Next Article in Journal
Astrocyte-Derived TGFβ1 Facilitates Blood–Brain Barrier Function via Non-Canonical Hedgehog Signaling in Brain Microvascular Endothelial Cells
Next Article in Special Issue
Action Video Games Enhance Attentional Control and Phonological Decoding in Children with Developmental Dyslexia
Previous Article in Journal
Activation of Glutamate Transporter-1 (GLT-1) Confers Sex-Dependent Neuroprotection in Brain Ischemia
Previous Article in Special Issue
Are Linguistic Prediction Deficits Characteristic of Adults with Dyslexia?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleep-Related Declarative Memory Consolidation in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Dyslexia

1
Department of Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Biotechnological and Applied Clinical Sciences, University of L’Aquila, 67100 L’Aquila, Italy
3
IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy
4
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Unit, IRCCS Bambino Gesù Children’s Hospital, 00165 Rome, Italy
5
Department of Life Science and Public Health, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, 00153 Rome, Italy
6
Department of Education and Human Sciences, University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, 42121 Reggio Emilia, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(1), 73; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11010073
Submission received: 10 December 2020 / Revised: 31 December 2020 / Accepted: 4 January 2021 / Published: 8 January 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Multiple Neurocognitive Deficits and Dyslexia)

Abstract

:
Sleep has a crucial role in memory processes, and maturational changes in sleep electrophysiology are involved in cognitive development. Albeit both sleep and memory alterations have been observed in Developmental Dyslexia (DD), their relation in this population has been scarcely investigated, particularly concerning topographical aspects. The study aimed to compare sleep topography and associated sleep-related declarative memory consolidation in participants with DD and normal readers (NR). Eleven participants with DD and 18 NR (9–14 years old) underwent a whole-night polysomnography. They were administered a word pair task before and after sleep to assess for declarative memory consolidation. Memory performance and sleep features (macro and microstructural) were compared between the groups, and the intercorrelations between consolidation rate and sleep measures were assessed. DD showed a deeper worsening in memory after sleep compared to NR and reduced slow spindles in occipito-parietal and left fronto-central areas. Our results suggest specific alterations in local sleep EEG (i.e., sleep spindles) and in sleep-dependent memory consolidation processes in DD. We highlight the importance of a topographical approach, which might shed light on potential alteration in regional cortical oscillation dynamics in DD. The latter might represent a target for therapeutic interventions aimed at enhancing cognitive functioning in DD.

1. Introduction

Human sleep plays a crucial role in learning and memory consolidation, through reorganization processes based on neural plasticity [1,2,3]. Specifically, for what concerns declarative memory, the beneficial effect of sleep in the integration of new information has been widely demonstrated in adults [3,4,5,6,7]. Two hallmarks of Non-rapid Eye Movement (NREM) sleep EEG have been recognized to be mainly involved in the consolidation of declarative memory: Slow Wave Activity (SWA) and sleep spindles [3,8,9].
A growing body of evidence suggests that sleep supports cognitive functioning and brain maturation during typical human development, a period characterized by intense plastic changes [9]. The relation between sleep and declarative memory, as well as the role of SWA and sleep spindles, has also been observed in children and adolescents, even if data are more conflicting [9]. Sleep problems also are an increased risk in several neurodevelopmental disorders [10,11,12]. Furthermore, the presence of sleep EEG alterations has been hypothesized to be related to the specific nature of the cognitive and behavioral phenotypes characterizing the different Neurodevelopmental Disorders (NDDs) (for a review see [10,13]). Besides, these alterations have been shown also to have an impact on memory functioning [14,15,16,17,18,19].
According to this general background, it is surprising that only one study [20] has investigated the relation between sleep features and memory consolidation in the most common neurodevelopmental disorder, which is Developmental Dyslexia (DD). This disorder is characterized by unexpected lower reading abilities for the chronological age, in presence of normal IQ and education [21] and its prevalence rates range from 5% to 15% [22]. The most established explanatory hypothesis about the etiology of DD suggests the presence of a phonological deficit as core feature of the disturbance [23]. However, in the last years, an interest towards a more complex definition of the disorder has been growing and recent models support a multiple deficit hypothesis [23,24,25].
Among the different cognitive domains involved, long-term memory seems to play a role in the disorder, even if the findings in this field are exiguous and controversial [26,27]. Concerning declarative memory, early findings showed alternatively the presence [28,29] or alternatively the absence [30] of a visual sequential memory deficit in populations with DD. Similar inconsistencies have been found in the verbal domain, with preserved [31] or impaired [30] long-term verbal abilities in children with DD. In university students with DD, cross-modal long-term memory problems have been reported in binding associations between pairs of phonological and visual features [32]. Similarly, in adults with DD, deficits in long-term memory representations have been documented [33], together with self-reported problems in remembering both facts [34] and episodically experienced events [35].
The assessment of sleep-dependent memory processes may help to better understand learning mechanisms in this population.
Sleep studies in DD are, to date, inconsistent and heterogeneous even if there is a general agreement on the presence of some kinds of alteration. Only one study [36] investigated sleep problems in DD, highlighting an increased incidence in this disturbance compared to normal readers (NR). Regarding sleep architecture, some studies found alterations in children with DD compared to NR [37,38,39], in terms of a decreased REM sleep and stage 3 NREM sleep percentage and an increase of stage 2 in children with DD compared to NR [37,38]. However, the most recent work in this field does not confirm these findings [20]. Moreover, microstructural abnormalities in DD, in particular an increase of sleep spindles and slow-wave activity (SWA), have also been observed [37,38].
Only one study, examining the potential impact of altered sleep on memory processes in DD [20], suggested a reduced role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation processes. While memory performance correlated with the expression of SWA (<1 Hz) in NR, no correlation was found for the group with DD. Moreover, children with DD showed a significant worsening after one week from the learning session, compared to the reading-abilities matched group, composed of NR younger children.
At present, given the well-established knowledge about the local nature of sleep [40,41], the main limitation of the literature about sleep in DD is the absence of data concerning the topographical distribution of its features. The aforementioned studies [20,37,38] did not analyze how the microstructural patterns are distributed on different cortical regions.
Starting from this background, the present study has three main aims: (1) to describe for the first time the topographic EEG changes of Slow-Wave Activity (SWA) and sleep spindles in a group of children and adolescents with DD, (2) to evaluate the sleep-dependent learning of declarative memories in participants with DD, and (3) to investigate the relation between sleep features and memory performance.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty-nine children and adolescents (9–14 years old) were involved in the study: 11 participants with DD (mean age: 11.04 ± 1.67; 5 males) and 18 chronological-age matched NR (mean age: 11.72 ± 1.56; 11 males). Four participants (1 DD and 3 NR) had to be excluded from EEG analyses due to technical difficulties during the polysomnographic (PSG) recording; hence the final sample for EEG analyses included 10 participants with DD and 15 NR.
Participants with DD were recruited at the Child and Adolescents Neuropsychiatric Unit of the Bambino Gesù Children’s Hospital, while NRs were recruited through advertisements in the community.
Before entering the study, all participants were assessed for intelligence, reading abilities, psychiatric and sleep comorbidities by means of a screening test battery.
The absence of intellectual disabilities was assessed through Raven’s Matrices [42] (colored or standard based on age) or Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Fourth Edition (WISC-IV; [43]): none of the participants had an IQ score below 85. For reading abilities, the text reading test (MT-2; [44]) and the word and non-word reading tests (DDE-2; [45]) were administered. In order to exclude the presence of psychiatric comorbidities and sleep problems, the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [46] and the Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children [47] were filled out by parents. All participants were free of any medication for the week preceding the experimental night.
Participants and their parents were informed about the procedure and the aims of the study and gave their written informed consent. At the end of the session, they received a gift. The study has been approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Psychology (#0001266) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Procedure

Participants who fulfilled the inclusion/exclusion criteria required for participation in the study underwent a complete PSG recording of a nocturnal sleep, performed at the Sleep Psychophysiology Laboratory of the Department of Psychology (Sapienza, University of Rome).
During the week preceding the experimental night, participants were asked to fill out sleep diaries every morning within 15 min after awakening in order to monitor the regularity of the sleep-wake cycle. They were asked to avoid naps in the day of the experimental night. Each participant took part in the experimental session alone and each on a different day. Participants arrived at the sleep lab between 17:00 and 19:00 and, at first, performed a declarative memory task (encoding and immediate recall phases), that consists of a Word-Pair Task (WPT). After the application of the PSG, participants had a light dinner. Sleep PSG recordings started between 21:00 and 23:00, according to the habitual bedtime of the participants, and finished after 10 h of sleep or with the spontaneous awakening of the subjects. In the morning, the subjects filled out a sleep diary and after 45 min from the awakening the WPT was newly administered (delayed recall phase) (please refer to the Measures Section for more details). Each session lasted about 15 h. Data recordings were conducted on weekends or free-school days. Environmental influences that could disturb sleep (light, noise, temperature) were controlled.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Declarative Memory Word-Pair Task

To test the declarative memory consolidation, a WPT modified ad hoc for the present study was used. It consisted of a list of 34 semantically unrelated word pairs that were presented out loud in a randomized order. The auditory version of the task [48,49], instead of the more classical visual one, was chosen and constructed with Psychtoolbox 3 for Matlab R2014a (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA), in order to limit potential encoding difficulties for the participants with DD. The first and the last two word-pairs of the list served to buffer primacy and recency effects and were excluded from the calculation of the performance.
Words were selected from the “Lexvar” Italian database of child language [50]. They had to be bi- or three-syllabic and characterized by high imaginability and concreteness and by a low emotional connotation. To control for mnemonic strategies, participants were instructed to visually imagine a relation between the two randomly related words (e.g., for “cake-river” one could imagine a cake floating on a river) [51]. In the evening, the entire list was presented (encoding session). The time interval between the two words of each pair (intra-stimulus interval) had a 1-s duration, while the interstimulus interval (between one pair and the following) lasted 5 s. The presentation of the list was immediately followed by the cued recall session (immediate recall). In this phase, only the first word of each pair was presented, and subjects were asked to name the second word out loud within 15 s. The experimenter gave feedback to the participants, in terms of right or wrong answer. The list was presented repeatedly, at most four times (or trials), until the subjects remembered at least 50% of the word-pairs (items). The delayed recall session was performed in the morning, 45 min after lights on.
The overnight memory consolidation was calculated as the number of correctly retrieved words at the morning recall referred to the number of correctly retrieved words at the last repetition of evening recall (consolidation rate) [52].
The other WPT parameters considered for evaluating the memory performance were the following:
  • number of trials (repetition of the list) needed to achieve the cutoff (50% of pairs recalled);
  • number of correctly retrieved word-pairs in the last trial in the evening (immediate recall);
  • number of correctly retrieved word-pairs in the morning (delayed recall).

2.3.2. Polysomnographic Recordings

The PSG recordings were acquired in a sound-proof, temperature-controlled room, with a Brain Amp MR plus system (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Munich, Germany).
The 28 unipolar EEG derivations of the international 10-10 system (C3, C4, Cp1, Cp2, Cp5, Cp6, Cz, F3, F4, F7, F8, Fc1, Fc2, Fc5, Fc6, Fp1, Fp2, Fz, O1, O2, Oz, P3, P4, P7, P8, Pz, T7, T8) were recorded from scalp electrodes (Ag/AgCl) with averaged mastoid references (A1 and A2). EEG signals were acquired with a sampling rate of 250 Hz and bandpass filtered at 30 Hz. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. The bipolar electrooculogram (EOG) electrodes were placed about 1 cm from the lateral canthi up for the right eye and down for the left eye and were recorded with a time constant of 1 s. Submental electromyogram (EMG) was recorded with a time constant of 0.03 s. EEG data were digitally stored for further offline analyses.

2.4. Data Analyses

2.4.1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

All data were analyzed using Matlab 2011b (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Demographic, cognitive and clinical characteristics (Age, IQ, reading abilities, CBCL, and Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC) scores) were compared between the two groups by means of two-tailed t-tests for independent samples. Alpha level was set at 0.05.

2.4.2. Declarative Memory Performance

Regarding memory performance, the encoding variables (number of trials and number of recalled word-pairs in the evening) were compared between the groups to control for potential performance differences at the baseline. The crucial WPT variable (consolidation rate) was calculated as the number of correctly retrieved words at the morning recall as a function of the number of correctly retrieved words at the last repetition of evening recall. It was compared between the two groups by means of two-tailed unpaired t-tests. Alpha level was set at 0.05.

2.4.3. Sleep Measures

Sleep stages were visually scored in 20 s epochs, according to Rechtschaffen and Kales’ criteria [53], excluding ocular, muscle, and electrical artifacts. The following macrostructural variables were calculated: stage 1, stage 2 and Slow Wave Sleep (SWS) latency (min); stage 1, stage 2, SWS, REM and NREM (stage 2 + SWS) duration (min); Total Sleep Time (TST, min), given by the sum of stage 1, stage 2, SWS and REM duration; Total Bed Time (TBT); Wake After Sleep Onset (WASO); number of Movement Arousal (MA); Sleep Onset Latency (SOL); Sleep Efficiency Index (SEI) as the ratio between TST and TBT. Differences between the two groups were computed for all the macrostructural variables by means of two-tailed unpaired t-tests. Alpha level was set at 0.05.

2.4.4. Slow-Wave Activity (SWA)

For the whole-night NREM sleep, the power spectra for the 28 derivations were computed by Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) for 20 s epochs (periodogram: 4 s), resulting in a frequency resolution of 0.25 Hz. As total spectral power may vary considerably between subjects, EEG power values were normalized for total power and expressed by relative spectral power measures. For each scalp derivation, relative power values for adjacent frequency bins were summed together to obtain the delta frequency band (0.5–4.75 Hz). Differences between groups were computed by means of two-tailed unpaired t-tests for the 28 cortical channels. To correct for multiple comparisons the False Discovery Rate (FDR) was applied [54].

2.4.5. Spindle Detection and Analysis

Spindle detection was performed by means of a customized algorithm in Matlab [55,56,57,58,59]. NREM epochs were bandpass-filtered between 11 and 15 Hz (−3 dB at 10 and 16 Hz) using a Chebyshev Type II filter. The detection of a spindle occurred when the mean signal amplitude of each channel exceeded an upper threshold set at six times the mean single channel amplitude. The local amplitude maximum above the upper threshold was considered as the peak amplitude of the single spindle. The points at which the amplitude fell below a lower threshold (two times the mean amplitude of each channel) occurring at least 0.25 s from the peak were considered as the beginning and the end of the spindle (maximum duration: 1.5 s). Spindles falling within the 11–13 Hz frequency range were considered as “slow,” while those falling in the 13–15 Hz range were considered as “fast.” For each participant, the raw number of spindles was calculated for each electrode, separately for fast and slow spindles and jointly for the whole frequency range. Subsequently, spindle density was calculated as the number of spindles divided by artifact-free NREM (stages 2, 3, and 4) sleep minutes.
Differences between groups for spindle density were computed by means of two-tailed unpaired t-tests for the 28 cortical channels. To correct for multiple comparisons the FDR was applied.

2.4.6. Correlation between Sleep Measures and Memory Performance

The relations between all the macrostructural variables and the overnight memory consolidation rate were computed by means of Spearman’s correlations. The same analysis was computed for SWA and spindle density for the whole topography (28 cortical derivations) and corrected by means of FDR.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Means, standard deviations and between-group comparisons (t-tests) for demographic and clinical characteristics of the groups are reported in Table 1. The two groups did not differ significantly in age (t1,27 = −1.13; p = 0.269), IQ (t1,27 = 0.14; p = 0.9) or psychiatric comorbidities (internalizing problems: t1,27 = −0.25; p = 0.81; externalizing problems: t1,27 = −0.07; p = 0.95; total problems: t1,27 = 0.05; p = 0.96). As expected, children and adolescents with DD had worse reading abilities than NR in each reading measure considered (MT-2 text reading speed: t1,27 = −5.9, p < 0.001; MT-2 text reading errors, t1,27 = 5.7, p < 0.001; DDE-2 word reading speed: t1,27 = 5.9, p < 0.001; DDE-2 word reading errors: t1,27 = 3.97, p ≤ 0.001; DDE-2 non-word reading speed: t1,27 = 5.7, p < 0.001; DDE-2 non-word reading errors, t1,27 = 5.72, p ≤ 0.001). The two groups were homogeneous regarding the incidence of parent-reported sleep problems (see Table 2). According to the exclusion/inclusion criteria, the two groups were free of psychopathological or sleep problems, assessed through parent-report questionnaires.

3.2. Word Pair Task

Measures of memory performance are listed in Table 3. Participants with DD and NR did not differ in the numbers of trials to criterion nor in the final number of correctly encoded word-pairs in the evening. On the other hand, the group with DD showed a significantly lower sleep-dependent consolidation rate (t1,27 = −2.501; p = 0.019) and a significantly lower number of recalled word pairs in the morning (t1,27 = −2.125; p = 0.043) compared to the NR.

3.3. Sleep Macrostructure

Table 4 reports the sleep macrostructural variables in the participants with DD and NR and the results of the between-group statistical comparisons (t-tests). No significant differences were found between the two groups for any variable.

3.4. SWA

The topographical distribution of mean SWA during NREM sleep for the two groups is reported in Figure 1A. As expected, SWA is predominantly located in the fronto-central areas in both groups. Statistical comparisons computed by means of unpaired t-tests (Figure 1B) failed to reach significance after FDR corrections, although there was a trend in the direction of an increased SWA in the group with DD at the Fc5 (t1,23 = 1.97 p = 0.06) derivation and a decreased SWA at the Cp2 derivation (t1,23 = −2.76 p = 0.01).

3.5. Sleep Spindles

Figure 2A shows the topographical distribution of mean sleep spindle density in the two groups and the relative statistical comparisons. The topographical distribution of both fast and slow spindles is consistent with the pre-existing knowledge on this topic [60,61,62] with slow spindles exhibiting their maxima in the frontal areas, while fast spindles peak on more posterior derivations, predominantly central.
After FDR correction, the t-tests showed the absence of significant differences between the two groups for spindle density in the whole frequency range and the fast spindles range (Figure 2B). On the other hand, slow spindle density was significantly lower (FDR q ≤ 0.043; p ≤ 0.0077 corresponds to a t ≥ 2.92 in the group with DD compared with NR at Fc1 (t1,23 = −3.06; p = 0.0056), O1 (t1,23 = −3.39; p = 0.002), Oz (t1,23 = −2.92; p = 0.0077), P7 (t1,23 = −3.00; p = 0.006) e P8 (t1,23 = −3.14; p = 0.0046).

3.6. Correlations between Memory Performance and Sleep Measures

3.6.1. Macrostructural Parameters x Memory Performance

No correlations between memory consolidation rate and sleep macrostructural parameters were significant in the group with DD or in NR (Table 5).

3.6.2. SWA and Memory Performance

Figure 3 shows the topographical distributions of correlation coefficients (Rho values) between WPT consolidation rate and SWA of NREM sleep. There were no significant correlations that survived FDR correction, even if some trends can be observed. In particular, in NR there is a positive correlation at F3 (Rho = 0.60; p = 0.02), while a negative one is observed at P4 (Rho = −0.70; p = 0.003). In the group with DD no correlation was statistically significant.

3.6.3. Sleep Spindle Density and Memory Performance

Figure 4 reports the topographical distributions of the correlation coefficients (Rho values) between spindle (all, fast and slow) density and memory consolidation, separately for the two groups. No significant correlation was found. However, the topographical distribution of the correlational patterns should be noted: in the group with DD negative correlation coefficients are localized mainly in the anterior areas and the positive coefficients in the posterior ones, while in the NR group the correlations have a positive direction diffusely on the scalp.

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the current study is the first to investigate sleep-dependent declarative memory consolidation in children and adolescents with DD considering the topographical aspects of NREM EEG hallmarks. According to our results, participants with DD exhibit a significant worsening in declarative memory performance after a night of sleep compared to age-matched NR, starting from similar baseline levels. Regarding sleep, the differences between the two groups are observable only considering the microstructural features and their topographical distribution, in terms of a net decrease of slow sleep spindles in children and adolescents with DD compared to NR, particularly in the posterior areas. Sleep quality and macrostructural characteristics do not differ between the groups.

4.1. Sleep-Dependent Memory Performance

The group with DD shows a reduced consolidation rate at the WPT, compared to NR, starting from a baseline level (number of recalled word-pairs in the evening and number of trials to the criterion) similar between the groups. The absence of differences at the encoding phase seems to confirm the appropriateness of the adjustments applied to the classical WPT (i.e., auditory version). The worsening in the performance appears at the morning recall and we hypothesize that it could be linked to peculiar sleep processes involved in memory consolidation.
Previous studies that investigated long-term declarative memory in DD provided conflicting findings. Some authors showed impaired long-term declarative memory abilities [29,32,33,34] reporting widespread deficits (e.g., [29]), while others failed in showing any difference between individuals with DD and controls [26,30,31].
However, the aforementioned studies did not analyze the potential role of sleep in this process. Only Smith et al. (2017) [20] evaluated sleep-dependent declarative memory consolidation in DD, through the administration of a novel word learning task, suggesting a less active role of sleep in this process in preadolescents with DD compared to NR. Looking at the absence of differences at the baseline performance between participants with DD and NR, our results are partially consistent with that finding. We hypothesize that DD might be characterized by an alteration during the sleep-dependent consolidation processes more than by a declarative memory deficit per se. Nevertheless, the involvement of sleep in the observed deficit cannot be confirmed, as our study lacks a parallel protocol with a wake- instead of a sleep-interval between the immediate and the delayed recall.

4.2. Sleep Problems and Macrostructure

The occurrence of sleep problems, parent-reported through the SDSC questionnaire, is similar between the two groups. The only previous study [36] that investigated this aspect showed an increased sleep problem percentage in children with DD compared to controls. Although the involved sample was very large, participants were not assessed for psychopathological comorbidities, very often found in DD [63] and typically associated with sleep problems [64].
Concerning sleep architecture, we have not found any difference between the groups. Earlier findings are heterogeneous. A first study in this field [39] found changes in sleep architecture in DD. However, these previous results are scarcely comparable to the present findings (e.g., the mean IQ in the DD group was lower in the study by Mercier et al. than in our study). More recently, Bruni et al. (2009a; 2009b) [37,38] showed a decrease in SWS and REM sleep percentage, along with an increase of stage 2. On the other hand, the results of Smith et al. (2017) [20] are in line with our findings. In our opinion, the different age range might explain this heterogeneity, since sleep architecture undergoes large modifications during development [9]. While Smith et al. (2017) [20] studied a sample comparable to ours in terms of age (8–13 years), in the studies of Bruni et al. (2009a,b) [37,38] the age range was widespread (8–16).
To summarize, we hypothesize that the sleep disturbances and the alterations of sleep quality and macrostructure previously found in populations with dyslexia might not be part of the phenotype per se but might be due to the presence of psychopathological comorbidities [36,39] and/or to methodological issues [37,38].

4.3. Cortical Sleep Topography

The present study is the first that analyzed the topographical distribution (on 28 cortical channels) of sleep spindles and SWA in DD. Our main finding is a significant decrease of bilateral occipito-parietal and left fronto-central slow spindles in the group with DD compared to NR.
A decrease of slow spindles in DD is shown for the first time. Bruni et al. (2009a) [37] reported an increased 11–12 Hz slow EEG activity, which mostly corresponds to the frequency range of slow spindles, and an increased spindle density limited to stage 2 and measured over a single central derivation, both positively related to the degree of reading difficulties. The authors interpreted these patterns in terms of an enhanced role of spindles in the transfer of information between the damaged areas of the posterior left hemisphere and the posterior and anterior vicariant areas, localized in the right hemisphere. Nevertheless, this latter point might appear counterintuitive in light of the knowledge about the role of sleep spindles in the enhancement of cognitive performance [65]. From a topographic point of view, Bruni et al. (2009a) [37] investigated spindle characteristics only on the channel Cz, while in the present work, the main difference between the groups has been found in the posterior areas. Moreover, the authors performed a visual and not an automatic spindle detection, without differentiating between slow and fast spindles. On the other hand, Smith et al. (2017) [20] did not find differences in spindle density between participants with DD and NR. In this case, density was calculated for central and frontal derivations and subsequently averaged across the considered channels, so that no topographical information was provided. Moreover, they did not distinguish between slow and fast spindles and considered a smaller frequency range (12–15 Hz).
To sum up, specific changes of sleep spindle activity in DD might become observable only when considering the topographical distribution and distinguishing between the subtypes, aspects that have been introduced for the first time in our study. This evidence might open the way to a new approach to the study of sleep-related aspects in NDDs, aimed to highlight the local features of EEG activities. This is in line with the local sleep theory [40,41], which claims that several regional EEG phenomena during sleep are expressed proportionally to the brain activity during the previous wake period with a frequency- and topographic-specificity.
The specific functional role of centro-parietal fast and frontal slow spindles is still debated but what is agreed is their positive correlation with cognitive functioning and, specifically, memory consolidation [7,65]. However, a possible distinction in the functional role of the two subtypes during healthy development has yet to be clarified [9]. At a network level, it has been suggested that fast spindles are related to thalamo-cortical couplings, whereas slow spindles are related to cortico-cortical couplings [66]. Starting from this assumption, the reduction in slow spindle density, observed in our study, might reflect the disconnection deficit that has been hypothesized to be at the base of DD [67,68]. A potential cause of this reduced connectivity might be an alteration of white matter. One of the first works in the field [69] showed white matter alterations in subjects with DD compared to NR in bilateral temporo-parietal areas, which are involved in the connection between anterior and posterior regions. Moreover, the integrity of white matter pathways in the left temporo-parietal area has been related to reading abilities (e.g., [70]). Finally, individual differences in sleep spindles might depend at least in part on the structural properties of white matter tracts [71]. According to this view, the posterior reduction of slow spindles in participants with DD observed in our study may be the electrophysiological marker during sleep of the white matter alteration in this population. Future studies on structural and functional connectivity during sleep should directly assess this issue.
Concerning SWA, albeit our findings do not reach statistical significance, they suggest a clear antero-posterior dissociation, showing a trend of increased fronto-central SWA paralleled by a posterior decrease in DD, compared to NR. The anterior delta enhancement is consistent with the central increased SWA observed by Bruni et al. (2009a) [37], extending their finding to several anterior cortical sites. On the other hand, SWA in the posterior regions has never been investigated in DD. Interestingly, individuals with DD compared to NRs show a hypoactivation of two brain reading systems, localized in the parieto-temporal and the occipito-temporal regions of the left hemisphere, and some evidence highlights a concurrent hyperactivation of vicariant systems, in the frontal area bilaterally and the right hemisphere [72,73]. Starting from these findings, the hypothesis that the differences in SWA observed in our study may mirror the functional abnormalities observed in neuroimaging studies represents an intriguing speculation and should be directly investigated.
A limitation of the present study is the absence of an EEG-recorded baseline night without the administration of a cognitive task before sleep. As stated before, the processing of new information that takes place during sleep involves sleep spindles and SWA. The observed differences between the groups in sleep spindles and SWA topographical patterns might be explained by different memory consolidation processes, as suggested by the memory performance data (this point will be further discussed in the next session). This implies that the generalizability of our results on the brain topography should be taken with caution, since some changes may be related to the administration of the declarative task before going to sleep. Future studies should investigate baseline sleep microstructure in this population with a topographical approach.

4.4. Relations between Sleep Features and Memory Performance

Macrostructural variables are not significantly related to the WPT performance. This is not surprising as the relation between this kind of sleep parameter and declarative memory consolidation in developmental age is not well-established [9,74].
On the other hand, the correlations between NREM hallmarks and memory performance, albeit not significant, are completely original. Indeed, the correlational patterns are strongly differentiated between the two groups.
Regarding SWA, the coefficients are positive in the left frontal region and negative in the right posterior areas for the NR, while in the group with DD the negative coefficients are located only on the left hemisphere and the positive ones are shifted more centrally and on the left. This might mean that cortical areas involved in sleep-dependent memory consolidation are differentiated between the two groups. The topographical dissociation could be connected to the functional alteration of the left hemisphere networks in DD [72] and/or to the application of unalike mnemonic strategies, that would mirror themselves at a local level during sleep. On the other hand, the stronger correlations in the NR might reflect a more passive role of sleep in the group with DD, consistently with the hypothesis of Smith et al. (2017) [20], who showed significant correlations between memory consolidation and SWA in NR but not in preadolescents with DD.
In the NR the relations between spindle density and memory performance are generally positive, partially consistent with the already existent knowledge about the relation between spindle and memory consolidation, even if in children the evidence is quite heterogeneous [51,62]. On the contrary, the correlational pattern appears dissociated in the group with DD, with positive coefficients located on the posterior areas, concurrently with a negative direction of the effects on frontal areas. Therefore, spindle activity might in this case interfere with memory consolidation processes. Smith et al. (2017) [20] found a moderate correlation (not statistically significant) between power spectra in the spindle frequency range and the performance in the recall phase of a declarative memory task in the control group but not in participants with DD. Albeit our results are scarcely comparable with those of Smith and coworkers (2017) [20] (i.e., different tasks; absence of topographical information), taken together they point to the need for a further effort in the assessment of the relation between cognitive functioning and sleep spindles in DD, particularly considering its regional aspects. In light of this aim future studies might focus on brain oscillation dynamics and connectivity by means of pattern recognition methods capable to modelling several EEG data in parallel, with a concomitant evaluation of local and global dynamics (e.g., [75]).

5. Conclusions

Our results represent the first exhaustive description of the sleep EEG topography in DD after declarative learning, showing a worse sleep-dependent memory consolidation and reduced posterior slow spindles activity. Even if in the present study memory performance and sleep EEG were not found to be directly associated, the observed topographical patterns of correlational trends are promising and need further exploration in the near future. However, several methodological choices may have affected our results. The main limitation of the present study is the absence of a baseline night, due to the difficulties in the recruitment of the sample for age and inclusion/exclusion criteria issues. The latter aspect was particularly critical in recruiting participants with DD for the frequent contemporary presence of reading deficits and psychopathological problems. Beyond considering a baseline night, future research should include larger samples of participants to differentiate groups of DD with distinct characteristics.
Our results suggest that only a topographical approach might shed light on potential alteration in cortical oscillation dynamics in NDDs and, consequently, a global view of the sleep phenomenon appears not particularly informative in this field. Functional and structural brain alterations in learning disabilities might be paralleled by localized alteration of sleep microstructural features that become clearly observable only at a local level during sleep. On this basis, sleep EEG oscillations might represent a target for therapeutic interventions aimed at enhancing cognitive (in this case memory) functioning in DD, in the vein of the already consolidated non-invasive brain stimulation interventions [76,77,78,79], potentially effective in improving reading abilities in DD.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.D.G., G.S., S.V., D.M., F.R., S.S.; methodology, F.R., L.D.G., S.S., A.D., M.G., D.M.; software, A.D.; validation, L.D.G., M.G.; formal analysis, F.R., A.D., M.G.; investigation, F.R.; M.C.; E.D.C. resources, L.D.G., S.V.; data curation, F.R., A.D., M.G., S.S., M.C.; E.D.C., L.D.G.; writing—original draft preparation, F.R., M.G.; writing—review and editing, M.G., F.R., L.D.G., D.M., S.V., G.S.; visualization, F.R., M.G.; supervision, L.D.G., S.V., D.M., G.S.; project administration, L.D.G.; funding acquisition, L.D.G., G.S., F.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Sapienza University of Rome, “Avvio alla Ricerca 2017” (grant number: AR11715C54F9A316) to F.R., and by the Department of Education and Human Sciences University of Modena and Reggio Emilia, Italy.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study has been approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Department of Psychology (#0001266) and was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Informed Consent Statement

Participants and their parents were informed about the procedure and the aims of the study and gave their written informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Emiliano Pes and to the Electronics Laboratory of Sapienza University of Rome for their contributions to the task development. A special thank goes to all participants and their parents.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Tononi, G.; Cirelli, C. Sleep and the price of plasticity: From synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 2014, 81, 12–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Gorgoni, M.; D’Atri, A.; Lauri, G.; Rossini, P.M.; Ferlazzo, F.; De Gennaro, L. Is sleep essential for neural plasticity in humans, and how does it affect motor and cognitive recovery? Neural Plast. 2013, 103949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Rasch, B.; Born, J. About Sleep’s Role in Memory. Physiol. Rev. 2013, 93, 681–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Walker, M.P.; Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Neuron 2004, 44, 121–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Stickgold, R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature 2005, 437, 1272–1278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Gais, S.; Albouy, G.; Boly, M.; Dang-Vu, T.T.; Darsaud, A.; Desseilles, M.; Rauchs, G.; Schanus, M.; Sterpenich, V.; Vandewalle, G.; et al. Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 18778–18783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Diekelmann, S.; Born, J. The memory function of sleep. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 114–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Tononi, G.; Cirelli, C. Sleep and synaptic down-selection. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 51, 413–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Gorgoni, M.; D’Atri, A.; Scarpelli, S.; Reda, F.; De Gennaro, L. Sleep electroencephalography and brain maturation: Developmental trajectories and the relation with cognitive functioning. Sleep Med. 2020, 66, 33–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Angrimann, M.; Caravale, B.; Novelli, L.; Ferri, R.; Bruni, O. Sleep in children with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Neuropediatrics 2015, 46, 199–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Doran, S.M.; Harvey, M.T.; Horner, R.H. Sleep and developmental disabilities: Assessment, treatment and outcome measures. Ment. Retard. 2006, 44, 13–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Scarpelli, S.; Bartolacci, C.; D’Atri, A.; Gorgoni, M.; De Gennaro, L. The functional role of dreaming in emotional processes. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Gorgoni, M.; Scarpelli, S.; Reda, F.; De Gennaro, L. Sleep EEG oscillations in neurodevelopmental disorders without intellectual disabilities. Sleep Med. Rev. 2020, 49, 101224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Ashworth, A.; Hill, C.M.; Karmiloff-Smith, A.; Dimitriou, D. Sleep enhances memory consolidation in children. J. Sleep Res. 2014, 23, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Urbain, C.; Galer, S.; Van Bogaert, P.; Peigneux, P. Pathophysiology of sleep-dependent memory consolidation processes in children. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 89, 273–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Novelli, L.; Ferri, R.; Bruni, O. Sleep cyclic alternating pattern and cognition in children: A review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 89, 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gruber, R.; Wise, M.S. Sleep spindle characteristics in children with neurodevelopmental disorders and their relation to cognition. Neural Plast. 2016, 2016, 4724792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Maski, K.; Holbrook, H.; Manoach, D.; Hanson, E.; Kapur, K.; Stickgold, R. Sleep dependent memory consolidation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Sleep 2015, 38, 1955–1963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Prehn-Kristensen, A.; Munz, M.; Molzow, I.; Wilhelm, I.; Wiesner, C.D.; Baving, L. Sleep promotes consolidation of emotional memory in healthy children but not in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65098. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Smith, F.R.H.; Gaskell, M.G.; Weighall, A.R.; Warmington, M.; Reid, A.M.; Henderson, L.M. Consolidation of vocabulary is associated with sleep in typically developing children, but not in children with dyslexia. Dev. Sci. 2017, 21, e12639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  22. Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 2012, 379, 1997–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Peterson, R.L.; Pennington, B.F. Developmental dyslexia. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2015, 11, 283–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Menghini, D.; Finzi, A.; Benassi, M.; Bolzani, R.; Facoetti, A.; Giovagnoli, S.; Ruffino, M.; Vicari, S. Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: A comparative study. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 863–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fletcher, J.M.; Grigorenko, E.L. Neurpsychology of learning disabilities: The past and the future. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2017, 23, 930–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. West, G.; Clayton, F.J.; Shankes, D.R.; Hume, C. Procedural and declarative learning in dyslexia. Dyslexia 2019, 25, 246–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Conway, C.M.; Arciuli, J.; Lum, J.A.G.; Ullman, M.T. Seeing problems that may not exist: A reply to West et al.’s (2018) questioning of the procedural deficit hypothesis. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Watson, C.; Willows, D.M. Information-processing patterns in specific reading disability. J. Lear. Disabil. 1995, 28, 216–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Menghini, D.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Marotta, L.; Finzi, A.; Vicari, S. Developmental dyslexia and explicit long-term memory. Dyslexia 2010, 16, 213–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bell, T.K. Rapid sequential processing in dyslexic and ordinary readers. Percept. Mot. Skills 1990, 71, 1155–1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kibby, M.; Cohen, M. Memory functioning in children with reading disabilities and/or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A clinical investigation of their working memory and long-term memory functioning. Child. Neuropsychol. 2008, 14, 525–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Jones, M.W.; Ashby, J.; Branigan, H.P. Dyslexia and fluency: Parafoveal and foveal influences on rapid automatized naming. J. Exp. Psychol. 2013, 39, 554–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Smith-Spark, J.H.; Moore, V. The representation and processing of familiar faces in dyslexia: Differences in age of acquisition effects. Dyslexia 2009, 15, 129–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Mortimore, T.; Crozier, W.R. Dyslexia and difficulties with study skills in higher education. Stud. High. Edu. 2006, 31, 235–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Smith-Spark, J.H.; Zięcik, A.P.; Sterling, C. Self-reports of increased prospective and retrospective memory problems in adults with developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 2016, 22, 245–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Carotenuto, M.; Esposito, M.; Cortese, S.; Laino, D.; Verrotti, A. Children with developmental dyslexia showed greater sleep disturbances than controls, including problems initiating and maintaining sleep. Acta Pediatr. 2016, 105, 1079–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Bruni, O.; Ferri, R.; Novelli, L.; Terribili, M.; Troianello, M.; Finotti, E.; Leuzzi, V.; Curatolo, P. Sleep spindle activity is correlated with reading abilities in developmental dyslexia. Sleep 2009, 32, 1333–1340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Bruni, O.; Ferri, R.; Novelli, L.; Finotti, E.; Terribili, M.; Troianello, M.; Valente, D.; Sabatello, U.; Curatolo, P. Slow EEG amplitude oscillations during NREM sleep and reading disabilities in children with dyslexia. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2009, 34, 539–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Mercier, L.; Pivik, R.T.; Busby, K. Sleep patterns in reading disabled children. Sleep 1993, 16, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Siclari, F.; Tononi, G. Local aspects of sleep and wakefulness. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2017, 44, 222–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L. Going Local: Insights from EEG and Stereo-EEG Studies of the Human Sleep-Wake Cycle. Curr. Top. Med. Chem. 2011, 11, 2423–2437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Raven, J.; Raven, J.C.; Court, J.H. Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales; Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wechsler, D. WISC-IV Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Quarta Edizione. Manuale di Somministrazione e Scoring; Giunti OS: Firenze, Italy, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  44. Cornoldi, C.; Colpo, G. Prove di Lettura MT per la Scuola Elementare-2; Edizioni OS: Firenze, Italy, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  45. Sartori, G.; Job, R.; Tressoldi, P.E. DDE-2. Batteria per la Valutazione Della Dislessia e Della Disortografia Evolutiva; Edizioni OS: Firenze, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  46. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L.A. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles; University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  47. Bruni, O.; Ottaviano, S.; Guidetti, V.; Romoli, M.; Innocenzi, M.; Cortesi, F.; Giannotti, F. The Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC) Construct ion and validation of an instrument to evaluate sleep disturbances in childhood and adolescence. J. Sleep Res. 1996, 5, 251–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Wilhelm, I.; Diekelmann, S.; Born, J. Sleep in children improves memory performance on declarative but not procedural tasks. Learn. Mem. 2008, 15, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Potkin, P.T.; Bunney, W.E., Jr. Sleep improves memory: The effect of sleep on long term memory in early adolescence. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e42191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Barca, L.; Burani, C.; Arduino, L.S. Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behav. Res. Methods 2002, 34, 424–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Hoedlmoser, K.; Heib, D.P.J.; Roell, J.; Peigneux, P.; Sadeh, A.; Gruber, G.; Schabus, M. Slow sleep spindle activity, declarative memory, and general cognitive abilities in children. Sleep 2014, 37, 1501–1513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Holz, J.; Piosczyk, H.; Landmann, N.; Feige, B.; Spiegelhalder, K.; Riemann, D.; Nissen, C.; Voderhlzer, U. The timing of learning before night-time sleep differentially affects declarative and procedural long-term memory consolidation in adolescents. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e40963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Rechtschaffen, A.; Kales, A. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System for Sleep Stages of Human Subjects; UCLA Brain Information Service: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1968. [Google Scholar]
  54. Storey, J.D.; Taylor, J.E.; Siegmund, D. Strong control, conservative point estimation, and simultaneous conservative consistency of false discovery rates: A unified approach. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Stat. Methodol. 2004, 66, 187–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Ferrarelli, F.; Peterson, M.J.; Sarasso, S.; Riedner, B.A.; Murphy, M.J.; Benca, R.M.; Bria, P.; Kalin, N.H.; Tononi, G. Thalamic dysfunction in schizophrenia suggested by whole-night deficits in slow and fast spindles. Am. J. Psychiatry 2010, 167, 1339–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Sarasso, S.; Proserpio, P.; Pigorini, A.; Moroni, F.; Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L.; De Carli, F.; Lo Russo, G.; Massimini, M.; Nobili, L. Hippocampal sleep spindles preceding neocortical sleep onset in humans. NeuroImage 2014, 86, 425–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gorgoni, M.; Lauri, G.; Truglia, I.; Cordone, S.; Sarasso, S.; Scarpelli, S.; Mangiaruga, A.; D’Atri, A.; Tempesta, D.; Ferrara, M.; et al. Parietal fast sleep spindle density decrease in Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Neural Plast. 2016, 2016, 8376108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. D’Atri, A.; Novelli, L.; Ferrara, M.; Bruni, O.; De Gennaro, L. Different maturational changes of fast and slow sleep spindles in the first four years of life. Sleep Med. 2018, 42, 73–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Alfonsi., V.; D’Atri, A.; Gorgoni, M.; Scarpelli, S.; Mangiaruga, A.; Ferrara, M.; De Gennaro, L. Spatiotemporal dynamics of sleep spindle sources across NREM sleep cycles. Front. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. De Gennaro, L.; Ferrara, M. Sleep spindles: An overview. Sleep Med. Rev. 2003, 7, 423–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Schabus, M.; Dang-Vu, T.T.; Albouy, G.; Balteau, E.; Boly, M.; Carrier, J.; Darsaud, A.; Degueldre, C.; Dessilles, M.; Gais, S.; et al. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 13164–13169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Hahn, M.; Joechner, A.K.; Roell, J.; Schabus, M.; Heib, D.P.; Gruber, G.; Peigneux, P.; Hoedlmoser, K. Developmental changes of sleep spindles and their impact on sleep-dependent memory consolidation and general cognitive abilities: A longitudinal approach. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Willcutt, E.G.; Pennington, B.F. Comorbidity of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Differences by gender and subtype. J. Learn. Disabil. 2000, 33, 179–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Baglioni, C.; Nanovska, S.; Regen, W.; Spielgelhalder, K.; Feige, B.; Nissen, C.; Reynolds, C.F., III; Riemann, D. Sleep and mental disorders: A meta-analysis of polysomnographic research. Pyschol. Bull. 2016, 142, 969–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Fogel, S.M.; Smith, C.T. The function of the sleep spindle: A physiological index of intelligence and a mechanism for sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011, 35, 1154–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Doran, S. The dynamic topography of individual sleep spindles. Sleep Res. Online 2003, 5, 133–139. [Google Scholar]
  67. Horwitz, B.; Rumsey, J.M.; Donohue, B.C. Functional connectivity of the angular gyrus in normal reading and dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 8939–8944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Paulesu, E.; Frith, U.; Snowling, M.; Gallagher, A.; Morton, J.; Frackowiak, R.S.; Frith, C.D. Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain 1996, 119(pt 1), 143–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Klingberg, T.; Hedehus, M.; Temple, E.; Salz, T.; Gabrieli, J.D.; Moseley, M.E.; Poldrack, R.A. Microstructure of temporoparietal white matter as a basis for reading ability: Evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuron 2000, 25, 493–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Ben-Shachar, M.; Dougherty, R.F.; Wandell, B.A. White matter pathways in reading. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007, 17, 258–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Piantoni, G.; Poil, S.-S.; Lonkenkaer-Hansen, K.; Verweij, I.M.; Ramautar, J.R.; Van Someren, E.J.W.; Van Der Werf, Y.D. Individual differences in white matter diffusion affect sleep oscillations. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Dyslexia (Specific Reading Disability). Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1301–1309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Dehaene, S. Reading in the Brain: The New Science of How We Read; Penguin: London, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  74. Astill, R.G.; Van der Heijden, K.B.; Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 1109–1138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Safont, G.; Salazar, A.; Vergara, L.; Gòmez, E.; Villanueva, V. Multichannel dynamic modeling of non-Gaussian mixtures. Pattern Recognit. 2019, 93, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Costanzo, F.; Menghini, D.; Caltagirone, C.; Olivieri, M.; Vicari, S. How to improve reading skills in dyslexics: The effect of high frequency rTMS. Neuropsychologia 2013, 51, 2953–2959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Costanzo, F.; Varuzza, C.; Rossi, S.; Sdoia, S.; Varvara, P.; Olivieri, M.; Koch, G.; Vicari, S.; Menghini, D. Reading changes in children and adolescents with dyslexia after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport 2016, 27, 295–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Costanzo, F.; Varuzza, C.; Rossi, S.; Sdoia, S.; Varvara, P.; Olivieri, M.; Koch, G.; Vicari, S.; Menghini, D. Evidence for reading improvement following tDCS treatment in children and adolescents with dyslexia. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 2016, 34, 215–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Costanzo, F.; Rossi, S.; Varuzza, C.; Varvara, P.; Vicari, S.; Menghini, D. Long-lasting improvement following tDCS treatment combined with a training for reading in children and adolescents with dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2019, 130, 38–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. (A) Topographical distribution of relative EEG power of delta (0.5–4.75 Hz) activity during NREM sleep for the group with DD and the NR. The average values are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface and interpolated between electrodes. The maps are based on 28 EEG derivations of the international 10-10 system with linked mastoid reference, and then normalized to average power across the individual EEG maps. (B) Results of the statistical comparisons (two-tailed t-tests for independent samples) on EEG delta power of NREM sleep between the group of DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The statistical map reports the t-values for each scalp location.
Figure 1. (A) Topographical distribution of relative EEG power of delta (0.5–4.75 Hz) activity during NREM sleep for the group with DD and the NR. The average values are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface and interpolated between electrodes. The maps are based on 28 EEG derivations of the international 10-10 system with linked mastoid reference, and then normalized to average power across the individual EEG maps. (B) Results of the statistical comparisons (two-tailed t-tests for independent samples) on EEG delta power of NREM sleep between the group of DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The statistical map reports the t-values for each scalp location.
Brainsci 11 00073 g001
Figure 2. (A) Whole frequency range (11–15 Hz), fast (13–15 Hz) and slow (11–13 Hz) spindle density topographical scalp maps in the group of DD and NR. The maps are based on the 28 derivation of the 10-10 system (electrodes positions indicated by black dots). Values are color-coded and plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the hemispheric scalp model. Values between electrodes were interpolated. Values are expressed in terms of number of spindles divided by artifact-free NREM sleep minutes. (B) Results of the statistical comparisons (two-tailed t-tests for independent samples) on EEG delta power of NREM sleep between the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The statistical map reports the t-values for each scalp location. White dots indicate significant differences after FDR correction (p < 0.0077).
Figure 2. (A) Whole frequency range (11–15 Hz), fast (13–15 Hz) and slow (11–13 Hz) spindle density topographical scalp maps in the group of DD and NR. The maps are based on the 28 derivation of the 10-10 system (electrodes positions indicated by black dots). Values are color-coded and plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the hemispheric scalp model. Values between electrodes were interpolated. Values are expressed in terms of number of spindles divided by artifact-free NREM sleep minutes. (B) Results of the statistical comparisons (two-tailed t-tests for independent samples) on EEG delta power of NREM sleep between the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The statistical map reports the t-values for each scalp location. White dots indicate significant differences after FDR correction (p < 0.0077).
