Skip to main content
Log in

The Relationships Between Initial Consonants in Japanese Sound Symbolic Words and Familiarity, Multi-Sensory Imageability, Emotional Valence, and Arousal

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sound symbolic words consist of inevitable associations between sounds and meanings. We aimed to identify differences in familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, and arousal between Japanese sound symbolic words with voiced initial consonants (VCs; dakuon in Japanese; e.g., biribiri) and Japanese sound symbolic words with semi-voiced initial consonants (SVCs; handakuon in Japanese; e.g., piripiri), and between VCs (e.g., daradara) and Japanese sound symbolic words with voiceless initial consonants (VLCs; seion in Japanese; e.g., taratara). First, auditory imageability and arousal were significantly higher in VCs than SVCs, whereas familiarity, tactile imageability, and positive emotion (emotional valence) were significantly higher in SVCs than VCs. Second, visual imageability was higher in VCs than VLCs, while familiarity and positive emotion were higher in VLCs than VCs. Initial consonants in Japanese sound symbolic words could be associated with specific subjective evaluations such as familiarity, visual imageability, auditory imageability, tactile imageability, emotional valence, and arousal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adelman, J. S., Estes, Z., & Cossu, M. (2018). Emotional sound symbolism: Languages rapidly signal valence via phonemes. Cognition, 175, 122–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aryani, A., & Jacobs, A. M. (2018). Affective congruence between sound and meaning of words facilitates semantic decision. Behavioral Sciences, 8(6), 56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aryani, A., Conrad, M., Schmidtke, D., & Jacobs, A. (2018a). Why “piss” is ruder than “pee”? The role of sound in affective meaning making. PLoS ONE, 13(6), e0198430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aryani, A., Hsu, C. T., & Jacobs, A. M. (2018b). The sound of words evokes affective brain responses. Brain sciences, 8(6), 94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aryani, A., Hsu, C. T., & Jacobs, A. M. (2019). Affective iconic words benefit from additional sound-meaning integration in the left amygdala. Human brain mapping, 40(18), 5289–5300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aryani, A., Isbilen, E. S., & Christiansen, M. H. (2020). Affective arousal links sound to meaning. Psychological Science, 31(8), 978–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-efects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D. M. (2005). Fitting linear mixed models in R. R News, 5, 27–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, A. J., Caparos, S., Davidoff, J., de Fockert, J., Linnell, K. J., & Spence, C. (2013). “Bouba” and “Kiki” in Namibia? A remote culture make similar shape-sound matches, but different shape-taste matches to Westerners. Cognition, 126(2), 165–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjafield, J., & Muckenheim, R. (1989). Dates of entry and measures of imagery, concreteness, goodness, and familiarity for 1,046 words sampled from the Oxford English Dictionary. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 21(1), 31–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC Psycholinguistic Database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Folkins, C., & Lenrow, P. B. (1966). An investigation of the expressive values of graphemes. The Psychological Record, 16(2), 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetry, V., Urbain, S., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2005). Paths to phonemic awareness in Japanese: Evidence from a training study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 285–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamano, S. (1998). The sound-symbolic system of Japanese. Studies in Japanese Linguistics, 10, 1–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., Kita, S., Nagumo, M., & Okada, H. (2008). Sound symbolism facilitates early verb learning. Cognition, 109, 54–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S. (1997). Two-dimensional semantic analysis of Japanese mimetics. Linguistics, 35, 379–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klink, R. R. (2000). Creating brand names with meaning: The use of sound symbolism. Marketing Letters, 11(1), 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1929). Gestalt psychology. New York, USA: Liveright.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, J. H. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119–137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monaghan, P., Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2007). The phonological-distributional coherence hypothesis: Cross-linguistic evidence in language acquisition. Cognitive Psychology, 55(4), 259–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers-Schulz, B., Pujara, M., Wolf, R. C., & Koenigs, M. (2013). Inherent emotional quality of human speech sounds. Cognition & Emotion, 27(6), 1105–1113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ono, M. (Ed.). (2007). Nihongo onomatope jiten [Japanese onomatopoeia dictionary]. Tokyo, Japan: Shogakukan.

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. http://www.r-project.org/.

  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought, and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(12), 3–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1929). A study in phonetic symbolism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westbury, C. (2005). Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task. Brain and Language, 93, 10–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. D. (1988). The MRC psycholinguistic database: Machine readable dictionary, version 2. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 20, 6–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, Z., Wu, J., Zhang, Y., & Wang, Z. (2017). Norms of valence, arousal, concreteness, familiarity, imageability, and context availability for 1,100 Chinese words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(4), 1374–1385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yee, L. T. S. (2017). Valence, arousal, familiarity, concreteness, and imageability ratings for 292 two-character Chinese nouns in Cantonese speakers in Hong Kong. PLoS ONE, 12(3), e0174569.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors were supported by Hiroshima University Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (to TK), KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Research Activity Start-up (to TK), and KAKENHI Grant-in-Aid for Early-Career Scientists (to TK and TU). This research was conducted as part of the School of Education Joint Research Project 2020 at Hiroshima University, and received research support from the School of Education. We would like to thank Editage (www.editage.com) for English language editing.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Toshimune Kambara.

Ethics declarations

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kambara, T., Umemura, T. The Relationships Between Initial Consonants in Japanese Sound Symbolic Words and Familiarity, Multi-Sensory Imageability, Emotional Valence, and Arousal. J Psycholinguist Res 50, 831–842 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09749-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-020-09749-w

Keywords

Navigation