betley, M. J., Tye, K. M., et al. (2012). Inputspecific control of reward and aversion in the ventral tegmental area. Nature 491, 212–217. doi: 10.1038/nature11527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, R. D. (2017). Moving beyond fight and flight: a contingent model of how the emotional regulation of anger and fear sparks proactivity. Acad. Manag. Rev. 42, 190–206. doi: 10.5465/amr.2014.0368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legrand, R. (2013). Successful aggression as the reinforcer for runway behavior of mice. Psychon. Sci. 20, 303–305. doi: 10.3758/bf03329080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 169–245. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x0500004x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenfors, P., and Tullberg, B. S. (2011). Evolutionary aspects of aggression the importance of sexual selection. Adv. Genet. 75, 7–22. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-380858-5.00009-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, R. W., and McClung, C. A. (2016). Review animal models of bipolar mania: the past, the present and future. Neuroscience 321, 163–188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. Z. (1937). The Companion in the Bird’s World. The Auk 54, 245–273. doi: 10.1007/BF01905355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. (1963). Das Sogenannte Böse zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression [So-called evil, Toward a Natural History of Aggression]. Borotha-Schoeler G: Original edition.

Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and Modification of Behavior. London: Metheun.

Google Scholar

Luckett, C., Norvelle, A., and Huhman, K. (2012). The role of the nucleus accumbens in the acquisition and expression of conditioned defeat. Behav. Brain Res. 227, 208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLean, P. D. (1990). The Triune Brain in Evolution. Role in Paleocerebral Functions. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Mahajan, N., Martinez, M. A., Guiterrez, N. L., Diesendruck, G., Banaji, M. R., and Santos, L. R. (2011). The evolution of intergroup bias: perceptions and attitudes in rhesus macaques. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 387–405. doi: 10.1037/a0022459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malatynska, E., and Knapp, R. J. (2005). Dominant-submissive behavior as models of mania and depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 715–737. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maninger, N., Wolkowitz, O. M., Reus, V. I., Epel, E. S., and Mellon, S. H. (2009). Neurobiological and neuropsychiatric effects of dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and DHEA sulfate (DHEAS). Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30, 65–91. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2008.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinelli, M., and White, F. J. (2000). Enhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of midbrain dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 20, 8876–8885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08876.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massen, J. J. M., Behrens, F., Martin, J. S., Stocker, M., and Brosnan, S. F. (2019). A comparative approach to affect and cooperation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 107, 370–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maze, J. R. (1993). The complementarity of object-relations and instinct theory. Int. J. Psychoanal. 74, 459–470.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., and Van Vugt, M. (2012). Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: the male warrior hypothesis. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 367, 670–679. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, J. P., Shuang, L., Valdez, J., Chavkin, T. A., and Chavkin, C. (2006). Social defeat stress-induced behavioral responses are mediated by the endogenous kappa opioid system. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 1241–1248. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., Zeichner, A., and Wilson, L. F. (2012). Personality correlates of aggression evidence from measures of the five-factor model, UPPS model of impulsivity and BIS/BAS. J. Int. Violence 27, 2903–2919. doi: 10.1177/0886260512438279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montag, C., and Panksepp, J. (2017). Primary emotional systems and personality: an evolutionary perspective. Front. Psychol. 8:464. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, D., Grant, K. A., Gage, H. D., Mach, R. H., Kaplan, J. R., Prioleau, O., et al. (2002). Social dominance in monkeys: dopamine D2 receptors and cocaine self-administration. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 169–174. doi: 10.1038/nn798

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundy, L. K., Romaniuk, H., Canterford, L., Hearps, S., Viner, R. M., Bayer, J. K., et al. (2015). Adrenarche and the emotional and behavioral problems of late childhood. J. Adolesc. Health 57, 608–616. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muñoz-Reyes, J. A., Polo, P., Valenzuela, N., Pavez, P., and Ramírez-Herrera, O. (2020). The male warrior hypothesis: testosterone-related cooperation and aggression in the context of intergroup conflict. Sci. Rep. 10:375. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-57259-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, M. A., and Czoty, P. W. (2005). PET imaging studies of dopamine D2 receptors in monkey models of cocaine abuse: genetic predisposition versus environmental modulation. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 1473–1482. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.8.1473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, M. A., Czoty, P. W., Nader, S. H., and Morgan, D. (2012a). Nonhuman primate models of social behavior and cocaine. Psychopharmacology 224, 57–67. doi: 10.1007/s00213-012-2843-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nader, M. A., Nader, S. H., Czoty, P. W., Riddick, N. V., Gage, H. D., Gould, R. W., et al. (2012b). Social dominance in female monkeys: dopamine receptor function and cocaine reinforcement. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Natarajan, D., and Caramaschi, D. (2000). Animal violence demystified. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:9. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J. (2002). Common molecular and cellular substrates of addiction and memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 78, 637–647. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J. (2014). Epigenetic mechanisms of drug addiction. Neuropharmacology 76, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmanabhan, A., Geier, C. F., Ordaz, S. J., Teslovich, T., and Luna, B. (2011). Developmental changes in brain function underlying the influence of reward processing on inhibitory control. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 517–529. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmanabhan, A., and Luna, B. (2014). Developmental imaging genetics: linking dopamine function to adolescent behavior. Brain Cogn. 89, 27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience. The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions. New York, NY: Oxford Universyty Press.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: do animals have affective lives? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1791–1804. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2014). Will better psychiatric treatments emerge from top-down or bottom-up neuroscientific studiesof affect? World Psychiatry 13, 141–142. doi: 10.1002/wps.20120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Biven, L. (2012). The Archaeology of Mind. New York, NY: W. W. Norton and Company.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Jalowiec, J., DeEskinazi, F. G., and Bishop, P. (1985). Opiates and play dominance in juvenile rats. Behav. Neurosci. 99, 441–453. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Moskal, J. R., Panksepp, J. B., and Kroes, R. A. (2002). Comparative approaches in evolutionary psychology: molecular neuroscience meets the mind. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 23, 105–115.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Siviy, S., and Normansell, L. (1984). The psychobiology of play: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 465–492. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90005-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Solms, M. (2012). The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sci. 2, 147–175. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2020147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Watt, D. (2011). Why does depression hurt? Ancestral primary-process separation-distress (PANIC/GRIEF) and diminished brain reward (SEEKING) processes in the genesis of depressive. Psychiatry 74, 5–13. doi: 10.1521/psyc.2011.74.1.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., and Zellner, M. R. (2004). Towards a neurobiologically based unified theory of aggression. Rev. Int. Psychol. Soc. 17, 37–62.

Google Scholar

Pellegrini, A. D., and Long, J. D. (2002). A longitudinal study of bullying, dominance and victimization during the transition from primary school through secondary school. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 20, 259–280. doi: 10.1348/026151002166442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellegrini, A. D., Roseth, C. J., Milner, S., Bohn, C. M., Van Ryzin, M., Vance, N., et al. (2007). Social dominance in preschool classrooms. J. Comp. Psychol. 121, 54–64. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.121.1.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pellis, S. M., and Pellis, V. C. (2007). Rough-and-tumble play and the development of the social brain. Assoc. Psychol. Sci. 16, 95–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00483.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennartz, C. M., Berke, J. D., Graybiel, A. M., Ito, R., Lansink, C. S., van der Meer, M., et al. (2009). Corticostriatal interactions during learning, memory processing, and decision making. J. Neurosci. 29, 12831–12838. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3177-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, J. H., and Allen, N. B. (2012). Arrested development? Reconsidering dual-systems models of brain function in adolescence and disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 322–329. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, P. V., Deminière, J. M., Le Moal, M., and Simon, H. (1989). Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science 245, 1511–1513. doi: 10.1126/science.2781295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, P. V., and Le Moal, M. (1998). The role of stress in drug self-administration. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 19, 67–74. doi: 10.1016/s0165-6147(97)01115-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierre, P. J., and Vezina, P. (1997). Predisposition to self-administer amphetamine: the contribution of response to novelty and prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology 129, 277–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130050191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, J. S., Gardner, R. Jr., Wilson, D. R., Sloman, L., Rohde, P., and Erickson, M. (2007). Territory, rank and mental health: the history of an idea. Evol. Psychol. 5, 531–554. doi: 10.1177/147470490700500305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puurtinen, M., and Mappes, T. (2009). Group competition and human cooperation. Proc. Biol. Soc. 276, 355–360. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratliff, E. A., Kaduri, P., Masao, F., Mbwambo, J., and McCurdy, S. (2016). Harm reduction as a complex adaptive system: a dynamic framework for analyzing Tanzanian policies concerning heroin use. Int. J. Drug Policy 30, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2015.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimers, L., and Diekhof, E. K. (2015). Testosterone is associated with cooperation during intergroup competition by enhancing parochial altruism. Front. Neurosci. 9:183. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2001). Incentive-sensitization and addiction. Addiction 96, 103–114. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.9611038.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2003). Addiction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2008). The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 3137–3146. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robson, S., and Wood, B. (2008). Hominin lie history: recostrunction and evolution. J. Anat. 212, 394–425. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2008.00867.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., and Correa, M. (2002). Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav. Brain Res. 137, 3–25. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00282-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., and Correa, M. (2012). The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76, 470–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Social status and health in humans and other animals. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 33, 393–418. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.33.070203.144000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science 308, 648–652. doi: 10.1126/science.1106477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapolsky, R. M. (2017). Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst. New York, NY: Penguin.

Google Scholar

Scarr, S., and McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Dev. 54, 424–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1983.tb03884.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitz, Y., Benoit-Marand, M., Gonon, F., and Sulzer, D. (2003). Presynaptic regulation of dopaminergic neurotransmission. J. Neurochem. 87, 273–289. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2003.02050.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, J. P., and Fredericson, E. (1951). The causes of fighting in mice and rats. Physiol. Zool. 24, 273–309. doi: 10.1086/physzool.24.4.30152137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selten, J.-P., and Cantor-Graae, E. (2007). Hypothesis: Social defeat is a risk factor for schizophrenia? Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 51, s9–s12. doi: 10.1192/bjp.191.51.s9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selten, J.-P., van der Ven, E., Rutten, B. P. F., and Cantor-Graae, E. (2013). The Social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr. Bull. 39, 1180–1186. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature 1936, 132–138. doi: 10.1038/138032a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1946). The general adaptation syndrome and the disease of adaptation. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 6, 117–130. doi: 10.1210/jcem-6-2-117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Selye, H. (1956). The Stress of Life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Shimamoto, A. (2018). Social defeat stress, sex, and addiction-like. Behav. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 140, 271–313. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shippenberg, T. S., Zapata, A., and Chefer, V. I. (2007). Dynorphin and the pathophysiology of drug addiction. Pharmacol. Ther. 116, 306–321. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2007.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, E. P., Smith, A. R., Silva, K., Icenogle, G., Duell, N., Chein, J., et al. (2016). The dual systems model: review, reappraisal and reaffirmation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 103–117. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, D., Purves-Tyson, T. D., Allen, K. M., and Weickert, C. S. (2014). Impacts of stress and sex hormones on dopamine neurotransmission in the adolescent brain. Psychopharmacology 231, 1581–1599. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3415-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sloman, L. (2002). “Involuntary defeat strategy as backdrop for depression,” in The Evolutionary Neuroethology, eds G. A. Jr. Cory and R. Gardner (Westport, CT: Praeger), 119–132.

Google Scholar

Sloman, L., and Dunham, D. W. (2004). The matthew effect: evolutionary implications. Evolut. Psychol. 2, 92–104. doi: 10.1177/147470490400200115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sloman, L., and Gilbert, P. (2000). Subordination and Defeat: An Evolutionary Approach to Mood Disorders and their Treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Solms, M. (2018). What is ‘the unconscious’ and where is it located in the brain? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1406, 90–97. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M. (2019). The hard problem of consciousness and the free energy principle. Front. Psychol. 9:2714. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solms, M., and Turnbull, O. (2002). The Brain and the Inner World: An Introduction to the Neuroscience of Subjective Experience. New York, NY: Other Press.

Google Scholar

Somerville, L. H., Hare, T., and Casey, B. J. (2011). Frontostriatal maturation predicts cognitive control failure to appetitive cues in adolescents. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2123–2134. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(00)00014-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2008). “The psychobiology of adolescence,” in Authoritative Communities, eds K. K. Kathleen (New York, NY: Springer), 263–280.

Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2011). Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 390–403. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spitz, R., and Wolf, K. (1946). Anaclitic depression: an inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanal. Study Child 2, 313–342.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk-taking in adolescence: new perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 55–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steketee, J. D., and Kalivas, P. W. (2011). Drug wanting: behavioral sensitization and relapse to drug-seeking behavior. Pharmacol. Rev. 63, 348–365. doi: 10.1124/pr.109.001933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strayer, J., and Strayer, F. F. (1978). Social aggression and power relations among preschool children. Aggressive Behav. 4, 173–182.

Google Scholar

Suchak, M., Eppley, T. M., Campbell, M. W., Feldman, R. A., Quarles, L. F., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2016). How chimpanzees cooperate in a competitive world. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 113, 10215–10220. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1611826113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, T. N., Farrell, A. D., and Kliewer, W. (2006). Peer victimization in early adolescence: association between physical and relational victimization and drug use, aggression and delinquent behaviors among urban middle school students. Dev. Psychopathol. 18, 119–137. doi: 10.1017/S095457940606007X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundquist, K., Frank, G., and Sundquist, J. (2004). Urbanisation and incidence of psychosis and depression: follow-up study of 4.4 million women and men in Sweden. Br. J. Psychiatry 184, 293–298. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.4.293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanimoto, H., Heisenberg, M., and Gerber, B. (2004). Experimental psychology: event timing turns punishment to reward. Nature 430:983. doi: 10.1038/430983a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terburg, D., Morgan, B., and Van Honk, J. (2009). The testosterone-cortisol ratio: a hormonal marker for proneness to social aggression. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 32, 216–223. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tharp-Taylor, S., Haviland, A., and D’Amico, E. J. (2009). Victimization from mental and physical bullying and substance use in early adolescence. Addict. Behav. 34, 561–567. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tidey, J. W., and Miczek, K. A. (1996). Social defeat stress selectively alters mesocorticolimbic dopamine release: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res. 721, 140–149. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00159-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Topper, L. R., Castellanos-Ryan, N., Mackie, C., and Conrod, P. J. (2011). Adolescent bullying victimization and alcohol related problem behavior mediated by coping drinking motives over a 12 months period. Addict. Behav. 36, 6–13. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trotman, H. D., Holtzman, C. W., Ryan, A. T., Shapiro, D. I., MacDonald, A. N., Goulding, S. M., et al. (2013). The development of psychotic disorders in adolescence: a potential role for hormones. Horm. Behav. 64, 411–419. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhl, G. R., Koob, G. F., and Cable, J. (2019). The neurobiology of addiction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1451, 5–28. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Dennen, J. M. G. (2005). The role of fear in the agonistic complex. Default journal. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure). Available online at: http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. Accessed June 11, 2018.

Google Scholar

van der Westhuizen, D., and Solms, M. (2015). Basic emotional foundations of social dominance in relation to Panksepp’s affective taxonomy. Neuropsychoanalysis 17, 19–37. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2015.1021371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Leijenhorst, L., Moor, B. G., Op de Macks, Z. A., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Westenberg, P. M., and Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. NeuroImage 51, 345–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., and Janssen, D. (2007). Gender differences in competition and cooperation: the male warrior hypothesis. Psychol. Sci. 18, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vicedo, M. (2010). The evolution of Harry Harlow: from the nature to the nurture of love. His. Psychiatry 21, 190–205. doi: 10.1177/0957154x10370909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., and Li, T. (2004). Drug addiction: the neurobiology of behavior gone awry. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 963–970. doi: 10.1038/nrn1539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wahlstrom, D., White, T., and Luciana, M. (2010). Neurobehavioral evidence for changes in dopamine system activity during adolescence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 631–648. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, D. M., Bell, M. R., Flores, C., Gulley, J. M., Willing, J., and Paul, M. J. (2017). Adolescence and reward: making sense of neural and behavioral changes Amid the Chaos. J. Neurosci. 37, 10855–10866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1834-17.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watt, D. F., and Panksepp, J. (2009). Depression: an evolutionarily conserved mechanism to terminate separation distress. A review of aminergic, peptidergic and neural network perspectives. Neuropsychoanalysis 11, 7–52. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2009.10773593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weierstall, R., and Elbert, T. (2011). The appetitive aggression scale—development of an instrument for the assessment of human’s attraction to violence. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 2:8430. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v2i0.8430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisfeld, G. E. (1999). Evolutionary Principles of Human Adolescence. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Weisfeldt, G. L., and Woodward, L. (2004). Current evolutionary perspectives on adolescent romantic relations and sexuality. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 11–19. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200401000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, N. M. (1996). Addictive drugs as reinforcers: multiple partial actions on memory systems. Addiction 91, 921–950.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Wierenga, L. M., Bos, M., Schreuders, E., Van de Kamp, F., Peper, J. S., Tamnes, C. K., et al. (2018). Unraveling age, puberty and testosterone effects on subcortical brain development across adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology 91, 105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. L., and Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 32, 363–392. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.32.061002.120046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, R. A., and Bozarth, M. A. (1987). A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. Psychol. Rev. 94, 469–492.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yovell, Y., Bar, G., Mashiah, M., Baruch, Y., Briskman, I., Asherov, J., et al. (2016). Ultra-low-dose buprenorphine as a time-limited treatment for severe suicidal ideation: a randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 491–498. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15040535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zellner, M. R., Watt, D. F., Solms, M., and Panksepp, J. (2011). Affective neuroscientific and neuropsychoanalytic approaches to two intractable psychiatric problems: why depression feels so bad and what addicts really want. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 2000–2008. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: addiction, adolescence, affective neuroscience, attachment, dominance/submission systems

Citation: Giacolini T, Conversi D and Alcaro A (2021) The Brain Emotional Systems in Addictions: From Attachment to Dominance/Submission Systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:609467. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.609467

Received: 23 September 2020; Accepted: 22 December 2020;
Published: 15 January 2021.

Edited by:

Human Friedrich Unterrainer, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Daniela Flores Mosri, Universidad Intercontinental, Mexico
Brian Johnson, Upstate Medical University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Giacolini, Conversi and Alcaro. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Teodosio Giacolini, teodosio.giacolini@uniroma1.it

Download