Brainsci 11 00073 g002
Figure 3. Maps of the correlation coefficients (Spearman’s Rho) between the EEG power of SWA for each electrode (28 channels) and the sleep-dependent consolidation rate at Word pair task (WPT) for the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The values of the correlation coefficients are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface, and interpolated between electrodes.
Figure 3. Maps of the correlation coefficients (Spearman’s Rho) between the EEG power of SWA for each electrode (28 channels) and the sleep-dependent consolidation rate at Word pair task (WPT) for the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The values of the correlation coefficients are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface, and interpolated between electrodes.
Brainsci 11 00073 g003
Figure 4. Maps of the correlation coefficients (Spearman’s Rho) between spindle density (all, fast and slow) for each electrode (28 channels) and the sleep-dependent consolidation rate at Word pair task (WPT) for the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The values of the correlation coefficients are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface, and interpolated between electrodes.
Figure 4. Maps of the correlation coefficients (Spearman’s Rho) between spindle density (all, fast and slow) for each electrode (28 channels) and the sleep-dependent consolidation rate at Word pair task (WPT) for the group with DD (n = 10) and the NR (n = 15). The values of the correlation coefficients are color-coded, plotted at the corresponding position on the planar projection of the scalp surface, and interpolated between electrodes.
Brainsci 11 00073 g004
Table 1. Means and standard deviations (SD) of demographic data (age), Intelligence Quotient (IQ), reading (DDE-2 and MT test) and clinical (CBCL) measures for children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) and Normal Readers (NR). Results of between-group comparisons are also reported (t and p values). Significant differences between groups (p < 0.05) are indicated in bold.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations (SD) of demographic data (age), Intelligence Quotient (IQ), reading (DDE-2 and MT test) and clinical (CBCL) measures for children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) and Normal Readers (NR). Results of between-group comparisons are also reported (t and p values). Significant differences between groups (p < 0.05) are indicated in bold.
DD
Mean (SD)
(n = 11)
NR
Mean (SD)
(n = 18)
t(1,27)p
Age (years, months)11.04 (1.67)11.72 (1.56)−1.130.269
IQ111.3 (6.21)110.72 (12.37)0.140.9
DDE-2 Word reading
Speed1181.46 (69.87)76.74 (23.35)5.90<0.001
Errors8.45 (6.36)1.72 (2.7)3.97<0.001
DDE-2 Non-word reading
Speed1119.96 (39.4)57.7 (19.4)5.7<0.001
Errors14.54 (5.34)4.44 (4.13)5.72<0.001
MT-2 Text reading
Speed21.90 (0.92)4.66 (1.2)−5.9<0.001
Errors15.10 (5.28)3.5 (3)5.7<0.001
CBCL
Internalizing probl.50.6 (10.6)52.2 (12.4)−0.250.81
Externalizing probl.48.4 (10)48.8 (10.64)−0.070.95
Total probl.50.6 (9.58)50.3 (11.5)0.050.96
1 seconds; 2 syllables/seconds. Abbreviations: DDE-2, Word and Non-word reading tests from the Battery for evaluating Dyslexia and Dysortography-2; MT-2, Text reading test; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations (SD) of the Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC) parent-report questionnaire, composed by six subscales and a total score, for children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 11) and Normal Readers (NR) (n = 18). Results of between-group comparisons (t and p values) are also reported.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations (SD) of the Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC) parent-report questionnaire, composed by six subscales and a total score, for children with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 11) and Normal Readers (NR) (n = 18). Results of between-group comparisons (t and p values) are also reported.
SDSCDD
Mean (SD)
(n = 11)
NR
Mean (SD)
(n = 18)
t(1,27)p
DIMS11.91 (3.48)10.67 (2.70)1.080.291
SBD3.73 (0.79)4.22 (1.00)−1.390.175
DA3.36 (0.50)3.33 (0.49)0.160.873
SWTD8.73 (2.10)8.22 (2.21)0.610.548
DOES8.00 (2.05)7.44 (3.18)0.520.611
SHY2.55 (0.93)2.89 (1.49)−0.680.500
Total Score38.27 (6.17)36.78 (7.36)0.560.578
Abbreviations: DIMS, Disorders of initiating and Maintaining Sleep; SBD, Sleep Breathing Disorders; DA, Disorders of Arousal; SWTD, Sleep-Wake Transition Disorders; DOES, Disorders of Excessive Somnolence; SHY, Sleep Hyper-hydrosis.
Table 3. Means and standard deviations (SD) of Word-Pair Task (WPT) variables for participants with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 11) and Normal Readers (NR) (n = 18). Results of between-group comparisons, calculated using a two-tailed t-test for independent sample, are also reported (t and p values). Significant differences between groups (p < 0.05) are indicated in bold. #: number.
Table 3. Means and standard deviations (SD) of Word-Pair Task (WPT) variables for participants with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 11) and Normal Readers (NR) (n = 18). Results of between-group comparisons, calculated using a two-tailed t-test for independent sample, are also reported (t and p values). Significant differences between groups (p < 0.05) are indicated in bold. #: number.
WPT VariablesDD
Mean (SD)
NR
Mean (SD)
t(1,27)p
# of trials to criterion3 (0.89)2.5 (0.8)1.5780.126
# of recalled word pairs in the evening16.55 (5.88)19.11 (3.95)−1.4080.171
# of recalled word pairs in the morning14 (6.03)18.11(4.38)−2.1250.043
Sleep−dependent consolidation rate0.84(0.14)0.94 (0.09)−2.5010.019
Table 4. Means and standard deviations (SD) of macrostructural parameters for participants with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 10) and the Normal Readers (NR) (n = 15) groups. Results of between-group comparisons, calculated using a two-tailed t-test for independent sample, are also reported (t and p values). Abbreviations: S1, stage 1; S2, stage 2; SWS, Slow Wave Sleep; REM, Rapid Eye Movements; NREM, Non-rapid Eye Movements; WASO, Wake After Sleep Onset; TST, Total Sleep Time; TBT, Total Bedtime; #MA, number of Movement Arousal; SEI, Sleep Efficiency Index; SOL, Sleep Onset Latency.
Table 4. Means and standard deviations (SD) of macrostructural parameters for participants with Developmental Dyslexia (DD) (n = 10) and the Normal Readers (NR) (n = 15) groups. Results of between-group comparisons, calculated using a two-tailed t-test for independent sample, are also reported (t and p values). Abbreviations: S1, stage 1; S2, stage 2; SWS, Slow Wave Sleep; REM, Rapid Eye Movements; NREM, Non-rapid Eye Movements; WASO, Wake After Sleep Onset; TST, Total Sleep Time; TBT, Total Bedtime; #MA, number of Movement Arousal; SEI, Sleep Efficiency Index; SOL, Sleep Onset Latency.
Macrostructural VariablesDD
Mean (SD)
NR
Mean (SD)
t(1,23)p
Latency S17.91 (136.80)13.71 (10.02)1.660.11
Latency S211.82 (8.80)16.63 (11.23)−1.140.27
Latency SWS25.39 (8.99)29.34 (14.34)−0.770.45
Latency REM173.02 (32.21)193.64 (82.47)−0.750.46
Duration S18.00 (5.95)11.24 (8.64)−1.030.31
Duration S2280.53 (67.83)289.78 (58.27)−0360.72
Duration SWS91.43 (25.16)91.24 (22.97)0.020.98
Duration REM106.49 (34.33)104.03 (31.27)0.190.85
Duration NREM379.96 (56.64)391.93 (57.08)−0.520.61
% S11.70 (1.24)2.42 (1.93)−1.040.31
% S251.17 (8.23)58.13 (5.73)−0.340.73
% REM21.56 (4.83)20.70 (4.69)0.450.66
% NREM78.44 (4.83)79.24 (4.78)−0.410.69
% SWS19.56 (8.08)18.74 (5.46)0.300.76
WASO38.80 (31.12)55.71 (46.31)−1.010.32
TST486.46 (77.95)496.24 (75.56)−0.310.76
TBT534.50 (57.06)566.35 (46.63)−1.530.14
# MA53.80 (19.00)69.93 (27.55)−1.610.12
SEI (%)90.63 (7.80)86.58 (8.98)1.160.26
SOL (min)11.98 (8.86)16.35 (11.38)−1.050.31
Table 5. Spearman’s correlations between sleep macrostructural parameters and overnight memory recall for each group separately and for the entire sample.
Table 5. Spearman’s correlations between sleep macrostructural parameters and overnight memory recall for each group separately and for the entire sample.
Macrostructural VariablesDD
Rho (p)
NR
Rho (p)
Latency S10.26 (0.47)−0.02 (0.95)
Latency S20.05 (0.88)0.03 (0.91)
Latency SWS−0.05 (0.89)−0.01 (0.97)
Latency REM0.03 (0.93)0.04 (0.89)
Duration S1−0.33 (0.35)0.09 (0.74)
Duration S2−0.36 (0.31)0.01 (0.98)
Duration SWS0.24 (0.51)−0.23 (0.41)
Duration REM−0.60 (0.07)−0.18 (0.53)
Duration NREM−0.26 (0.47)−0.03 (0.90)
% S1−0.02 (0.96)0.11 (0.69)
% S2−0.08 (0.83)0.07 (0.81)
% REM−0.60 (0.07)−0.19 (0.49)
% NREM0.60 (0.07)0.18 (0.51)
% SWS0.55 (0.10)−0.07 (0.81)
WASO0.24 (0.51)0.16 (0.56)
TST−0.45 (0.19)0.07 (0.79)
TBT−0.50 (0.19)−0.08 (0.79)
# MA− 0.33(0.35)−0.15 (0.58)
SEI (%)−0.28 (0.43)−0.14 (0.62)
SOL (min)0.05 (0.88)0.05 (0.86)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Reda, F.; Gorgoni, M.; D'Atri, A.; Scarpelli, S.; Carpi, M.; Di Cola, E.; Menghini, D.; Vicari, S.; Stella, G.; De Gennaro, L. Sleep-Related Declarative Memory Consolidation in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Dyslexia. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 73. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11010073

AMA Style

Reda F, Gorgoni M, D'Atri A, Scarpelli S, Carpi M, Di Cola E, Menghini D, Vicari S, Stella G, De Gennaro L. Sleep-Related Declarative Memory Consolidation in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Dyslexia. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(1):73. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11010073

Chicago/Turabian Style

Reda, Flaminia, Maurizio Gorgoni, Aurora D'Atri, Serena Scarpelli, Matteo Carpi, Erica Di Cola, Deny Menghini, Stefano Vicari, Giacomo Stella, and Luigi De Gennaro. 2021. "Sleep-Related Declarative Memory Consolidation in Children and Adolescents with Developmental Dyslexia" Brain Sciences 11, no. 1: 73. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11010073

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop