Skip to main content
Log in

The impact of brain injury on within-individual changes in moral disengagement: implications for criminal and antisocial behavior

Brain injury and moral disengagement

  • Published:
Journal of Experimental Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

The current study aims to further unpack the link between head injury and criminal behavior by examining the association between brain injury and changes in moral disengagement.

Methods

The current study uses the Pathways to Desistance study (N = 1354) to estimate a series of longitudinal cross-lagged dynamic panel models to examine within-individual changes in moral disengagement across the study period.

Results

The results revealed that moral disengagement decreased over time, but sustaining a head injury resulted in a subsequent increase in moral disengagement across the study period.

Conclusions

Head injuries may compromise expected changes in moral disengagement via neuropsychological deficits in brain regions that are implicated in moral decision-making. A continued investigation of this link would inform both criminological theory and intervention programming.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the interest of clarity, we would like to note that we are in no way advocates of phrenology and we do not, as some critics have claimed, see properly conducted contemporary neuroscience as a modern form of phenology. We simply point out this connection to acknowledge that, while phrenology got most everything wrong, it was correct in the assumption that the human brain is implicated in the development of personality, traits, tendencies, and behavior.

  2. While it is possible to construct a person-wave dataset composed of 11 time periods, a person-year dataset was favored for the current study as it standardizes the measurement period between interviews. While it is not possible to calculate 6-month measures for the interviews spanning months 36 through 84 following the baseline interview, it is possible to collapse the 6-month measures collected earlier in the study period. However, in order to examine the sensitivity of the findings from the primary analysis, the cross-lagged dynamic panel models were re-estimated using a person-wave dataset rather than the person-year dataset employed in the primary analysis. The results did not differ from the primary results in any substantive way.

  3. To account for periods of substantial neurobiological development, supplemental analyses were performed with an additional control variable for age coded such that 0 = age 19 or older and 1 = age 14 through 18. The additional control was nonsignificant (b = .002, p = .875), and there were no meaningful changes in the observed association between the lagged brain injury measure and the changes in moral disengagement (b = .045, p = .003) in the cross-lagged dynamic panel models.

  4. A supplemental mixed effects model was estimated using the same measures that were included in the cross-lagged dynamic panel models including a lagged (i.e., t−1) brain injury measure. The only difference between the models was that all time-varying covariates were group mean (i.e., within-individual) centered to better capture within-individual differences (Horney et al. 1995). The results fell directly in line with the primary analysis, wherein the association between the lagged brain injury measure and moral disengagement was positive and significant (b = .048, p = .001).

  5. FIML assumes that missing data patters are at least missing at random (MAR)—that is, the observed patterns of missingness can be explained by other observable values in the dataset, but missing values on any given measure are not correlated with the measure itself. This latter requirement of MAR data prevents the direct assessment of these assumptions, as the missing values would be required for such an analysis (Young and Johnson 2015). Despite this limitation, additional analyses were performed to examine the extent to which missing data patterns covaried with other observed measures. A series of t tests and logistic regression models were estimated. While some of the study measures were associated with patterns of missingness, no systematic patterns were observed, providing preliminary evidence that the examined measures were MAR. However, to examine the robustness of the study findings, the cross-lagged dynamic panel model was re-estimated using listwise deletion (as opposed to FIML). The results were virtually identical to those presented in the primary analysis.

References

  • Acock, A. C. (2005). Working with missing values. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67, 1012–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allison, P. D., Williams, R., & Moral-Benito, E. (2017). Maximum likelihood for cross-lagged panel models with fixed effects. Socius, 3, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alvarez, J. A., & Emory, E. (2006). Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 17–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amemiya, J., Vanderhei, S., & Monahan, K. C. (2017). Parsing apart the persisters: etiological mechanisms and criminal offense patterns of moderate- and high-level persistent offenders. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 819–835.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of Moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 364–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A. (2001). The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychology Review, 11, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23, 361–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, D. J., Preacher, K. J., & Gil, K. M. (2006). Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: new procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 11, 142–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, M. H., Dooley, J. J., & Anderson, V. (2013). A preliminary investigation of moral reasoning and empathy after traumatic brain injury in adolescents. Brain Injury, 27, 896–902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beauchamp, M. H., Vera-Estay, E., Morasse, F., Anderson, V., & Dooley, J. (2019). Moral reasoning and decision-making in adolescents who sustain traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 33, 32–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 295–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belanger, H. G., Curtiss, G., Demery, J. A., Lebowitz, B. K., & Vanderploeg, R. D. (2005). Factors moderating neuropsychological outcomes following mild traumatic brain injury: a meta-analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 215–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, E. D., Abildskov, T. J., Petrie, J. A., Farrer, T. J., Dennis, M., Simic, N., et al. (2013). Heterogeneity of brain lesions in pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 27, 438–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boman, J. H., & Mowen, T. J. (2018). The role of turning points in establishing baseline differences between people in developmental and life-course criminology. Criminology, 56, 191–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. (2015). All Denver jail inmates in high-risk unit have brain trauma. The Denver Post.

  • Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Là, E., & Di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective deficit in personal moral judgment following damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex. SCAN, 2, 84–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. E. (1981). Modeling crime trends: a criminal opportunity perspective. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 18, 138–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. E., & Felson, M. (1979). Social change and crime rate trends: a routine activity approach. American Sociological Review, 44, 588.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, E. J., & McCormick, B. F. (2019). Mild traumatic brain injury and psychopathology in adolescence: evidence from the project on human development in Chicago neighborhoods. Journal of Adolescent Health, 65, 79–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dégeilh, F., Bernier, A., Gravel, J., & Beauchamp, M. H. (2018). Developmental trajectories of adaptive functioning following early mild traumatic brain injury. Developmental Psychobiology, 60, 1037–1047.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delisi, M., Peters, D. J., Vaughn, M. G., Shook, J. J., & Hochstetler, A. (2014). Dynamics of psychopathy and moral disengagement in the etiology of crime psychopathy as unified theory of crime view project contextual influences on child and adolescent health and behavior view project. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 12, 295–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dikmen, S. S., Corrigan, J. D., Levin, H. S., MacHamer, J., Stiers, W., & Weisskopf, M. G. (2009). Cognitive outcome following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 24, 430–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrer, T. J., & Hedges, D. W. (2011). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in incarcerated groups compared to the general population: a meta-analysis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 390–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrer, T. J., Frost, R. B., & Hedges, D. W. (2013). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in juvenile offenders: a meta-analysis. Child Neuropsychology, 19, 225–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farrington, D. P. (1973). Self-reports of deviant behavior: predictive and stable? The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 64, 99–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fazel, S., Lichtenstein, P., Grann, M., & Långström, N. (2011). Risk of violent crime in individuals with epilepsy and traumatic brain injury: a 35-year Swedish population study. PLoS Medicine, 8, e1001150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, D., Dariotis, J. K., Ferguson, P. L., & Pickelsimer, E. E. (2016). Relationships between traumatic brain injury and illicit drug use and their association with aggression in inmates. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 60, 575–597.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., de Fries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. B., Farrer, T. J., Primosch, M., & Hedges, D. W. (2013). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in the general adult population: a meta-analysis. Neuroepidemiology, 40, 154–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gil-Fenoy, M. J., García-García, J., Carmona-Samper, E., & Ortega-Campos, E. (2018). Antisocial behavior and executive functions in young offenders (Conducta antisocial y funciones ejecutivas de jóvenes infractores). Revista de Psicdidactica, 23, 70–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giordano, P. C., Cernkovich, S. A., & Rudolph, J. L. (2002). Gender, crime, and desistance: toward a theory of cognitive transformation. American Journal of Sociology, 107, 990–1064.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2014). Neurocriminology: implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15, 54–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenn, A. L., Raine, A., & Schug, R. A. (2009). The neural correlates of moral decision-making in psychopathy. Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 5–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, C. (1978). Stroop color and word test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafman, J., Schwab, K., Warden, D., Pridgen, A., Brown, H. R., & Salazar, A. M. (1996). Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology, 46, 1231–1231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron, 44, 389–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guberman, G. I., Robitaille, M.-P., Larm, P., Ptito, A., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., & Hodgins, S. (2018). Are traumatic brain injuries associated with criminality after taking account of childhood family social status and disruptive behaviors? The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, appi.neuropsych.

  • Haidt, J. (2008). Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 65–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, S. A., Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Carlo, G. (2008). Parenting dimensions and adolescents’ internalisation of moral values. Journal of Moral Education, 37, 205–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton, T. F. (2011). Neuroscience of self and self-regulation. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 363–390.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1971). Commentary on the indiscriminate state of social class measurement. Social Forces, 49, 563–567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horney, J., Osgood, D. W., & Marshall, I. H. (1995). Criminal careers in the short-term: intra-individual variability in crime and its relation to local life circumstances. American Sociological Review, 60, 655–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutcherson, C. A., Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Woodward, J., & Rangel, A. (2015). Emotional and utilitarian appraisals of moral dilemmas are encoded in separate areas and integrated in ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 12593–12605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Singh, V. (2013). Hippocampal contributions to the processing of social emotions. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 945–955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, T. L., Braun, J. M., Mello, M., Triche, E. W., & Buka, S. L. (2017). The relationship between early childhood head injury and later life criminal behaviour: a longitudinal cohort study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 71, 800–805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiehl, K. A., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., Brink, J., & Liddle, P. F. (2001). Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 677–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs, M. (2012). The role of prefrontal cortex in psychopathy. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23, 253–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446, 908–911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konrad, C., Geburek, A. J., Rist, F., Blumenroth, H., Fischer, B., Husstedt, I., et al. (2011). Long-term cognitive and emotional consequences of mild traumatic brain injury. Psychological Medicine, 41, 1197–1211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancaster, T. (2000). The incidental parameter problem since 1948. Journal of Econometrics, 95, 391–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (2003). Shared beginnings, divergent lives: delinquent boys to age 70. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauterbach, M. D., Notarangelo, P. L., Nichols, S. J., Llane, K. S., & Koliatsos, V. E. (2015). Diagnostic and treatment challenges in traumatic brain injury patients with severe neuropsychiatric symptoms: Insights into psychiatric practice. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 11, 1601–1607.

  • Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 718–729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, B., Kovacevic, N., Nica, E. I., Cheung, G., Gao, F., Schwartz, M. L., & Black, S. E. (2008). The Toronto traumatic brain injury study: injury severity and quantified mrisymbol. Neurology, 70, 771–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucke-Wold, B., Seidel, K., Udo, R., Omalu, B., Ornstein, M., Nolan, R., … Ross, J. S. (2017). Role of tau acetylation in alzheimer’s disease and chronic traumatic encephalopathy: the way forward for successful treatment HHS public access. Journal of Neurology and Neurosurgery, 4. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.2839.A.

  • May, G., Benson, R., Balon, R., & Boutros, N. (2013). Neurofeedback and traumatic brain injury: a literature review. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 25, 289–296.

  • McAllister, T. W. (2011). Neurobiological consequences of traumatic brain injury. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 13, 287–300.

  • McAllister, T. W., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A., Sparling, M. B., Johnson, S. C., Guerin, S. J., et al. (1999). Brain activation during working memory 1 month after mild traumatic brain injury: a functional MRI study. Neurology, 53, 1300–1300.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald, B. C., Flashman, L. A., Arciniegas, D. B., Ferguson, R. J., Xing, L., Harezlak, J., Sprehn, G. C., Hammond, F. M., Maerlender, A. C., Kruck, C. L., Gillock, K. L., Frey, K., Wall, R. N., Saykin, A. J., McAllister, T. W. (2017). Methylphenidate and memory and attention adaptation training for persistent cognitive symptoms after traumatic brain injury: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42, 1766–1775.

  • Mendez, M. F. (2009). The neurobiology of moral behavior: review and neuropsychiatric implications. CNS Spectrums, 14, 608–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourã O-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., & Pessoa, L. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 2730–2736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Zahn, R., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2005). The neural basis of human moral cognition. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 6, 799–809.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2008). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16, 696–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2013). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. In Social neuroscience: key readings (Vol. 16, pp. 199–210).

  • Moral-Benito, E., Allison, P. D., & Williams, R. (2018). Dynamic panel data modeling using maximum likelihood: an alternative to Arellano-bond.

  • Morgan, A. B., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2000). A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 113–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mott, M. C., Gordon, J. A., & Koroshetz, W. J. (2018). The NIH BRAIN Initiative: advancing neurotechnologies, integrating disciplines. PLoS Biology, 16, e3000066.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulvey, E. P. (2012). Research on pathways to desistance [Maricopa County, AZ and Philadelphia County, PA]: subject measures, 2000–2010 ICPSR29961-v2 2012-08-20 2013 Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR).

  • Mulvey, E. P. (2016). Research on pathways to desistance [Maricopa County, AZ and Philadelphia County, PA]: subject measures, 2000-2010. Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor].

  • Mulvey, E. P., Steinberg, L., Fagan, J., Cauffman, E., Piquero, A. R., Chassin, L., et al. (2004). Theory and research on desistance from antisocial activity among serious adolescent offenders. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 2, 213–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institutes of Health. (2014). Brain 2025: a scientific vision.

  • Osgood, D. W. (2009). Handbook of quantitative criminology. In A. R. Piquero & D. Weisburd (Eds.), Handbook of quantitative criminology. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascual, L., Rodrigues, P., & Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2013). How does morality work in the brain? A functional and structural perspective of moral behavior. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perron, B. E., & Howard, M. O. (2008). Prevalence and correlates of traumatic brain injury among delinquent youths. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 18, 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polito, M. Z., Thompson, J. W. G., & DeFina, P. A. (2010). A review of the international brain research foundation novel approach to mild traumatic brain injury presented at the international conference on behavioral health and traumatic brain injury. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners, 22, 504–509.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15, 209–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyrooz, D. C., Gartner, N., & Smith, M. (2017). Consequences of incarceration for gang membership: a longitudinal study of serious offenders in Philadelphia and phoenix. Criminology, 55, 273–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Skrondal, A. (2012). Multilevel and longitudinal modeling using Stata, volumes I and II (3rd ed.). College Station, TX: Stata Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A. (1997). Antisocial behavior and psychophysiology: a biosocial perspective and a prefrontal dysfunction hypothesis. In D. M. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 289–304). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A. (2019). The neuromoral theory of antisocial, violent, and psychopathic behavior. Psychiatry Research, 277, 64–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 203–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raskin, S. A., & Rearick, E. (1996). Verbal fluency in individuals with mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 10, 416–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, B., & Richardson, N. J. (2017). Traumatic brain injury and recidivism among returning inmates. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 44, 472–486.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodrigo, M. J., Padrón, I., de Vega, M., & Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Adolescents’ risky decision-making activates neural networks related to social cognition and cognitive control processes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1995). Crime in the making: pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapolsky, R. M. (2018). Behave: the biology of humans at our best and worst. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sariaslan, A., Lichtenstein, P., Larsson, H., & Fazel, S. (2016a). Triggers for violent criminality in patients with psychotic disorders. JAMA Psychiatry, 73, 796–803.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sariaslan, A., Sharp, D. J., D’Onofrio, B. M., Larsson, H., & Fazel, S. (2016b). Long-term outcomes associated with traumatic brain injury in childhood and adolescence: a nationwide Swedish cohort study of a wide range of medical and social outcomes. PLoS Medicine, 13, 15–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarpina, F., & Tagini, S. (2017). The stroop color and word test. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 557.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, C. A., Mulvey, E. P., Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., Losoya, S. H., Hecker, T., et al. (2004). Operational lessons from the pathways to desistance project. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 2, 237–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. A. (2019). A longitudinal assessment of head injuries as a source of acquired neuropsychological deficits and the implications for criminal persistence. Justice Quarterly, 1–28.

  • Schwartz, J. A., Savolainen, J., Aaltonen, M., Merikukka, M., Paananen, R., & Gissler, M. (2015). Intelligence and criminal behavior in a total birth cohort: an examination of functional form, dimensions of intelligence, and the nature of offending. Intelligence, 51, 109–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., & Brauer, J. R. (2017). Head injuries and changes in delinquency from adolescence to emerging adulthood: the importance of self-control as a mediating influence. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 54, 869–901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., & Valgardson, B. A. (2018). An evaluation of the directional relationship between head injuries and subsequent changes in impulse control and delinquency in a sample of previously adjudicated males. Journal of Criminal Justice, 56, 70–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, J. A., Jodis, C. A., Breen, K. M., & Parker, B. N. (2019). Brain injury and adverse outcomes: a contemporary review of the evidence. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27, 67–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, C., McKinlay, A., McLellan, T., Britt, E., Grace, R., & MacFarlane, M. (2015). A comparison of adult outcomes for males compared to females following pediatric traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 29, 501–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiroma, E. J., Ferguson, P. L., & Pickelsimer, E. E. (2010). Prevalence of traumatic brain injury in an offender population: a meta-analysis. Journal of Correctional Health Care, 16, 147–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • StataCorp. (2017). Stata statistical software: release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, D., Demos, K., & Heatherton, T. (2010). Staying in control: the neural basis of self-regulation and its failure. In J. Cacioppo & J. Decety (Eds.), Handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 360–377). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walker, L. J., Hennig, K. H., & Krettenauer, T. (2000). Parent and peer contexts for children’s moral reasoning development. Child Development, 71, 1033–1048.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. New York: The Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Brace & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, D. A., & Schwartz, G. E. (1990). Distress and restraint as superordinate dimensions of self-reported adjustment: a typological perspective. Journal of Personality, 58, 381–417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, R., Allison, P. D., & Moral-Benito Banco de España Madrid, E. (2018a). Linear dynamic panel-data estimation using maximum likelihood and structural equation modeling. The Stata Journal, 18, 293–326.

  • Williams, W. H., Chitsabesan, P., Fazel, S., McMillan, T., Hughes, N., Parsonage, M., & Tonks, J. (2018b). Traumatic brain injury: a potential cause of violent crime? 5, 836–844.

  • Wooldridge, J. M. (2005). Simple solutions to the initial conditions problem in dynamic, nonlinear panel data models with unobserved heterogeneity. Journal of Applied Econometrics, 20, 39–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wooldridge, J. M. (2010). Econometric analysis of cross sectional and panel data (2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoder, K. J., Harenski, C., Kiehl, K. A., & Decety, J. (2015). Neural networks underlying implicit and explicit moral evaluations in psychopathy. Translational Psychiatry, 5, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, R., & Johnson, D. R. (2015). Handling missing values in longitudinal panel data with multiple imputation. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 77, 277–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, L., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Hauser, M., & Damasio, A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron, 65, 845–851.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The project described was supported by funds from the following: Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention (2007-MU-FX-0002), National Institute of Justice (2008-IJ-CX-0023), John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, William T. Grant Foundation, Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, William Penn Foundation, Center for Disease Control, National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA019697), Pennsylvania Commission on Crime and Delinquency, and the Arizona Governor's Justice Commission. We are grateful for their support. The content of this paper, however, is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of these agencies.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joseph A. Schwartz.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schwartz, J.A., Fitter, B. & Jodis, C.A. The impact of brain injury on within-individual changes in moral disengagement: implications for criminal and antisocial behavior. J Exp Criminol 16, 407–429 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-020-09439-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-020-09439-6

Keywords

Navigation