Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000211

Abstract. After experiencing an emotional event, people either seek out others’ presence (social affiliation) or avoid others’ presence (social isolation). The determinants and effects of social affiliation are now well-known, but social psychologists have not yet thoroughly studied social isolation. This study aims to ascertain which motives and corresponding regulation strategies participants report for social isolation following negative emotional events. A group of 96 participants retrieved from memory an actual negative event that led them to temporarily socially isolate themselves and freely listed up to 10 motives for social isolation. Through semantic categorization of the 423 motives reported by the participants, we found that “cognitive clarification” and “keeping one’s distance” – that is, the need for cognitive regulation and the refusal of socioaffective regulation, respectively – were the most commonly and quickly reported motives for social isolation. We discuss the findings in terms of ideas for future studies aimed at clarifying the role of social isolation in health situations.

References

  • Borys, S., & Perlman, D. (1985). Gender differences in loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 63–74. doi 10.1177/ 0146167285111006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emotion, 13, 926–939. doi 10.1037/a0032400 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruhn, J. (2009). The group effect: Social cohesion and health outcomes. New York: Springer-Verlag. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davison, K. P., Pennebaker, J. W., & Dickerson, S. S. (2000). Who talks? The social psychology of illness support groups. American Psychologist, 55, 205–217. doi 10.1037/0003-066X.55.2.205 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Delelis, G. (2002). Emotions et interactions sociales: l’affiliation et l’isolement comme modes de régulation personnelle et sociale [Emotions and social interactions: Social affiliation and isolation as ways of individual and social regulation]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Lille, France. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Delelis, G., Desombre, C., & Mignon, A. (2014). Régulation émotionnelle et valeur sociale: réévaluer ses émotions pour se sentir à l’aise et pour bien agir [Emotion regulation and social value: Reevaluating one’s emotions to feel at ease and to act right]. In V. ChristopheC. DucroP. AntoineEds., Psychologie de la Santé: Individu, famille et société (pp. 193–196). Lille, France: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Delfosse, C., Nils, F., Lasserre, S., & Rimé, B. (2004). Les motifs allégués du partage social et de la rumination mentale des émotions: comparaison des épisodes positifs et négatifs [Alleged motives for social sharing and mental rumination of emotions: A comparison of positive and negative episodes]. Cahiers Internationaux de Psychologie Sociale, 64, 35–44. doi 10.3917/cips.064.0035 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duprez, C., Christophe, V., Rimé, B., Congard, A., & Antoine, P. (2015). Motives for the social sharing of an emotional experience. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32, 757–787. doi 10.1177/0265407514548393 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. doi 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.224 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Helgeson, V. S., Cohen, S., & Fritz, H. L. (1998). Social ties and cancer. In J. C. HollandW. BreitbartEds., Psycho-oncology (pp. 99–109). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lau, S., & Gruen, G. E. (1992). The social stigma of loneliness: Effect of target person’s and perceiver’s sex. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 182–189. doi 10.1177/0146167292182009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leroy, T., Delelis, G., Nandrino, J.-L., & Christophe, V. (2014). Régulations endogène et exogène des émotions: des processus indissociables [Endogenous and exogenous emotion regulation: Complementary and indivisible processes]. Psychologie Française, 59, 183–197. doi 10.1016/j.psfr.2013.10.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marangoni, C., & Ickes, W. (1989). Loneliness: A theoretical review with implications for measurement. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 93–128. doi 10.1177/026540758900600107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2012). An attachment perspective on psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 11, 11–15. doi 10.1016/ j.wpsyc.2012.01.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2015). Responsiveness. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 67–71. doi 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rimé, B. (2007). Interpersonal emotion regulation. In J. J. GrossEd., Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 466–485). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shankar, A., McMunn, A., Banks, J., & Steptoe, A. (2011). Loneliness, social isolation and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychology, 30, 377–385. doi 10.1037/a0022826 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Emotion regulation choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1391–1396. doi 10.1177/ 0956797611418350 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stokes, J. P., & Levin, I. (1986). Gender differences in predicting loneliness from social network characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1069–1074. doi 10.1037/0022-3514. 51.5.1069 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamres, L. K., Janicki, D., & Helgeson, V. S. (2002). Sex differences in coping behavior: A meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 2–30. doi 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vandervoort, D. (2000). Social isolation and gender. Current Psychology, 19, 229–236. doi 10.1007/s12144-000-1017-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vanhalst, J., Goossens, L., Luyckx, K., Scholte, R. H. J., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (2013). The development of loneliness from mid- to late adolescence: Trajectory classes, personality traits, and psychosocial functioning. Journal of Adolescence, 36, 1305–1312. doi 10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.04.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Waltz, M., Badura, B., Pfaff, H., & Schott, T. (1988). Marriage and the psychological consequences of a heart attack: A longitudinal study of adaptation to chronic illness after 3 years. Social Science and Medicine, 27, 149–158. doi 10.1007/978-3-642-84312-9_3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wills, T. A. (1997). Modes and families of coping: An analysis of downward comparison in the structure of other cognitive and behavioral mechanisms. In B. P. BuunkF. X. GibbonsEds., Health, coping, and well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory (pp. 167–193). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar