Skip to main content
Original Article

The Perception of Available Resources Influences the After-Effect of Cognitive Control on Cognitive Performance and Pain

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000386

Abstract. In two experiments, we investigated the impact of perceived available cognitive resources using a sequential-task paradigm. First, participants worked on an easy or difficult cognitive task. Then, they received a cue suggesting that their cognitive resources were still optimal or they did not receive any information on their resources. Subsequently, they worked on a second difficult cognitive task (Experiment 1) or received painful electrical stimulations (Experiment 2). We predicted that the cue on optimal resources would neutralize the effect of the first difficult task on subsequent cognitive performance and pain. Overall, results supported our predictions. We interpret these findings as showing an important role of perceived available resources in the after-effect induced by the sequential-task paradigm.

References

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bandura, A., O’Leary, A., Taylor, C. B., Gauthier, J., & Gossard, D. (1987). Perceived self-efficacy and pain control: Opioid and nonopioid mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 563–571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.3.563 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 351–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boksem, M. A., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue: Costs and benefits. Brain Research Reviews, 59, 125–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Breivik, H., Collett, B., Ventafridda, V., Cohen, R., & Gallacher, D. (2006). Survey of chronic pain in Europe: Prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. European Journal of Pain, 10, 287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.06.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brickenkamp, R., & Zillmer, E. (1998). d2 test of attention. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buhle, J., & Wager, T. D. (2010). Performance-dependent inhibition of pain by an executive working memory task. Pain, 149, 19–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.027 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bush, G., Whalen, P. J., Rosen, B. R., Jenike, M. A., McInerney, S. C., & Rauch, S. L. (1998). The counting Stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging – validation study with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 270–282. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1998)6:4<270::AID-HBM6>3.0.CO;2-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 796–815. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000083 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chow, J. T., Hui, C. M., & Lau, S. (2015). A depleted mind feels inefficacious: Ego-depletion reduces self-efficacy to exert further self-control. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 754–768. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2120 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clarkson, J. J., Hirt, E. R., Jia, L., & Alexander, M. B. (2010). When perception is more than reality: The effects of perceived versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 29–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017539 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dolce, J. J., Doleys, D. M., Raczynski, J. M., Lossie, J., Poole, L., & Smith, M. (1986). The role of self-efficacy expectancies in the prediction of pain tolerance. Pain, 27, 261–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(86)90216-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Self-regulation and sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self-regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 173–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206293472 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., … Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.325 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giacomantonio, M., Jordan, J., Fennis, B. M., & Panno, A. (2014). When the motivational consequences of ego depletion collide: Conservation dominates over reward-seeking. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 217–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.07.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 546–573. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019486 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York, NY: Appleton-Century. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion – Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610384745 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Job, V., Walton, G. M., Bernecker, K., & Dweck, C. S. (2015). Implicit theories about willpower predict self-regulation and grades in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 637–647. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keefe, F. J., Rumble, M. E., Scipio, C. D., Giordano, L. A., & Perri, L. M. (2004). Psychological aspects of persistent pain: current state of the science. The Journal of Pain, 5, 195–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2004.02.576 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure tradeoff in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 131–141. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031048 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kool, W., McGuire, J. T., Rosen, Z. B., & Botvinick, M. M. (2010). Decision making and the avoidance of cognitive demand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 665–682. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020198 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kurzban, R. (2010). Does the brain consume additional glucose during self-control tasks? Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 244–259. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491000800208 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 661–679. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003196 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A. S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 199–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martijn, C., Tenbült, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E., & de Vries, N. K. (2002). Getting a grip on ourselves: Challenging expectancies about loss of energy after self-control. Social Cognition, 20, 441–460. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.6.441.22978 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., & Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02343.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Merskey, H. (1986). Classification of chronic pain: Descriptions of chronic pain syndromes and definitions of pain terms. Pain, Suppl, 3, 226. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 524–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 894–906. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029007008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Savani, K., & Job, V. (2017). Reverse ego-depletion: Acts of self-control can improve subsequent performance in Indian cultural contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 589–607. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000099 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 770–782. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014635 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., & Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 154–167. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2994 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silvestrini, N., & Rainville, P. (2013). After-effects of cognitive control on pain. European Journal of Pain, 17, 1225–1233. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00299.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Skljarevski, V., & Ramadan, N. M. (2002). The nociceptive flexion reflex in humans – review article. Pain, 96, 3–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00018-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Solberg Nes, L., Roach, A., & Segerstrom, S. (2009). Executive functions, self-regulation, and chronic pain: A review. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 173–183. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9096-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153, 652–654. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.153.3736.652 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 379–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Villemure, C., & Bushnell, M. C. (2002). Cognitive modulation of pain: How do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain, 95, 195–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00007-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2008). Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.883 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wallace, H. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). The effects of success versus failure feedback on further self-control. Self and Identity, 1, 35. https://doi.org/10.1080/152988602317232786 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiech, K., Ploner, M., & Tracey, I. (2008). Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 306–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willer, J. C. (1977). Comparative study of perceived pain and nociceptive flexion reflex in man. Pain, 3, 69–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(77)90036-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilkinson, L., & The Task Force on Statistical Inference. (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594–604. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wright, R. A., & Mlynski, C. (2018). Fatigue determination of inhibitory strength and control: A babe in a bath. Motivation Science, 5, 66–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/mot0000114 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar