Introduction

The archaeological record of the Middle Stone Age (MSA) in Eastern Africa has played a prominent role in debates surrounding the evolution of human behaviour. The Eastern African MSA record is particularly rich, revealed by an extensive research history (Basell 2008; Blinkhorn and Grove 2018; Clark 1988; Tryon and Faith 2013. Critically, recent research at Olorgesailie has identified the oldest MSA sites across Africa, dating to 320–297 ka (Brooks et al. 2018; Deino et al. 2018; Potts et al. 2018). Notably, this overlaps with the earliest appearance of Homo sapiens fossils in the African record, dating to 300 ka at Jebel Irhoud in Northern Africa (Hublin et al. 2017). This significantly precedes the Eastern African fossil record for Homo sapiens (McDougall et al. 2005), which formerly led to a strong focus on the region as the centre of endemism for modern humans. Such straightforward associations between any single African region and the appearance of Homo sapiens appear increasingly untenable, with the growing acknowledgement that complex population interactions across the continent are likely to have occurred (Scerri et al. 2018). Nevertheless, the Eastern African MSA record remains an important testing ground for hypotheses regarding behavioural change over a critical period in human evolution.

Characterising the MSA of Eastern Africa has proved complex as there are no behavioural features that are ubiquitous within the archaeological record that span the early emergence of MSA assemblages in Marine Isotope Stage (MIS) 9 until its final appearance in the region during MIS 3 (Blinkhorn and Grove 2018). Nevertheless, common typological features of stone tool assemblages attributed to the MSA include the use of varied Levallois methods, the focus on a diverse retouched tool kit focusing upon medium-sized flakes, and the proliferation of point technologies. Late Acheulean technologies persist in the region until 212 ka at Mieso (De la Torre et al. 2014), with MSA industries typically focusing on more fine-grained raw materials with a decreased use and diversity of heavy tools employed. Later Stone Age industries first appear in the region from 64 ka at Panga ya Saidi (Shipton et al. 2018), marked by a significant shift in artefact size associated with the increased prominence of microlithic tools and bipolar technology, along with a further change in raw material preference, including more widespread exploitation of quartz.

Identifying clear patterns of behaviour across the MSA record of Eastern Africa has proved complex. The recent application of quantitative methods to examine the diversity of archaeological assemblages has begun to illuminate some trends. Tryon and Faith (2013) applied correspondence analyses to a presence/absence dataset of key artefact types including lithic artefacts, beads and ochre, identifying a chronological distinction between earlier (before MIS 5) and later (MIS 5-3) assemblages. Further to this, a regression analysis was conducted on the distance between sites and a Dice-Sorensen index calculated from the same dataset, indicating a weak but significant trend of increasing difference in behaviour with distance (Tryon and Faith 2013). Recently, we examined an expanded dataset of lithic artefact typology alongside geographic and environmental parameters using a combination of dissimilarity matrices and hierarchical clustering (Blinkhorn and Grove 2018). This highlighted a tripartite split of the MSA record, between assemblages dating to MIS 5 and those that precede and succeed them, defined two common core clusters of stone tool types, illuminated the diversity and structure of geographic and environmental settings of MSA sites and identified pulsed patterns of occupation intensity and shifting patterns of landscape colonisation through time (Blinkhorn and Grove 2018). Here, we expand upon the use of dissimilarity matrices to examine a presence/absence dataset of lithic artefact types using matrix correlations.

Mantel tests examine correlations between dissimilarity matrices (Mantel 1967). As dissimilarity matrices do not present independent data points, significance testing is achieved through a process of multiple random permutations of one of the matrices to evaluate the proportion of correlation coefficients that are greater than that with the original matrix. Partial Mantel tests and multiple matrix (or multiple Mantel) regression follow similar procedure whilst controlling for the impact of a third or multiple additional matrices respectively (Legendre et al. 2015; Smouse et al. 1986). These tests present the means to investigate causal relationships between distance matrices rather than between the paired vectors themselves. Such methods have been routinely employed in population studies and ecology (Legendre 2000; Legendre et al. 2015), and lately, both simple and partial Mantel tests have begun to be used more frequently amongst archaeologists and anthropologists. Buchanan and Hamilton (2009) used simple and partial Mantel tests to examine variability in early Paleoindian projectile points, suggesting functional modifications as adaptations to different biomes were minimal and emphasising the impact of spatial autocorrelation. Ross et al. (2013) used these methods to identify independent effects of geographical distance and ethnolinguistic affiliation upon folktale diversity. Brown et al. (2013) employ Mantel tests to demonstrate significant and discrete correlations between genetic diversity and both musical and linguistic traditions. Shennan et al. (2015) employ matrix correlations to examine the impact of isolation by distance on stylistic features of pottery and jewellery assemblages from Neolithic Europe compared to cultural affiliation. Lycett (2018) employs these methods to determine the impact of isolation by distance in beadwork designs and manufacture amongst historic-era Great Plains populations. Alongside their use in other disciplines, these studies illustrate the potential of these methods to explore archaeological and anthropological questions. Isolation by distance (Wright 1943) plays a prominent role in studies that employ matrix correlations, drawing upon their significance in population biology and ecology, although the examples noted above clearly demonstrate the potential to examine other forms of dissimilarity, including distances in behaviour, time, geography and ecology.

What explains difference in stone tool composition

A range of factors may influence the composition of stone tool assemblages (Andrefsky 2008). Firstly, different approaches to the practice of archaeology can affect the composition of stone tool assemblages. The method of reporting stone tools can impact the evaluation of lithic assemblage diversity, as researchers employ different typological schemes that may split or lump the reporting of certain artefact forms (Whittaker et al. 1998). Artefact assemblages from surface surveys may differ in composition for those recovered by systematic excavations, whilst larger excavations or more systematic survey protocols may recover larger stone tool assemblages with a chance of sampling greater behavioural diversity. Whilst partly governed by depositional processes and factors of preservation, as well as past behaviour, the examination of caves and rock shelters in contrast to open-air sites is also a choice that features in archaeological fieldwork design. It is also critical to be aware that examination of Stone Age archaeological sites involves studies of palimpsests of behaviour that can range between a small number of knapping episodes to those that spanned many generations (Bailey 2007).

Secondly, stone tools have played a significant functional role in the lives of past populations, and as such, assemblage composition may reflect the impact of varied practical concerns. Functional impacts on assemblage composition may be manifest in multiple, overlapping ways. The dichotomy of residential and logistic hunter-gatherer sites (Binford 1980) likely compresses much variability as to how sites identified by archaeologists were occupied and used by past populations but does characterise differences between generalised, larger and longer-term occupations likely to record a broader range of reduction activities and smaller, specialised occupations that may record only a subset of a wider technological repertoire. Geographic factors, such as altitude and roughness of terrain, can significantly impact patterns of mobility amongst human populations as well as faunal and floral communities that present the subsistence base (Binford 2019), such that alternate stone tools or combinations of them may yield functional advantages in different geographic contexts. Similarly, environmental factors, including patterns of temperature and precipitation, can drive substantive differences in ecology and patterns of engagement by hunter-gatherer populations (Binford 2019) for which alternate combinations of stone tools may offer differing functional advantages. Critically, the individual tools may not represent finalised forms but rather occur as part of a reduction continuum (Rolland and Dibble 1990), and the intensity of artefact reduction prior to entering the archaeological record may reflect the variety of factors discussed above. A significant body of research has documented the impact of using differing raw materials upon lithic reduction practices yet beyond the functional constraints of differing fracture mechanics between alternate materials for controlling the reduction process, access to alternate raw materials, the clast size available, distribution in the landscape and proximity to an occupation site and the impact of material crystal structure on tool use may all impact raw material choices (Andrefsky 1994; Eren et al. 2014b). Such functional constraints are likely to have been engaged with through a social lens, with multiple alternative behavioural strategies possible, though strategies in which basic subsistence needs cannot be met are unlikely to have been long lasting (see Tostevin 2013).

Stone tool reduction practices that create archaeological assemblages are transmitted between individuals and groups through cultural transmission and may be subject to both neutral and directional selections (Creanza et al. 2017). The examination of behavioural variability through time and space plays a foundation role in archaeological research. Within biological studies, isolation by distance (Wright 1943) is a key concept suggesting increasing genetic differences are likely to occur between populations that are separated by larger geographic distances as a result of reproductive isolation. This presents an analogy for cultural variability, in which greater cultural differences may be anticipated between populations that are separated by larger distances in space and time, as a result of decreased opportunity for high fidelity transmission of cultural transmission and increased opportunity for either random or adaptive variations to cultural practice. It is important to note that the factors noted above may impact the influence of distance through time and space. For example, greater cultural differences could occur between stone tool assemblages located within closer geographic proximity to one another but in starkly different ecological contexts compared to those at greater geographic distances within similar habitats. Finally, population size, population structure and the scale of population mobility are all recognised as features that influence both the rate of behavioural innovation and potential patterns of cultural transmission (Powell et al., 2009; Shennan et al. 2015; Loog et al. 2017).

The aim of this paper is to examine the extent to which a range of these factors can explain the variability between stone tool assemblages spanning the Eastern African MSA record. We employ simple and partial Mantel tests to examine correlations between dissimilarity matrices for stone tool assemblage composition and a range of factors described above. We examine whether distance in behaviour is correlated to distance in raw material use, site type, investigative strategy, age, location, the combination of time and space, and a range of characteristics of site environments at 5-km and 50-km radii, including Altitude, roughness of terrain and mean annual temperature and precipitation for modern conditions and modelled arid and humid conditions. We then explore the extent to which such correlations are independent of one another. By demonstrating the impact of archaeological and functional constraints on our understanding of past behavioural variability, we hope to identify avenues to pursue the role of cultural forces on behavioural evolution in Stone Age Eastern Africa.

Datasets and methods

The dataset is derived from the Eastern African literature, including typology, chronometric ages, raw material use and location, building on the dataset from Blinkhorn and Grove (2018). Stone tool assemblages were included in the dataset for analysis where details of raw material use, the method of investigation (survey or excavation) and site type (open-air or cave/rock shelter) are reported and a minimum and maximum age estimate is available. The sites, assemblages, locations, method of investigation and site type along with the minimum and maximum age and key references are listed in Table 1, and their locations are illustrated in Fig. 1.

Table 1 List of sites and assemblages used in the analysis, including details of location (latitude and longitude), whether the assemblage was recovered from surface or by excavation, whether from an open-air site (OpenAir) or from a cave or rock shelter (Cave/RS), and the minimum and maximum age estimates reported, along with key references
Fig. 1
figure 1

Location map of Eastern African MSA sites included in the analysis, illustrating their location on an SRTM-DEM (Jarvis et al. 2008)

The stone artefact typology used here follows Grove and Blinkhorn (2020). Terms used include the following: Backed/Microliths, BipolarTech, BladeTech, Borer, Burin, CentripetalTech, CoreTool, Denticulate, LevalloisBladeTech, LevalloisFlakeTech, LevalloisPointTech, Notch, PlatformCore, PointTech, RTBifacial and Scraper. These terms are described in SI and the relationship between these terms are original terminology reported in the literature are set out in Table S1. The presence of at least two of the artefact types was required for inclusion of an assemblage in the dataset. This typology is derived from patterns of terminological use in the literature to enable a wide as possible synthesis of data, with potential drawbacks of this approach discussed in Blinkhorn and Grove (2018). All reports of raw material use were recorded grouped into one of eight categories, based upon their frequency, all of which occur in a minimum of 8 different assemblages: cherts, quartzs, obsidians, basalts, metamorphics, crypto-crystalline silica (CCS), other igneous and sedimentary. These data are illustrated in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Binary matrix of presence and absence of artefact types (left) and raw material types (right) for Middle Stone Age sites in Eastern Africa, ordered from oldest (top) to youngest (bottom)

Site types were recorded as either open-air sites or rock shelters and caves. The method of archaeological investigation was recorded as either excavation or survey. Artefact assemblages are only included in the dataset for analyses when they can be ascribed a minimum and maximum age, based upon reports from the literature. Raw data for radiocarbon dates were recorded and calibrated using the IntCal13 curve using the bchron v4.3 package (Parnell 2018) in R. A simple age estimate for each assemblage is provided by the mid-point between the minimum and maximum age. Additional data employed in the study include the SRTM 1-km DEM (Jarvis et al. 2008), and annual precipitation and temperature datasets for modern (MOD), last glacial maximum (LGM) and last interglacial (LIG) timeframes from www.worldclim.org, scaled between 0 and 1. We employ modern, LGM and LIG temperature and precipitation datasets as a means to bracket the scale of likely Late Pleistocene environmental variability in the absence of comparable proxy records from all sites included in the analysis.

Within the methods presented below and the results section, the differences between sites in each variable, which comprise the dissimilarity matrices, are referred to by the name of the variable highlighted in italics (e.g. Behaviour). The presence of stone tool types and raw material types were coded as present (1) or absent (0) and dissimilarity matrices (Behaviour; RawMat) calculated using Jaccard’s coefficient, which excludes the impact of shared absences from calculations. Site type and investigation method were coded as discrete binary variables (e.g. open air = 0; rock shelter/cave = 1) for calculation of dissimilarity matrices (Site; Method [see Table 1]), also using Jaccard’s coefficient. A simple estimate of distance in time between sites was achieved by subtracting the younger age from the older age to produce a dissimilarity matrix based on mid-point between the maximum and minimum assemblage ages (Age). For example, the mid-point between minimum (80 ka) and maximum (100 ka) ages at Aduma is 90 ka.

Distances between sites (Costpath) were calculated as cost paths using Tobler’s function for speed of movement across terrain as a factor of changing slope derived from the SRTM 1-km DEM, using the gdistance package (van Etten 2017), with major water bodies (e.g. Lake Turkana) masked from the analysis. Following Blinkhorn and Grove (2018) we derived a raster dataset from the SRTM DEM (Jarvis et al. 2008) relating to slope of the terrain, which were transformed to values of energy expenditure (Joules per metre per second) following Minetti et al. (2015) (Roughness). This accounts for the fact that differences in cost of moving on flat ground vs 10° slopes compared to 10° and 20° slopes are not equal, although the differences in slope are the same. For this raster dataset (Roughness), as well as altitude and both modern and modelled past climatic conditions, buffers were created around site locations with a radius of 5 km and 50 km to examine the immediate residential landscape of the site (5 km) and the broader logistical terrain in which it is situated (50 km), creating histogram datasets. The distance matrices for these histogram-based datasets (Roughness, Altitude, MOD/LGM/LIG temp and precip at both 5-km and 50-km scales [e.g. MODprecip50km]) were calculated using the L2 Wasserstien metric (Irpino 2017).

Loog et al. (2017) highlight the impact of changing mobility of past populations on patterns of isolation by distance and time, and present a method to combine these two factors into a single extra-correlated variable that recognised impacts of both dimensions on cultural inheritance. Whilst both increased spatial separation of contemporaneous populations or chronological separation within occupation of single site could impact patterns of cultural inheritance, the combination of distance in space and time is likely to compound this effect. We generated 9999 incremental combinations of the distance matrices for Age and Costpath distance, from 1/9999 contribution to 9998/9999 contribution of Costpath distance. Mantel tests were conducted for each of these 9999 composite timespace matrices with behavioural distance to identify which combination of Age and Costpath distance produced the strongest relationship with behavioural distance. This resulted in the identification of an extra-correlated relationship to behavioural distance with a ratio of contribution from Costpath and Age distance of 1.603 to produce the dissimilarity matrix (Timespace).

The null hypothesis of simple Mantel tests, partial Mantel tests and multiple matrix regression is that there is no correlation between two dissimilarity matrices, factoring in the impact of additional variables for partial Mantel tests and multiple matrix regressions. For example, the null hypothesis for simple Mantel test between Behaviour and Costpath is that no relationship exists in how dissimilar archaeological assemblages are with regards to the difference in cost in moving between the sites where assemblages are found. The alternative hypothesis, therefore, is that there is a relationship between the dissimilarity in these variables, in the form of either a positive or negative correlation. For example, a positive correlation in dissimilarity may exist, such that sites that are close together (i.e. low dissimilarity in Costpath) may share more similar constellations of stone tool types (i.e. low dissimilarity in Behaviour), with high dissimilarity in Costpath distance correlating to high dissimilarity in distance for behaviour. A negative correlation suggests that as dissimilarity increases in one variable, it decreases in another.

Simple Mantel tests were then conducted between behaviour and all other matrices in R using the vegan package (Oksanen et al. 2019) and employing 9999 permutations to obtain p values. In this process, the rows and columns of one matrix are randomly permuted and Pearson’s correlation coefficients are calculated, ranging from 1 to − 1. The significance of correlation between the un-permuted matrices is derived from the proportion of permuted r values that are higher than that of the un-permuted r value. Partial Mantel tests (also conducted using vegan and 9999 permutations) were then calculated with controlling variables identified in light of simple Mantel tests: Method and Site, RawMat, Altitude and Roughness, MODPrecip50km, MODTemp50km and LGMTemp50km, LGMPrecip50km, Age, Costpath and Timespace. Finally, multiple matrix regression examined significant correlations between Behaviour and RawMat, Method, Site, Age, Costpath, Altitude50km, Roughness50km, MODprecip50km and MODtemp50km (as an enter, rather than stepwise, procedure) controlling for the impact of multiple response variables, with 9999 permutations using the phytools package in R (Revell 2012). In all cases, test statistics reported are produced via permutation. Further testing was undertaken employing the same methods described above to examine the relationships between raw material use and other variables. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure is used to adjust p values for simple and partial Mantel tests.

Results

Simple Mantel tests

A number of significant correlations are observed between distance in assemblage composition and other variables (Table 2). Behavioural distance (Behaviour) amongst MSA sites is most strongly (and significantly) correlated with RawMat. Roughness50km shows comparable strength of relationship to Behaviour as RawMat, but the weakest relationship across all variables at the 5-km scale (Roughness5km). Site altitude shows strong correlations to behavioural distance for both 50-km and 5-km scales, with temperature variables showing stronger correlations than precipitation variables, with stronger correlations typically observed across both groups at the 5-km rather than 50-km scale. Only LGmPrecip50km does not show a correlation to behavioural distance that is statistically significant with an alpha = 0.05 threshold when adjusted. Differences in the extra-correlated Timespace variable are significantly correlated Behaviour, as is Costpath, but differences in Age alone are not. Site exhibits a significant correlation to behavioural distance, although Method does not.

Table 2 Results of simple Mantel tests between the presence and absence of either key MSA artefact types (behaviour) and raw material use (raw material), and other variables spanning time, space, geography and ecology. p values from individual tests are reported, with statistically significant results after p-adjustment highlighted in bold

Simple Mantel tests suggest both similarities and differences to the patterns described above when the correlations in distance between raw material use and other variables are considered. RawMat is most strongly correlated to Behaviour, followed by Timespace, with both Costpath and Age showing significant correlation to RawMat. Amongst environmental variables examined, only MODTemp5km, LIGTemp5km and LGMPrecip5km distances do not show a significant correlation to differences in raw material use after p value adjustment. Amongst environmental variables, temperature variables tend to show stronger correlation to raw material use, as do environmental variables at the 50-km rather than 5-km scale, with LGMTemp50km showing the strongest relationship, closely followed by LIGTemp50km and LIGPrecip50km. In contrast to Behaviour, terrain roughness is significantly correlated to RawMat at the 5-km scale, but not the 50-km scale, whereas altitude only shows a significant relationship to RawMat at the 50-km scale. Differences in Site show a weaker but significant correlation to raw material distance in contrast to Method, which is not significant at an alpha = 0.05 level.

Partial Mantel tests

The strongest correlation observed with Behaviour with simple Mantel tests was RawMat. When RawMat is controlled for, fourteen of twenty-two variables examined show substantive changes in the strength of correlations with Behaviour (> 10% change in test statistic) (Table 3). Variables with more limited change include Site, Roughness50km, Altitude5km, LGMPrecip5km, LGMTemp5km, LIGTemp5km and MODPrecip5km. The difference in Site, differentiating open-air sites from rock shelters and caves, exhibited a significant correlation with Behaviour, which may reflect choices made by archaeologists and past populations, as well as a factor of depositional contingency. When differences in Site are controlled for under a partial Mantel test, some modulations to correlation strength are observed, though only seven variables show a change > 10%, with no change noted in significant correlations to Behaviour. The largest changes in strength of correlation occur for Roughness5km and Age, neither of which demonstrated a significant relationship to behavioural differences.

Notable correlation was identified between Behaviour and difference in both Altitude (both at 50-km and 5-km scales) and Roughness50km. A number of substantive changes in the strength of correlation with Behaviour and significance of relationships are observed for environmental variables when either Altitude50km or Roughness50km are controlled for. When Altitude50km is controlled for, significant relationships occur between Behaviour and Roughness50km, RawMat, Site and Timespace, whereas only the latter three variables retain significant relationships to Behaviour when Roughness50km is controlled for. When the various precipitation and temperature datasets are controlled for, a common pattern of impact on other environmental variables is observed, which are disregarded due to the impact of potential autocorrelations. Although the strength of correlations are modulated for non-environmental variables, these typically do not result in changes to significant relationships with Behaviour except for altitude variables, which may represent further autocorrelation between geography and climate.

Age showed weak but insignificant correlation to Behaviour, with no difference in the significance of correlations from the result of simple Mantel tests observed when it is controlled for, and modulation of test statistics is < 10% in all instances. When Costpath is controlled for, most variables show little difference in the strength or significance of correlations with Behaviour, with the exception of LIGPrecip50km, which no longer show significant correlations. In the majority of cases, only minor modulation to the strength of correlations are observed when the extra-correlated variable Timespace is controlled for, whilst the correlations between Behaviour with LIGPrecip50km are no longer significant.

Table 3 Test statistics of partial Mantel tests for Behaviour for each variable (rows) when the impact of individual variables (columns) are controlled for. Significant correlations after p-adjustment are highlighted in bold

The results of partial Mantel tests examining the relationship between RawMat and other variables are presented in Table 4. Relatively minor modulations in the strength of correlations are observed to a small number of variables when Site is controlled for, without impacting the significance of any relationships with RawMat. Controlling for the impact of Behaviour, correlations between RawMat and Altitude50km, LGMTemp5km, MODPrecip5km, LGMPrecip50km and Site show a substantial drop in strength and are no longer significant, with more minor modulation across other variables that retain significant relationship to RawMat. Limited changes in the strength of correlations between RawMat and other variables are observed when Roughness50km is controlled for, although the relationship between RawMat and LIGTemp5km becomes significant at an alpha = 0.05 threshold after p-adjustment. In contrast, more extensive changes in the strength of correlation to RawMat is observed when Altitude50km is controlled for, resulting in insignificant relationships between Site, LGMtemp5km, LGMprecip50km and MODPrecip5km. When alternate environment variables are considered, limited changes occur to other, non-environmental variables, with the relationship between differences in both Site and Altitude50km to RawMat repeatedly changing so that it is no longer significant. A single change occurs in the correlations between RawMat and other distance variables when Age is controlled for, with Site no longer showing a significant relationship to RawMat. When Costpath is controlled for, notable modulation of the strength of correlations to RawMat is observed across eleven variables, resulting in LGMprecip50km and Site no longer significantly correlated to RawMat. Controlling for Timespace, substantive changes in the strength of correlation between RawMat and sixteen other variables occur, resulting in insignificant relationships for Site, Altitude50km, LGMPrecip50km, LGMTemp5km and MODPrecip5km.

Table 4 Test statistics of partial Mantel tests for raw materials for each variable (rows) when the impact of individual variables (columns) are controlled for. Significant correlations after p-adjustment are highlighted in bold

Multiple matrix regressions

The alternate partial Mantel tests clearly show complex patterns of modulation of correlation between the variables under consideration and either Behaviour or RawMat, when the impact of individual variables is controlled for. Critically, it is difficult to constrain the impacts of spatial autocorrelations using Partial Mantel tests alone given the data sets employed, but multiple matrix regressions are able to limit their impact. Based on the results of simple testing above, we selected to use Age and Costpath, rather than Timespace, and to include MODprecip50km and MODtemp50km datasets to characterise environment variables, alongside Rawmat, Roughness50km, Site, Method and Altitude50km. The results of multiple matrix regression for Behaviour are shown in Table 5.

Table 5 Results of multiple matrix regression examining relationship between stone tool assemblage composition and other variables. Significant results are shown in bold

These results show that when nine variables that may explain differences in artefact assemblage composition are examined, three exhibit independent effects on Behaviour. RawMat and Roughness50km have the most significant correlations to Behaviour. Site also shows significant correlation to artefact assemblage dissimilarity. The results of multiple matrix regression (F-statistic: 59.829; r2 = 0.13413; p = 0.0001) clearly indicate important correlations between Behaviour and the variables under examination here.

In the multiple matrix regression for RawMat, Behaviour shows the strongest, independent correlation to RawMat (Table 6) and Behaviour appears to play a stronger role in explaining RawMat than vice versa. Age and Costpath show significant, independent correlations to RawMat. Finally, Roughness50km shows a significant, independent correlation with RawMat. The results of multiple matrix regression (F-statistic: 55.992; r2 = 0.1266; p = 0.0001) show the variables examined here explain a comparable proportion of variability in RawMat to Behaviour, but differences exist as to which variables and the extent to which they explain this variability.

Table 6 Results of multiple matrix regression examining relationship between raw material use and other variables. Significant results are shown in bold

Discussion

These results show that distances in assemblage composition and raw material use significantly correlate with distances across a wide range of variables analysed. Multiple matrix regressions suggest that the variables examined here explain ~ 13% of the variability for both stone tool assemblage composition and patterns of raw material use. In order to engage with the breadth of MSA assemblages across Eastern Africa, we have focused upon using the presence and absence of alternate artefact forms or raw material types. The suite of artefact forms employed combines types that represent technological approaches (e.g. Levallois), key artefact forms (e.g. bifaces) and retouched tools that may exist on reduction continua (e.g. scrapers/denticulates). This complicates direct assessment of whether variability identified here relates to functional differences in tool use, but the possibility for redundancy in tool kits should be noted. For instance, both blade and Levallois blade strategies produce elongate blanks, with the choice of using either (or both) potentially relating to cultural choices or historical contingencies in patterns of cultural transmission that may help explain the large amount of unexplained variability.

Amongst the variables under consideration, distance in assemblage composition is most strongly correlated to differences in raw material use and vice versa, as identified by simple Mantel tests. The strength of correlations between raw material and behavioural distance is only marginally modified when other factors are controlled for in partial Mantel tests, preserving the strongest, significant relationship in all circumstances. This is further supported by the strong, significant relationships between behaviour and raw material distances identified under multiple matrix regressions. Critically, these results suggest that the relationships between raw material use and behavioural differences are largely independent from other factors that have been examined. Relatively few exclusive relationships can be identified when the presence/absence of raw material use and tool types are cross tabulated (Table 7). No RTBifacial tools are identified in assemblages that include the use of CCS, other igneous or other sedimentary raw materials. In addition, burins are absent from assemblages that use other sedimentary raw materials. The remaining fourteen artefact forms examined here are found to be included in lithic assemblages that overlap with the use of all raw material types, whereas all tool types are found to some extent in assemblages containing basalts, cherts, obsidian, metamorphics and quartzs. Eleven of the assemblages under consideration exhibit the use of a single raw material, with all others including a combination of multiple raw material types, including four assemblages that include six distinct raw material types. Finer resolution data is required to examine further the associations between particular tool types and raw materials, but from this dataset, it is apparent that the means to produce a diverse tool kit may have been managed through selection of diverse or complementary raw material resources.

Table 7 Proportions of assemblages that contain each combination of tool type and raw material type

Lithic raw material resources are not evenly spread across the landscape and their distribution may affect patterns of access and therefore use; this in turn may result in a degree of spatial autocorrelation in raw material use. Minimal impact on the strength of correlation between the difference in raw material use and behavioural distance is observed when cost path distance is controlled for in partial Mantel tests. However, in multiple matrix regressions, cost path distance shows independent correlation to raw material differences, supporting suggestions that a pattern of isolation by distance has some role in explaining diversity in the MSA records of Eastern Africa (Tryon and Faith 2013). When raw material use is controlled for in partial Mantel tests, the strength of correlations between behavioural distance and both geographic and environmental variables are notably reduced, with a comparable pattern observed when behavioural distance is controlled for and correlations to raw material distances analysed. This suggests that some modulation of assemblage composition and raw material use may reflect differences in physical landscapes and ecological contexts, but such modulation reflects a flexible means to engage with environmental diversity, rather than a rigid requirement for certain toolkits for occupation of certain habitats.

Differences in time appear to have a more substantive relationship to changing raw material use than varying assemblage compositions. Previous research has employed quantitative methods to successfully identify patterns of change through time in constellations of stone tool use (Blinkhorn and Grove 2018; Tryon and Faith 2013). The results of this study suggest that although constellations of stone tool types may change through time, isolation by time does not offer a strong means to explain such patterns. However, the methods employed here may not fully account for pulsed changes in stone tool assemblage composition through time and particularly those that are identified at younger timeframes with shorter time differences between assemblages. Similarly, growing evidence suggests that substantial spatial structure exists within trajectories of change in the MSA during the Late Pleistocene, with the continuity of MSA technologies in regions such as highland Ethiopia whilst LSA technologies had become widely adopted in the Kenyan and Tanzanian Rift. The use of the extra-correlated Timespace variable presents one means to tackle this issue. Changing constellations of raw material use present a more discrete picture of change through time than modulations in assemblage composition in the MSA of Eastern Africa. Whilst no discrete patterns of exclusion occur, a notable shift in emphasis occurs from earlier assemblages which are more likely to include basalts to later assemblages that are more likely to contain quartz, with substantial overlap in preferences for both types occurring during MIS 5.

The different types of sites targeted correlate with patterns of behavioural distance. This appears to be independent of other factors that may have more directly affected past assemblage composition, as the strength and significance of relationships to behavioural distance remain largely unaffected when site type are controlled for, whilst the relationship between site type to behavioural distance remains comparable when other factors are controlled for in partial Mantel tests. Notably, site type shows significant independent correlations to behavioural distance in multiple matrix regressions. In contrast, differences between site types offer more limited means to explain differences in patterns of raw material use. This may partly reflect the alternate size of artefact and raw material inventories used, and the structure of past activities and tool use between open air compared to rock shelter or cave sites that may occur within landscapes with access to similar raw material resources.

Both assemblage composition and raw material use show significant relationships to differences in terrain roughness, but the nature of this relationship varies between these variables and between the analyses. Within simple Mantel tests, significant relationships were observed at different spatial scales, with distance in assemblage composition correlated to differences in roughness at the 50-km scale whereas patterns of raw material use correlated to distance in roughness at a 5-km scale. Nevertheless, differences in terrain roughness at the 50-km scale provide a substantial and independent means to explain variation in both assemblage composition and raw material use as identified in the multiple matrix regressions. Unlike either altitude or environmental variables, terrain roughness may not have direct relationships with patterns of ecology. However, roughness may substantively influence patterns of mobility and stone tool use across the landscape in which a site is situated. Differences in environmental conditions and altitude do show significant correlations to behavioural distance and raw material use, though a weaker relationship occurs between raw material use and altitude. It is notable that differences in temperature, rather than precipitation, typically exhibit the strongest correlations with patterns of assemblage composition and raw material use given the well-established relationship between temperature and biodiversity (Brown et al. 2004; Hamilton et al. 2016). However, neither modern temperature nor precipitation shows independent correlations in multiple matrix regressions, possibly indicating spatial autocorrelation with either terrain roughness or cost-path distances, though this would support the assertion that geographic rather than environmental factors play a more important role in explaining assemblage variability and patterns of raw material use. The LGM and LIG datasets, which offer a bracket for climatic extremities throughout a glacial cycle, do not show as close a relationship to behavioural distance as modern conditions. Some aspects of behavioural distance may be explained by different adaptations to different environmental (and ecological) conditions, although better terrestrial archives for environmental conditions may help constrain the use of modelled datasets.

The approach employed here deliberately targets the breadth of data available for the MSA of Eastern Africa, requiring a coarse resolution of analysis that is able to explain 12–13% of variability in patterns of stone tool assemblage composition and raw material use. Autocorrelation may explain why only one spatial variable is demonstrated to provide an independent correlation to patterns of assemblage composition and raw material use. The identification of terrain roughness as the spatial variable with the strongest relationship to both is notable given the heterogeneity of environmental conditions observed across Eastern African MSA sites, as well as occupations across a broad range of altitudes (Blinkhorn and Grove 2018). This is complemented by the significance of site type in explaining variability in stone tool assemblages. These results highlight the importance of the physical landscape in explaining artefact assemblage composition in the MSA of Eastern Africa over isolation by distance and time, though such factors could be mediated by the important impact they have on raw material use and its strong relationship with assemblage composition. Variability in stone toolkits appears significantly influenced by both the location of occupation and the cost of moving across a landscape. Stone Age hunter-gatherer mobility is often characterised as either comprising of logistical forays from a centre of occupation or as a number of seasonal moves of the residential base, though a spectrum of behaviour is likely obscured by such a dichotomy (Binford 1980, 2019). Reconciling our results with such models is not straightforward and is perhaps reliant upon the sparse, higher resolution sequences (e.g. Tryon et al. 2013), but we offer a complimentary approach that highlights the significance of examining mobility as a means to explain past behaviour and patterns of change. Patterns of raw material use also reflect how past mobile populations have engaged with their landscape. The spread of natural resources is uneven at both a local and regional scale whilstpatterns of raw material sourcing, lithic reduction and stone tool use may occur at discretelocations, both of which relate to patterns of isolation by distance in raw material use we identify. Similarly, patterns of isolation by time indicate changing preferences in raw material use through the MSA in Eastern Africa, which may reflect functional concerns regarding flaking properties of alternate materials, clast size availability and patterns of skill. Exotic raw material use, sourced from distances of ca. 300 km, appears a persistent feature of the MSA of Eastern Africa (Brooks et al. 2018; van Baelen et al. 2019), and studies across the continent have illustrated that MSA populations have made costly choices to access raw materials that appear to exclude purely functional concerns (Nash et al. 2016). Examining how terrain roughness impacts the juxtaposition of locally sourced and exotic raw material use offers a profitable avenue for future inquiry, but one that is reliant upon detailed provenance analyses.

Conclusions

Earlier syntheses of the MSA of Eastern Africa struggled to clarify patterns of behavioural variability through space or time, emphasising the mosaic nature of the archaeological record (Clark 1988). The application of quantitative methods has begun to identify elements of structure within this mosaic record, illuminating changing patterns of behaviour (Tryon and Faith 2013; Blinkhorn and Grove 2018). Here, we have employed matrix correlations as a means to examine the extent to which variability between MSA stone tool assemblages and patterns of raw material use can be explained by a range of archaeological, behavioural and environmental factors, including the first application of multiple matrix regression to such datasets. The results of the study emphasise the significant relationship between alternate patterns of raw material use and different constellations of stone tools in an assemblage, with the former showing strong, independent correlations to differences in time and space. The combination of these two factors, as the single Timespace variable, exhibits stronger correlation to behavioural distances that are independent of other spatial variables under consideration, potentially indicative of the role cultural inheritance may play on processes of adaptation and innovation. Although isolation by distance appears to play some role in explaining differences in assemblage composition, a stronger relationship is identified with roughness, suggesting toolkit constellations may better reflect adaptation to mobility and use in landscapes with different topographies. This study also highlights the significant impact that choices made by archaeologists can have upon the archaeological record, especially with the prominence of archaeological method and focus on different site types in the results of multiple matrix regressions. Other geographic and environmental variables also illuminate significant relationships to behavioural variability, supporting the emphasis Basell (2008) placed on examining the ecological contexts of MSA sites in Eastern Africa. Identifying alternate means for examining the ecological contexts of archaeological sites, such as site-based palaeoenvironmental proxies or integration of faunal records, may help further resolve their impact on behavioural variability and disentangle impacts of spatial autocorrelations that result from using modelled climate data.