Skip to main content
Log in

Sensing Happiness in Senseless Information

  • Published:
Applied Research in Quality of Life Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This work develops a theory of happiness from value mechanisms. We assume two opposite states of expectation and information that combine (superpose) to create a mixed state of value. Value’s state probabilistically subsists (by the auto-creative expectational states in uncertainty, i.e., the possibility of many solutions) and collapses into a unique solution (by the auto-destructive informational state, i.e., the disclosure of the solution). Happiness emerges from these collapse moments when expectation reaches its “wanted” novel information as the ultimate reward. Evidence to substantiate this theory comes from value mechanisms in economics, biology, and physics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abrasmson, E. E., & Stinson, S. G. (1977). Boredom and eating in non-obese individuals. Addictive Behaviors, 2(4), 181–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albert, D. Z. (1992). Quantum mechanics and experience. London: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, M. L. (2006). Evolution through genetic exchange. Georgia: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asghari, V., Sanyal, S., Buchwaldt, S., Paterson, A., Jovanovic, V., & Van Tol, H. H. (1995). Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. Journal of Neurochemistry, 65(3), 1157–1165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bardo, M. T., Donohew, R. L., & Harrington, N. G. (1996). Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behaviour. Behavioral and Brain Research, 77, 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baucells, M., & Sarin, R. K. (2007). Satiation in discounted utility. Operations Research, 55(1), 170–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaulieu, J.-M., & Gainetdinov, R. R. (2011). The physiology, signaling, and pharmacology of dopamine receptors. Pharmacological Reviews, 63(1), 182–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, B., Murphy, K., & Tamura, R. (1990). Human capital, fertility and economic growth. Journal of Political Economy, 98, 12–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, D. E. (1974). Evaluating time streams of income. International Journal of Management Science, 2(5), 691–699.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, J., Li, L., Patterson, C., Greenberg, B. D., Murphy, D. L., & Hamer, D. H. (1996). Population and familial association between the D4 dopamine receptor gene and measures of novelty seeking. Nature Genetics, 12(1), 81–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. (2007). The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: The case for incentive salience. Psychopharmacology., 191, 391–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2011). Building a neuroscience of pleasure and well-being. Psychology of Well-Being, 1(3), 1–26.

  • Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28, 309–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bérut, A., Arakelyan, A., Petrosyan, A., Ciliberto, S., Dillenschneider, R., & Lutz, E. (2012). Experimental verification of Landauer’s principle linking information and thermodynamics. Nature, 483, 187–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., & Vessel, E. A. (2006). Perceptual pleasure and the brain: A novel theory explains why the brain craves information and seeks it through the senses. American Scientist, 94(3), 247–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binswanger, M. (2006). Why does income growth fail to make us happier? Searching for the treadmills behind the paradox of happiness. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 35, 366–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blum, K., et al. (2009). Genes and happiness. Gene Therapy and Molecular Biology, 13, 91–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., & Campbell, D. (1971). Hedonic relativism and planning the good society. In M. H. Apley (Ed.), Adaptation-level theory: A symposium (pp. 287–302). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2006). Mental life in the space of reasons. Journal of the Theory of Social Behaviour, 36(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bromberg-Martin, E. S., Matsumoto, M., & Hikosaka, O. (2010). Dopamine in motivational control rewarding, aversive, and alerting. Neuron, 68, 815–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cabanac, M. (1971). Physiological role of pleasure. Science, 173, 1103–1107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2017). Valence and value. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 97, 658–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., et al. (2008). Relative income, happiness and utility: An explanation for the Easterlin paradox and other puzzles. Journal of Economic Literature, 46(1), 95–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Przybeck, T. R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. L. (1996). For better or for worse: The impact of upward social comparisons on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colombo, M., & Wright, W. (2017). Explanatory pluralism: An unrewarding prediction error for free energy theorists. Brain and Cognition, 112, 3–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, P. (1997). It’s boring: Notes on the meanings of boredom in everyday life. Qualitative Sociology, 20, 465–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cranford, J. (2010) Lifeos: Exploring the system that executes DNA (1st Ed.), Mama Yerba Publications, New York.

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren't we happy? Amercian Psychologist, 54(10), 821–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deth, R. C. (2003). Molecular origins of human attention: The dopamine-Folate connection. Massachusetts: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Di Paolo, E. A. (2003). Spike-timing dependent plasticity for evolved robots. Adaptive Behavior, 10(3/4), 243–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the adaptation theory of well-being. American Psychologist, 61(4), 305–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, Y. C., Chi, H. C., Grady, D. L., Morishima, A., Kidd, J. R., Kidd, K. K., et al. (2002). Evidence of positive selection acting at the humandopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A., 99(1), 309–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D'Souza, U. M., Russ, C., Tahir, E., Mill, J., McGuffin, P., Asherson, P. J., & Craig, I. W. (2004). Functional effects of a tandem duplication polymorphism in the 5-prime flanking region of the DRD4 gene. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 691–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dulawa, S. C., Grandy, D. K., Low, M. J., Paulus, M. P., & Geyer, M. A. (1999). Dopamine D4 receptor-knock-out mice exhibit reduced exploration of novel stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 9550–9556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Easterlin, R. A. (1974). In P. A. David & M. W. Reder (Eds.), Does economic growth improve the human lot? Some empirical evidence, in nations and households in economic growth: Essays in honour of Moses Abramowitz. Academic Press: New York and London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., & Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1035–1045.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T., Monti, J. M., & Summerfield, C. (2010). Expectation and surprise determine neural population responses in the ventral visual stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 16601–16608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, S. L., Georgeson, M. A., & Webster, M. A. (2011). Response normalization and blur adaptation: Data and multi-scale model. Journal of Vision, 11(2), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, L. H., et al. (2005). Effects of allocation of attention on habituation to olfactory and visual food stimuli in children. Physiology and Behavior, 84(2), 313–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksson, P., et al. (1998). Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nature Medicine, 4, 1313–1317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eunju, et al. (2015). Religiousness, spirituality, and eudaimonic and hedonic well-being. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 28(2), 132–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, A. H., Pavese, N., Lawrence, A. D., Tai, Y. F., Appel, S., Doder, M., Brooks, D. J., Lees, A. J., & Piccini, P. (2006). Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Annals of Neurology, 59, 852–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fink, J. S., & Smith, G. P. (1980). Mesolimbic and mesocortical dopaminergic neurons are necessary for normal exploratory behavior in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 17, 61–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, M. J., & Claus, E. d. (2006). Anatomy of a decision: Striatoorbitofrontal interactions in reinforcement learning, decision making, and reversal. Psychological Review, 113(2), 300–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friston, K. (2008). Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology, 4(11), e1000211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glimcher, P. W. (2003). Decisions, uncertainty, and the brain: The science of neuroeconomics. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grady, D. L., Chi, H. C., Ding, Y. C., Smith, M., Wang, E., Schuck, S., Flodman, P., Spence, M. A., Swanson, J. M., & Moyzis, R. K. (2003). High prevalence of rare dopamine receptor D4 alleles in children diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 8(5), 536–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Habib, A. M., Okorokov, A. L., Hill, M. N., Bras, J. T., Lee, M. C., Li, S., Gossage, S. J., van Drimmelen, M., Morena, M., Houlden, H., Ramirez, J. D., Bennett, D. L. H., Srivastava, D., & Cox, J. J. (2019). Microdeletion in a FAAH pseudogene identified in a patient with high anandamide concentrations and pain insensitivity. British Journal of Anaesthesia, 123(2), e249–e253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis: Finding modern truth in ancient wisdom. Cambridge: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hameroff, S., & Penrose, R. (2014). Consciousness in the universe – A review of the Orch OR. Theory Physics of Life Reviews, 11(1), 39–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, A. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Martin, N. G. (1994). Testing a model for the genetic structure of personality: A comparison of the personality systems of Cloninger and Eysenck. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 762–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heizenberg, W. (1927) The physical principles of the quantum theory (translated by Carl Eckart and Frank C. Hoyt in 1930), University of ChicagoPress, Illinois.

  • Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herbst, J. J., et al. (2000). Do the dimensions of the temperament and character inventory map a simple genetic architecture? Evidence from molecular genetics and factor analysis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1285–1290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgs, S., & Woodward, M. (2009). Television watching during lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young women. Appetite, 52(1), 39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holt, D. B. (1998). Does cultural capital structure american consumption? Journal of Consumer Research, 25(1), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ikemoto, S., & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 6–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R. F. (1990). Culture shift in advanced industrial society. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Crowley, E. D. (1991). Adolescent substance abuse and leisure boredom. Journal of Leisure Research, 23(3), 260–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iso-Ahola, S. E., & Weissinger, E. (1987). Leisure and boredom. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5(3), 356–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joffily, M., & Coricelli, G. (2013). Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Computational Biology, 9(6), e1003094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshanloo, M. (2012). A comparison of Western and Islamic conceptions of happiness. Journal of Happiness Studies, 14(6), 1857–1874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jovanovic, V., Guan, H. C., & Von Tol, H. H. (1999). Comparative pharmacological and functional analysis of the human dopamine D4.2 and D4.10 receptor variants. Pharmacogenetics, 9(5), 561–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being: Thefoundations of hedonic psychology. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kass, S. J., Vodanovich, S. J., Stanny, C. J., & Taylor, T. M. (2001). Watching the clock: Boredom and vigilance performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 92, 969–976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkup, L. and Frenkel, R. B. (2010). An introduction to uncertainty in measurement, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Kjell, O. (2011). Sustainable well-being: A potential synergy between sustainability and well-being research. Review of General Psychology, 15(3), 255–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluger, A. N., Siegfried, Z., & Ebstein, R. P. (2002). A meta-analysis of the association between DRD4 polymorphism and novelty seeking. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 712–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lachowicz, J. E., & Sibley, D. R. (1997). Molecular characteristics of mammalian dopamine receptors. Pharmacology and Toxicology, 81(3), 105–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, M. W. (1999). Evolutionary consequences of niche construction and their implications for ecology. Proceedings of the Natural Academy of Sciences, 96, 10242–10247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lauritsen, M. B., Pedersen, C. B., & Mortensen, P. B. (2005). Effects of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism: A nationwide register-based study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 963–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Layard, R. (2006). Happiness and public policy: A challenge to the profession. Economic Journal, 116, C24–C33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, W. R., & Robertson, M. L. (1997). Comparative primate energetics and hominid evolution. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 102, 265–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, S. (2001). Ultimate physical limits to computation. Nature, 406, 1047–1054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, L. J., & Butler, P. M. (2011). Novelty-seeking DRD4 polymorphisms are associated with human migration distance out-of-Africa after controlling for neutral population gene structure. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 145, 382–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McSweeney, F. K., & Swindell, S. (1999). General-process theories of motivation revisited: The role of habituation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 437–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montague, P. R., Dayan, P., Person, C., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1995). Bee foraging in uncertain environments using predictive Hebbian learning. Nature, 377, 725–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montague, P. R., Hyman, S. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Computational roles for dopamine in behavioral control. Nature, 431(7010), 760–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrant, J. C. (1984). Boredom in psychiatric practice. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry / La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie, 29(5), 431–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nasser, A. (2012). Can money buy happiness? A macroeconomic multinational perspective on the Easterlin paradox. THURJ, 40(9).

  • Nelson, L. D., Meyvis, T., & Galak, J. (2009). Enhancing the television-viewing experience through commercial interruptions. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 160–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niv, Y., Daw, N. D., & Dayan, P. (2006). Choice values. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 987–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, E. P., Ozkaragoz, T. Z., Ritchie, T. L., Zhang, X., Belin, T. R., & Sparkes, R. S. (1998). D2 and D4 dopamine receptor polymorphisms and personality. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81(1), 257–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • North D. C. (1990) Institutions, institutional change, and economic performance, Cambridge University Press.

  • Oak, J. N., Oldenhof, J., & Van Tol, H. H. M. (2000). The dopamine D4 receptor: One decade of research. European Journal of Pharmacology, 405, 303–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(6), 419–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olshausen, B. A., & Field, D. J. (1996). Emergence of simple-cell receptive fields properties by learning a sparse code for natural images. Nature, 381, 607–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ono, Y., Manki, H., Yoshimura, K., Muramatsu, T., Mizushima, H., Higuchi, S., Yagi, G., Kanba, S., & Asai, M. (1997). Association between dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism and novelty seeking in Japanese subjects. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 74, 501–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pani, L. (2000). Is there an evolutionary mismatch between the normal physiology of the human dopaminergic system and current environmental conditions in industrialized countries? Molecular Psychiatry, 5, 467–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pecina, S. (2008). Opioid reward ‘liking’ and ‘wanting’ in the nucleus accumbens. Physiological Behaviour, 94, 675–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penrose, R. (1996) Shadows of the mind: A search for the missing science of consciousness, Oxford University Press, United Kingdom.

  • Pierce, R. C., Crawford, C. A., Nonneman, A. J., Mattingly, B. A., & Bardo, M. T. (1990). Effect of forebrain dopamine depletion on novelty-induced place preference behavior in rats. Biochemistry and Behavior, 36, 321–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pogue-Geile, M., Ferrell, R., Deka, R., Debski, T., & Manuck, S. (1998). Human novelty-seeking personality traits and dopamine D4 receptor polymorphisms: A twin and genetic association study. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81, 44–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, K. (1957). Aristotle discovers the economy. In K. Polanyi, C. Arensberg, & H. Pearson (Eds.), Trade and market in the early empires (pp. 64–94). Chicago: Henry Regnery.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polard, A. F. (2012) A unified theory of happiness: An east-meets-west approach to fully loving your life, Louisville: Sounds True, USA.

  • Potjans, W., Diesmann, M., & Morrison, A. (2011). An imperfect dopaminergic error signal can drive temporal-difference learning. PLoS Computational Biology, 7(5), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Previc, F. H. (1999). Dopamine and the origins of human intelligence. Brain and Cognition, 41, 299–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redden, J. P., & Galak, J. (2013). The subjective sense of feeling satiated: The role of metacognitions in the construction of satiation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 209–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redish, A. D. (2004). Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science, 306(5703), 1944–1947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reich, T., & Wheeler, S. C. (2016). The good and bad of ambivalence: Desiring ambivalence under outcome uncertainty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(4), 493–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimers, S., Maylor, E. A., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 973–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17(6), 651–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ridley, M. (2013) Genome: Autobiography of a species in 23 chapters, HarperCollins Publishers, Toronto.

  • Robbins, R., McKone, E., & Edwards, M. (2007). Aftereffects for face attributes with different natural variability: Adapter position effects and neural models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(3), 570–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, B. J., Rowe, E. A., Rolls, E. T., Kingston, B., Megson, A., & Gunary, R. (1981). Variety in a meal enhances food intake in man. Physiological Behaviours, 26, 215–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romo, R., & Schultz, W. (1990). Dopamine neurons of the monkey midbrain: Contingencies of responses to active touch during self-initiated arm movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 63, 592–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, M., Cepeda, C., Hurst, R. S., Flores-Hernandez, J., Ariano, M. A., Falzone, T. L., et al. (2001). Dopamine D4 receptor-deficient mice display cortical hyperexcitability. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(11), 3756–3763.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Mingote, S. M., & Weber, S. M. (2005). Beyond the reward hypothesis: Alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 5, 34–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Farrar, A., & Mingote, S. M. (2007). Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology, 191, 461–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schoots, O., & Van Tol, H. H. (2003). The human dopamine D4 receptor repeat sequences modulate expression. Pharmacogenomics Journal, 3(6), 343–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrage, M. (1999). Sixteen tons of information overload. Fortune, 140, 244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80(1), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (2010). Dopamine signals for reward value and risk: Basic and recent data. Behavioural and Brain Functions, 6(24), 2–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (2015). Neuronal reward and decision signals: From theories to data. Physical Review, 95, 853–951.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W., Apicella, P., & Ljungberg, T. (1993). Responses of monkey dopamine neurons to reward and conditioned stimuli during successive steps of learning a delayed response task. Journal of Neuroscience, 13(3), 900–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 1593–1599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seaman, M. I., Fisher, J. B., Chang, F. M., & Kidd, K. K. (1999). Tandem duplication polymorphism upstream of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). American Journal of Medical Genetics, 88, 705–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. London: Nicholas Brealey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanta, B. N. (2015). Life and consciousness – The Vedantic view. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 8(5), e1085138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shenk, D. (1997). Data smog: Surviving the information glut. San Francisco: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimony, A. (1993). Search for a naturalistic world view - volume II, natural science and metaphysics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shohamy, D., & Adcock, A. (2010). Dopamine and adaptive memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 464–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinding, C. (2016) New determinants of olfactory habituation. Scientific Reports, 7.

  • Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stapel, D. A., & Marx, D. M. (2007). Distinctiveness is key: How different types of self-other similarity moderate social comparison effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(3), 439–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steger, M. F., & Frazier, P. (2005). Meaning in life: One link in the chain from religiousness towell-being. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 574–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, B., & Wolfers, J. (2008). Happiness inequality in the United States. Journal of Legal Studies, 37(2), 533–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, J., Deutsch, C., Cantwell, D., Posner, M., Kennedy, J., Barr, C., Moyzis, R., Schuck, S., Flodman, P., & Spence, M. A. (2001). Genes and attention-deficit hyperactvity disorder. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 1(3), 207–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terzis, G. and Arp, R. (2011) Information and living systems: Philosophical and scientific perspectives, MIT Press, Cambridge.

  • Todorovic, A., & De Lange, F. P. (2012). Repetition suppression and expectation suppression are dissociable in time in early auditory evoked fields. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(39), 13389–13395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tonegawa, S., Liu, X., Ramirez, S., & Redondo, R. (2015). Memory engram cells have come of age. Neuron, 87(5), 918–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevorrow, K., & Moore, S. (1998). The association between loneliness, social isolation and women's electronic gaming machine gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(3), 263–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2003). To do or to have? That is the question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6), 1193–1202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Tol, et al. (1992). Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature, 358, 149–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Neumann, J. (1932) Mathematische Grundlagen der Quantenmechanik (mathematical foundations of quantum mechanics) Princeton University press.

  • Walker, E. H. (2000). The physics of consciousness: Quantum minds and the meaning of life. Cambridge: Perseus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, E., Ding, Y. C., Flodman, P., Kidd, J. R., Kidd, K. K., Grady, D. L., Ryder, O. A., Spence, M. A., Swanson, J. M., & Moyzis, R. K. (2004). The genetic architecture of selection at the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4). American Journal of Human Genetics, 74, 931–944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, J. D., & Vodanovich, S. J. (1999). Boredom proneness and psychosocial development. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 133(3), 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. (1982). Community noise problems: Evidence against adaptation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2(2), 87–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, L., Xie, X., & Cleanthous, C. C. (1995). Purpose in life, boredom, and volunteerism in a group of retirees. Psychological Reports, 76(2), 482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White House. (2015) Big data: Seizing opportunities preserving values, Interim progress report, Executive Office of the President, Wasthington, USA.

  • Wigner, E. P. (1967). Remarks on the mind-body question in symmetries and reflections (pp. 171–184). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J., & Dayan, P. (2005). Dopamine, learning, and impulsivity: A biological account of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(2), 160–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, S. N. (2007). How to increase serotonin in the human brain without drugs. Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 32(6), 394–399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1995). Good and bad humours: Biochemical bases of personality and its disorders. Psychological Science, 6(6), 325–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ünsal Özdilek.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

I declare that I have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Özdilek, Ü. Sensing Happiness in Senseless Information. Applied Research Quality Life 16, 2059–2084 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-020-09854-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-020-09854-x

Keywords

Navigation